Skip to main content
  • Submit
  • About
    • Editorial Board
    • PNAS Staff
    • FAQ
    • Rights and Permissions
    • Site Map
  • Contact
  • Journal Club
  • Subscribe
    • Subscription Rates
    • Subscriptions FAQ
    • Open Access
    • Recommend PNAS to Your Librarian
  • Log in
  • My Cart

Main menu

  • Home
  • Articles
    • Current
    • Latest Articles
    • Special Features
    • Colloquia
    • Collected Articles
    • PNAS Classics
    • Archive
  • Front Matter
  • News
    • For the Press
    • Highlights from Latest Articles
    • PNAS in the News
  • Podcasts
  • Authors
    • Information for Authors
    • Purpose and Scope
    • Editorial and Journal Policies
    • Submission Procedures
    • For Reviewers
    • Author FAQ
  • Submit
  • About
    • Editorial Board
    • PNAS Staff
    • FAQ
    • Rights and Permissions
    • Site Map
  • Contact
  • Journal Club
  • Subscribe
    • Subscription Rates
    • Subscriptions FAQ
    • Open Access
    • Recommend PNAS to Your Librarian

User menu

  • Log in
  • My Cart

Search

  • Advanced search
Home
Home

Advanced Search

  • Home
  • Articles
    • Current
    • Latest Articles
    • Special Features
    • Colloquia
    • Collected Articles
    • PNAS Classics
    • Archive
  • Front Matter
  • News
    • For the Press
    • Highlights from Latest Articles
    • PNAS in the News
  • Podcasts
  • Authors
    • Information for Authors
    • Purpose and Scope
    • Editorial and Journal Policies
    • Submission Procedures
    • For Reviewers
    • Author FAQ

New Research In

Physical Sciences

Featured Portals

  • Physics
  • Chemistry
  • Sustainability Science

Articles by Topic

  • Applied Mathematics
  • Applied Physical Sciences
  • Astronomy
  • Computer Sciences
  • Earth, Atmospheric, and Planetary Sciences
  • Engineering
  • Environmental Sciences
  • Mathematics
  • Statistics

Social Sciences

Featured Portals

  • Anthropology
  • Sustainability Science

Articles by Topic

  • Economic Sciences
  • Environmental Sciences
  • Political Sciences
  • Psychological and Cognitive Sciences
  • Social Sciences

Biological Sciences

Featured Portals

  • Sustainability Science

Articles by Topic

  • Agricultural Sciences
  • Anthropology
  • Applied Biological Sciences
  • Biochemistry
  • Biophysics and Computational Biology
  • Cell Biology
  • Developmental Biology
  • Ecology
  • Environmental Sciences
  • Evolution
  • Genetics
  • Immunology and Inflammation
  • Medical Sciences
  • Microbiology
  • Neuroscience
  • Pharmacology
  • Physiology
  • Plant Biology
  • Population Biology
  • Psychological and Cognitive Sciences
  • Sustainability Science
  • Systems Biology

Opposing effects of attention and consciousness on afterimages

Jeroen J. A. van Boxtel, Naotsugu Tsuchiya, and Christof Koch
PNAS May 11, 2010 107 (19) 8883-8888; https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.0913292107
Jeroen J. A. van Boxtel
aDivision of Biology and
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
  • For correspondence: j.j.a.vanboxtel@gmail.com
Naotsugu Tsuchiya
bDivision of Humanities and Social Sciences, California Institute of Technology, Pasadena, CA 91125;cBrain Science Institute, Tamagawa University, Tamagawagakuen 6-1-1, Machida, Tokyo, 194-610, Japan; and
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Christof Koch
aDivision of Biology anddBrain and Cognitive Engineering, Korea University, Seoul 136-713, Korea
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
  1. Edited* by Anne Treisman, Princeton University, Princeton, NJ, and approved March 31, 2010 (received for review November 17, 2009)

  • Article
  • Figures & SI
  • Info & Metrics
  • PDF
Loading

Article Figures & SI

Figures

  • Fig. 1.
    • Download figure
    • Open in new tab
    • Download powerpoint
    Fig. 1.

