Skip to main content

Main menu

  • Home
  • Articles
    • Current
    • Special Feature Articles - Most Recent
    • Special Features
    • Colloquia
    • Collected Articles
    • PNAS Classics
    • List of Issues
  • Front Matter
    • Front Matter Portal
    • Journal Club
  • News
    • For the Press
    • This Week In PNAS
    • PNAS in the News
  • Podcasts
  • Authors
    • Information for Authors
    • Editorial and Journal Policies
    • Submission Procedures
    • Fees and Licenses
  • Submit
  • Submit
  • About
    • Editorial Board
    • PNAS Staff
    • FAQ
    • Accessibility Statement
    • Rights and Permissions
    • Site Map
  • Contact
  • Journal Club
  • Subscribe
    • Subscription Rates
    • Subscriptions FAQ
    • Open Access
    • Recommend PNAS to Your Librarian

User menu

  • Log in
  • My Cart

Search

  • Advanced search
Home
Home
  • Log in
  • My Cart

Advanced Search

  • Home
  • Articles
    • Current
    • Special Feature Articles - Most Recent
    • Special Features
    • Colloquia
    • Collected Articles
    • PNAS Classics
    • List of Issues
  • Front Matter
    • Front Matter Portal
    • Journal Club
  • News
    • For the Press
    • This Week In PNAS
    • PNAS in the News
  • Podcasts
  • Authors
    • Information for Authors
    • Editorial and Journal Policies
    • Submission Procedures
    • Fees and Licenses
  • Submit
Research Article

Culture, distress, and oxytocin receptor polymorphism (OXTR) interact to influence emotional support seeking

Heejung S. Kim, David K. Sherman, Joni Y. Sasaki, Jun Xu, Thai Q. Chu, Chorong Ryu, Eunkook M. Suh, Kelsey Graham, and Shelley E. Taylor
  1. aDepartment of Psychology, University of California, Santa Barbara, CA 93106-9660;
  2. bDepartment of Biomedical Sciences, Tufts University, Grafton, MA 01536;
  3. cDepartment of Psychology, Yonsei University, Seoul 120-749, Korea; and
  4. dDepartment of Psychology, University of California, Los Angeles, CA 90095

See allHide authors and affiliations

PNAS September 7, 2010 107 (36) 15717-15721; https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1010830107
Heejung S. Kim
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
  • For correspondence: kim@psych.ucsb.edu taylors@psych.ucla.edu
David K. Sherman
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Joni Y. Sasaki
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Jun Xu
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Thai Q. Chu
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Chorong Ryu
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Eunkook M. Suh
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Kelsey Graham
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Shelley E. Taylor
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
  • For correspondence: kim@psych.ucsb.edu taylors@psych.ucla.edu
  1. Contributed by Shelley E. Taylor, July 26, 2010 (sent for review June 21, 2010)

  • Article
  • Figures & SI
  • Info & Metrics
  • PDF
Loading

Abstract

Research has demonstrated that certain genotypes are expressed in different forms, depending on input from the social environment. To examine sensitivity to cultural norms regarding emotional support seeking as a type of social environment, we explored the behavioral expression of oxytocin receptor polymorphism (OXTR) rs53576, a gene previously related to socio-emotional sensitivity. Seeking emotional support in times of distress is normative in American culture but not in Korean culture. Consequently, we predicted a three-way interaction of culture, distress, and OXTR genotype on emotional support seeking. Korean and American participants (n = 274) completed assessments of psychological distress and emotional support seeking and were genotyped for OXTR. We found the predicted three-way interaction: among distressed American participants, those with the GG/AG genotypes reported seeking more emotional social support, compared with those with the AA genotype, whereas Korean participants did not differ significantly by genotype; under conditions of low distress, OXTR groups did not differ significantly in either cultural group. These findings suggest that OXTR rs53576 is sensitive to input from the social environment, specifically cultural norms regarding emotional social support seeking. These findings also indicate that psychological distress and culture are important moderators that shape behavioral outcomes associated with OXTR genotypes.

  • genetics
  • social support
  • psychological distress
  • stress coping
  • gene-by-culture interaction

The behavioral expression of certain genotypes is sensitive to input from the social environment (1). Specifically, studies have demonstrated that certain genotypes are expressed in different forms depending upon the harshness/beneficence of social conditions. This interaction between genes and environment has been observed with the serotonin transporter gene (2, 3), the monoamine oxidase A gene (4), the dopamine receptor gene seven-repeat polymorphism (5), and the glucocorticoid receptor gene (6), among others (7). For example, in the case of the serotonin transporter gene, individuals who are homozygous for the allele associated with lower transcriptional efficiency of the promoter (s/s) are at a greater risk for depressive symptomatology in unsupportive social environments, but those with the same genotype show lower depressive symptomatology in supportive social environments, compared with those with s/l or l/l genotypes (3, reviewed in ref. 8). Persons with the genotype for low levels of the enzyme metabolizing the neurotransmitters serotonin, epinephrine and norepinephrine [monoamine oxidase A (MAOA)] may be more likely to develop antisocial behavior as adults if maltreated as children than persons with the high MAOA activity genotype (4).

Guided by this perspective, the present investigation explored the possibility that an oxytocin receptor gene (OXTR rs53576) may be sensitive to social input in the form of culture-specific relational norms. OXTR rs53576 is a polymorphic site in the oxytocin receptor gene, which is localized in a single copy to chromosome 3 of the human genome (9). Recent studies have identified a connection between OXTR and social behavior phenotypes in humans. Individuals with the G allele of OXTR rs53576, relative to those with the A allele, exhibit more sensitive parenting behavior (10), report being less lonely (11), show more empathy (12), and have lower rates of autism (13). In addition to findings regarding OXTR, research links social and emotional sensitivity to oxytocin as a neuropeptide. For example, intranasal administration of oxytocin leads to increased ability to infer the affective mental state of others (14) and greater interpersonal trust (15).

We examined whether culture-specific norms regarding relationships represent a source of social input that affects phenotypic expression of OXTR. We focused on norms surrounding the behavior of seeking emotional support—turning to one's social network for emotional solace—in times of stress. Emotional support has been found to ameliorate both psychological and biological stress reactivity (16), and the effects of lacking support on health and mortality are on par with well-established risk factors such as lipid levels and smoking (17). As such, it is an important behavioral response to stress. Emotional support seeking is a common response to stress that is influenced by both genetic factors (18) and culture (19–21).

Culture is a system of beliefs, institutions, and practices that govern norms and patterns of behaviors (22), including emotional support seeking. The norms regarding the seeking of emotional support differ markedly between Asian and American cultures, however. Across many different types of stressors, such as academic, health and social stressors, Asians are less likely to seek emotional support for dealing with their stressful events compared with European Americans (19–21). This cultural difference emerges with both open-ended assessments and standard coping inventories (20). The effectiveness of support seeking also varies cross-culturally, as it can have a negative impact on the psychological and biological stress responses of Asians responding to laboratory stressors (23). This cultural difference occurs because Asians are more concerned about the potential negative relational implications of support seeking than are European Americans (20, 21). Furthermore, studies (20) have found that U.S.-born Asians with greater exposure to the U.S. culture seek emotional support more than Asian immigrants and Asian nationals with less exposure to the U.S. culture, and the difference between the U.S.-born Asians and European Americans is minimal, supporting the argument that this difference is rooted in culture (ref. 19 discusses these generational patterns). We examined whether Koreans and Americans differed in the link between OXTR rs53576 and emotional support seeking for managing distress.