    Experiment 1. (A) Our study used a 2 × 2 full-factorial design, independently manipulating the levels of attention and visibility. (B) Each trial started with 4-s peripheral adaptation of a Gabor patch. Attention was modulated by having observers report on the perceptual (dis)appearance of the inducer (high-attention condition) or count the number of Xs that appeared in a central RSVP task (low-attention condition). Perceptual visibility was modulated by showing, or not showing, a rapidly flickering and rotating high-contrast stimulus in the eye contralateral to the afterimage inducer, while the adaptor was physically present throughout the trial (23). After the induction phase, observers indicated how long their afterimage lasted, by pressing and releasing a button. Finally, they entered how many Xs were counted (or they ignored the question in the high-attention conditions). (C) Afterimage durations depend on visibility and attention levels (n = 13 subjects; each subject has a different color). When the inducer stimulus is highly attended, afterimages are shorter than when the stimulus is not/slightly attended (in both visible and invisible conditions). Visible trials have longer afterimage durations than invisible trials, for both high-attention and low-attention conditions. (D) A table with mean ± SEM afterimage durations, and P values of statistical comparisons based on paired one-tailed t tests.

  • Fig. 2.
    • Download figure
    • Open in new tab
    • Download powerpoint
    Fig. 2.

    Experiment 2: Changing perceptual load. The amount of attention paid to the afterimage-inducing stimulus was manipulated by using a difficult or easy RSVP task at fixation, leading to low- and high-attention conditions respectively. (A) Increased attention to the inducer stimulus led to decreased afterimage durations, whereas increased visibility led to increased afterimage durations (both P < 0.03, two-way repeated-measures ANOVA). (B) A table with mean ± SEM afterimage durations, and P values of statistical comparisons based on paired one-tailed t tests.

  • Fig. 3.
    • Download figure
    • Open in new tab
    • Download powerpoint
    Fig. 3.

    Effects of CFS contrast. (A) The CFS mask started to have a suppressing effect on afterimage durations at contrasts higher than ∼6%. This is also when the mask started to have effects on the visibility of the afterimage inducer, which was kept at a fixed contrast of 34%. (B) The difference in afterimage duration between high- and low-attention conditions. The effects of attention were present without suppressor CFS (i.e., zero contrast), and for CFS contrasts larger or equal to 12%. Stars represent significance at the 0.05 level for one-tailed t tests, e.g., P < 0.025 for 0% and 100% contrast. Bars are SEM.

  • Fig. 4.
    • Download figure
    • Open in new tab
    • Download powerpoint
    Fig. 4.

    Dependence on visibility with constant stimuli. (A) In experiment 4, with low CFS contrast (∼10%), about half of the trials were reported to be completely invisible. When we divided the data into completely invisible, and partly/completely visible (n = 10 subjects), visible trials produced longer afterimages than invisible trials (1.79 ± 0.23 s vs. 1.52 ± 0.27 s, P = 0.02 one-tailed t test). (B) When divided into 10 bins (per subject and then averaged over subjects) according to the actual reported visibility duration, there was a clear correlation (R2 = 0.96, P < 0.001, slope = 0.13). Error bars SEM.

  • Fig. 5.
    • Download figure
    • Open in new tab
    • Download powerpoint
    Fig. 5.

    Troxler fading. In experiment 5, with a low adaptation contrast, a stimulus would periodically fade (Troxler fading), even without any CFS stimulus present. A clear, positive correlation between visibility duration and afterimage duration was again observed (R2 = 0.8, P < 0.005, slope = 0.25). Error bars SEM.

Data supplements

  • Supporting Information

    Files in this Data Supplement:

    • Download Supporting Information (PDF)
PreviousNext
Back to top
Article Alerts
Email Article

Thank you for your interest in spreading the word on PNAS.

NOTE: We only request your email address so that the person you are recommending the page to knows that you wanted them to see it, and that it is not junk mail. We do not capture any email address.

Enter multiple addresses on separate lines or separate them with commas.
Opposing effects of attention and consciousness on afterimages
(Your Name) has sent you a message from PNAS
(Your Name) thought you would like to see the PNAS web site.
Citation Tools
Opposing effects of attention and consciousness on afterimages
Jeroen J. A. van Boxtel, Naotsugu Tsuchiya, Christof Koch
Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences May 2010, 107 (19) 8883-8888; DOI: 10.1073/pnas.0913292107

Citation Manager Formats

  • BibTeX
  • Bookends
  • EasyBib
  • EndNote (tagged)
  • EndNote 8 (xml)
  • Medlars
  • Mendeley
  • Papers
  • RefWorks Tagged
  • Ref Manager
  • RIS
  • Zotero
Request Permissions
Share
Opposing effects of attention and consciousness on afterimages
Jeroen J. A. van Boxtel, Naotsugu Tsuchiya, Christof Koch
Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences May 2010, 107 (19) 8883-8888; DOI: 10.1073/pnas.0913292107
del.icio.us logo Digg logo Reddit logo Twitter logo CiteULike logo Facebook logo Google logo Mendeley logo
  • Tweet Widget
  • Facebook Like
  • Mendeley logo Mendeley
Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences: 116 (8)
Current Issue