In the present study, Koreans’ and Americans’ current psychological distress in response to a recent stressor and their seeking of emotional support were assessed, and they were genotyped for OXTR. (The American group included a small group of U.S.-born Korean Americans as comparisons who are genetically similar to Koreans but who were raised in American culture.) Consistent with prior research (19–21), we predicted that Americans would report using emotional support more than Koreans to manage distress. We further predicted that, under conditions of high distress, Americans with the GG/AG genotype [note that, consistent with approaches in other studies to deal with skewed genotypic distribution (10, 24), we combine the heterozygous genotype with the rarest homozygous genotype], who are more sensitive to relational norms, would seek emotional support more than Americans with the AA genotype, because emotional support seeking is a normative coping response in the United States. In contrast, Koreans with the GG/AG genotype would not increase support seeking any more than those with the AA genotype, because emotional support seeking is a nonnormative coping response. We did not expect these differences to emerge in the absence of distress. Thus, we predicted a three-way interaction among culture, psychological distress, and OXTR genotype on seeking emotional support.

Results

An ANOVA was conducted to examine whether there were cultural and genetic differences in the level of psychological distress, a composite measure of the Perceived Stress Scale (25) and the Brief Symptoms Inventory (26). There were no significant main effects for culture or genotype and no interaction on this measure (P values range between 0.12 and 0.97). Because the distribution of emotional support scores skewed left, the variable was transformed by squaring it before analysis. (Reported means and SDs are not transformed for ease of interpretation.)

A series of hierarchical linear regression analyses were conducted to test the gene–culture interaction hypothesis that culture would moderate the relationship among OXTR, psychological distress and emotional support seeking. At Step 1, the contrast-coded culture variable (−1 for Americans and 1 for Koreans), the contrast-coded OXTR variable (−1 for AA, and 1 for GG/AG), and the level of psychological distress (mean centered) were entered as predictors. At Step 1, there was the predicted cultural difference in reported emotional support seeking [β = 0.31, t(270) = −5.16, P < 0.001], as Koreans reported seeking less emotional support than did Americans. There were no other significant main effects. At Step 2, the two-way interactions were entered. There were no significant two-way interactions. At Step 3, the three-way interaction of culture, OXTR, and psychological distress was entered. As predicted, there was a significant three-way interaction among culture, OXTR and psychological distress [β = −0.23, t(266) = −3.53, P < 0.001, ΔR2 = 0.04 (the three-way interaction remains significant if the analysis excludes Korean Americans at P = 0.003)]. When OXTR is examined as three genotypes (−1 for AA, 0 for AG, and 1 for GG), the three-way interaction effect remains significant [β = −0.15, t(266) = −2.49, P = 0.01, ΔR2 = 0.02].

To examine the gene–culture interaction among participants with high and low psychological distress, participants were divided into two groups using a median split. An ANOVA with two factors (culture and genotype) was conducted separately for those with high and low psychological distress. In the high psychological distress group, there was a significant main effect of culture, F(1, 132) = 4.78, P = 0.03, η2 = 0.03, showing that Americans (mean = 4.10, SD = 1.48) reported more emotional support seeking than Koreans (mean = 3.25, SD = 1.43), but no main effect of OXTR, F(1, 132) = 0.58, P = 0.45, η2 = 0.004. The main effect of culture was qualified by the predicted significant interaction between culture and genotype, F(1, 132) = 6.33, P = 0.01, η2 = 0.04, as Fig. 1 shows. Planned pairwise comparisons show that Americans with the GG/AG genotype (mean = 4.24, SD = 1.46) reported seeking emotional support more than those with the AA genotype [mean = 3.44, SD = 1.47, P = 0.04, Cohen's d = 0.55. Analysis with three genotypes (AA, AG, and GG) revealed that among high distress Americans, the AG genotype significantly differed in emotional support seeking from the AA (P = 0.01) but not from the GG (P = 0.11). AG and GG genotypes do not significantly differ from each other in any other comparisons (high-distress Koreans and low-distress Americans and Koreans).] Koreans did not significantly differ by genotypes (mean = 3.01, SD = 1.45 for GG/GA, and mean = 3.55, SD = 1.38 for AA, P = 0.15, Cohen's d = 0.38), although individuals with the GG/AG genotype were somewhat less likely to seek emotional support than those with the AA genotype. The cultural difference in emotional support seeking was driven primarily by persons with the GG/AG genotype. Additional pairwise comparisons revealed that among those with the GG/AG genotype, Americans reported more emotional support seeking than Koreans (P < 0.001, Cohen's d = 0.85), whereas there was no cultural difference among those with the AA genotype (P = 0.85, Cohen's d = 0.08).

Fig. 1.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Fig. 1.

Gene–culture interaction and emotional support seeking, separated by the level of psychological distress (nontransformed means and SEs).

In the low psychological distress group, there was also a signifant main effect of culture [F(1, 134) = 16.11, P < 0.001, η2 = 0.11], showing that Americans (mean = 3.78, SD = 1.52) reported more emotional support seeking than Koreans (mean = 3.05, SD = 1.23), but no significant main effect of OXTR [F(1, 132) = 0.01, P = 0.93, η2 < 0.001]. The interaction was marginally significant [F(1, 134) = 3.24, P = 0.07, η2 = 0.02] (Fig. 1). However, the pairwise comparisons show that American individuals with the AA genotype (mean = 4.22, SD = 1.16) did not significantly differ in their emotional support seeking from individuals with the GG/GA genotype (mean = 3.65, SD = 1.60), P = 0.21, Cohen's d = 0.41. Among Koreans, individuals with the AA genotype (mean = 2.77, SD = 1.17) did not significantly differ from those with the GG/GA genotype (mean = 3.23, SD = 1.25), P = 0.20, Cohen's d = 0.38. Additional pairwise comparisons revealed that among both genotype groups, Americans reported more emotional support seeking than Koreans [P = 0.001, Cohen's d = 1.24 for AA genotype, and, P = 0.048, Cohen's d = 0.40 for GG/AG genotypes].

Supplemental Analyses of Korean Americans.

Koreans and European Americans differ not only in terms of culture, but also in the makeup of genes other than the two genes examined in the present study, which leaves gene–gene interactions as an alternative explanation for the present results regarding OXTR and culture. Thus, supplemental analyses were conducted to compare Korean Americans who were born and raised in the United States with European Americans and Koreans. Korean Americans would, by and large, share genetic makeup with Koreans but would be more culturally “Americanized.” If culture moderates the relationship of OXTR to emotional support seeking, the Americanized Korean Americans’ pattern should resemble the pattern of European Americans more than the pattern of Koreans.

Two separate analyses were conducted, one comparing Korean Americans with Koreans and the other comparing Korean Americans with European Americans in the same hierarchical linear regression analyses testing the gene–culture interaction on emotional support seeking. At Step 1, the contrast-coded culture variable (−1 for European Americans and 1 for Korean Americans in one analysis and −1 for Korean Americans and 1 for Koreans in another), the contrast coded OXTR variable (−1 for AA, and 1 for GG/AG) and the level of psychological distress (mean centered) were entered as predictors. At Step 2, the two-way interactions were entered, and there were no significant two-way interactions. At Step 3, the three-way interaction of culture, OXTR, and psychological distress was entered. In the comparison of Korean Americans and European Americans, there was no significant effect involving culture [β = −0.14, t(136) = −1.49, P = 0.14 for the main effect of culture, and β = 0.02, t(132) = 0.17, P = 0.87, ΔR2 < 0.001 for the three way interaction]. In the comparison of Korean Americans and Koreans, however, there was a significant main effect of culture [β = 0.16, t(162) = 2.15, P = 0.03], such that Korean Americans sought more support than Koreans, and a significant three-way interaction [β = 0.29, t(158) = 2.96, P = 0.004, ΔR2 = 0.05], reflecting the same pattern as that reported in the primary analyses. Taken together, this analysis shows that Korean Americans’ pattern is indeed much more similar to culturally similar European Americans than to genetically similar Koreans.

Discussion

In recent decades, scientific appreciation of the ways in which phenotypic expression of genotypes is affected by environmental input has grown. In particular, it has become clear that several genotypes show sensitivity to input from the social environment, altering and even reversing their phenotypic expression, depending upon social conditions. The present study examined whether cultural norms can also moderate behavioral expression associated with a specific gene.