Submit

Sign up for Article Alerts

Jump to section

  • Article
    • Abstract
    • Results
    • Discussion
    • Materials and Methods
    • Acknowledgments
    • Footnotes
    • References
  • Figures & SI
  • Info & Metrics
  • PDF

You May Also be Interested in

Several aspects of the proposal, which aims to expand open access, require serious discussion and, in some cases, a rethink.
Opinion: “Plan S” falls short for society publishers—and for the researchers they serve
Several aspects of the proposal, which aims to expand open access, require serious discussion and, in some cases, a rethink.
Image credit: Dave Cutler (artist).
Several large or long-lived animals seem strangely resistant to developing cancer. Elucidating the reasons why could lead to promising cancer-fighting strategies in humans.
Core Concept: Solving Peto’s Paradox to better understand cancer
Several large or long-lived animals seem strangely resistant to developing cancer. Elucidating the reasons why could lead to promising cancer-fighting strategies in humans.
Image credit: Shutterstock.com/ronnybas frimages.
Featured Profile
PNAS Profile of NAS member and biochemist Hao Wu
 Nonmonogamous strawberry poison frog (Oophaga pumilio).  Image courtesy of Yusan Yang (University of Pittsburgh, Pittsburgh).
Putative signature of monogamy
A study suggests a putative gene-expression hallmark common to monogamous male vertebrates of some species, namely cichlid fishes, dendrobatid frogs, passeroid songbirds, common voles, and deer mice, and identifies 24 candidate genes potentially associated with monogamy.
Image courtesy of Yusan Yang (University of Pittsburgh, Pittsburgh).
Active lifestyles. Image courtesy of Pixabay/MabelAmber.
Meaningful life tied to healthy aging
Physical and social well-being in old age are linked to self-assessments of life worth, and a spectrum of behavioral, economic, health, and social variables may influence whether aging individuals believe they are leading meaningful lives.
Image courtesy of Pixabay/MabelAmber.

More Articles of This Classification

Biological Sciences

  • DNA helicase RecQ1 regulates mutually exclusive expression of virulence genes in Plasmodium falciparum via heterochromatin alteration
  • Calcineurin dephosphorylates Kelch-like 3, reversing phosphorylation by angiotensin II and regulating renal electrolyte handling
  • Impacts of the Northwest Forest Plan on forest composition and bird populations
Show more

Psychological and Cognitive Sciences

  • Hippocampal atrophy and intrinsic brain network dysfunction relate to alterations in mind wandering in neurodegeneration
  • Musical reward prediction errors engage the nucleus accumbens and motivate learning
  • Self-regulation and the foraging gene (PRKG1) in humans
Show more

Related Content

  • No related articles found.
  • Scopus
  • PubMed
  • Google Scholar

Cited by...

  • Alpha-Band Brain Oscillations Shape the Processing of Perceptible as well as Imperceptible Somatosensory Stimuli during Selective Attention
  • Different Signal Enhancement Pathways of Attention and Consciousness Underlie Perception in Humans
  • Self-Grounded Vision: Hand Ownership Modulates Visual Location through Cortical {beta} and {gamma} Oscillations
  • The effect of eye movements and blinks on afterimage appearance and duration
  • Afterimage size is modulated by size-contrast illusions
  • Individual differences in subjective experience and objective performance in metacontrast masking
  • Eye gaze adaptation under interocular suppression
  • Unseen complex motion is modulated by attention and generates a visible aftereffect
  • High-level face shape adaptation depends on visual awareness: Evidence from continuous flash suppression
  • Scopus (100)
  • Google Scholar

Similar Articles

Site Logo
Powered by HighWire
  • Submit Manuscript
  • Twitter
  • Facebook
  • RSS Feeds
  • Email Alerts

Articles

  • Current Issue
  • Latest Articles
  • Archive

PNAS Portals

  • Classics
  • Front Matter
  • Teaching Resources
  • Anthropology
  • Chemistry
  • Physics
  • Sustainability Science

Information

  • Authors
  • Editorial Board
  • Reviewers
  • Press
  • Site Map

Feedback    Privacy/Legal

Copyright © 2019 National Academy of Sciences. Online ISSN 1091-6490