The G allele of OXTR rs53576 has been linked to socio-emotional sensitivity in previous studies (10–12), and we examined whether it is sensitive to culture-specific relational norms associated with seeking emotional support in response to stress. There is a robust cultural difference in social norms regarding emotional support seeking, such that American norms favor it but Korean norms discourage it, a difference that was also supported by the results of the present study; that is, Americans reported seeking more emotional support than Koreans.

Building on these findings, we predicted a three-way interaction such that, under conditions of distress, American G carriers would show more enhanced emotional support seeking than Americans homozygous for the A allele, whereas Koreans would not differ by genotype. No such differences were expected among people experiencing low distress. Consistent with predictions, among participants experiencing high distress, there was a significant interaction between genotype and culture, such that carriers of the G allele showed enhanced sensitivity to cultural norms under conditions of distress. Emotional support seeking was higher among Americans with the OXTR G allele compared with Americans homozygous for the A allele, whereas the reverse but nonsignificant pattern was found in Koreans, resulting in a greater cultural difference among G carriers than people homozygous for the A allele. The present findings add OXTR to a growing list of genes the phenotypic expression of which is substantially modified and even reversed by input from the social environment (1, 3, 8).

The three-way interaction obtained in the present study indicates that distress and cultural norms regarding whether it is appropriate to share distress with others influenced the behavioral outcome of GG and AG genotypes of OXTR. Most of the previous research examining moderation of genotypic expression by input from the social environment has focused on the harshness or beneficence of the social environment (1–8). The results of the present study thus add cultural norms as a form of social input that can affect behavioral expression. That is, culture may affect the decision to seek or not to seek support, in conjunction with the psychological propensities influenced by a particular gene.

The expected cultural difference in emotional support seeking was found among individuals with the GG/AG genotype, regardless of their level of distress, but the same difference was found among individuals with the AA genotype only when their distress level was low. Given these results, it may be that those individuals with the AA genotype are able to respond concordantly with the cultural expectation when their distress level is low, but when their distress level is high, they may be less able to behave in a culturally concordant manner, hence displaying lower sensitivity to culture-specific relational norms.

A notable aspect of OXTR is the pattern of ethnic differences in its allelic distribution, specifically a larger proportion of the A allele among Koreans than European Americans. Because psychological and behavioral differences often have at least some genetic basis (27, 28), and there are ethnic differences in the distribution of genotypes of many polymorphisms (29), researchers have considered how the interaction of culture and genes may explain cultural differences among ethnic groups. One possibility, termed “culture–gene coevolution,” proposes that cultural tendencies are adaptive and evolve and influence the social and physical environments under which genetic selection operates (30, 31). The current study shows a greater proportion of OXTR G allele among Americans than Koreans. To be sure, OXTR is only one of perhaps many genes that could underlie complex social behaviors such as emotional support seeking. Nevertheless, one interesting possibility is that the OXTR distribution difference might be linked to the difference in cultural values placed on emotional support seeking. That is, as there are more individuals with the socially affiliative genotype in America, American culture might foster seeking support from close others as a stress coping strategy, compared with Korean culture, in which there are fewer individuals with the socially affiliative genotype.

Adding to this idea regarding a long-term culture-level adaptation, the present findings provide an example of how genes and culture may interact in shaping behavioral patterns at a more individual and proximal level. Culture is not only constrained by genetics, but also influences the behavioral outcomes of genes (32, 33), and consequently, can result in diverse psychological and behavioral expressions of genotypes. Because the functional effect of the rs57576 polymorphism at the molecular level is presently unknown, whether and how it is related to oxytocin signaling pathways is also unknown. There is a possibility that culture, as a social input, may have an impact at the molecular level (34) as well as at the behavioral level, as shown in the present research. The current findings should encourage researchers to investigate the pathways linking the molecular and behavioral levels to further understand this culture-specific genetic effect.

In the present paper, we focused on a particular polymorphism, OXTR, and the behavior of social support seeking, but our findings underscore the importance of examining the interaction of genetic and cultural influences with respect to other human behaviors as well, especially behaviors that have documented cross-cultural differences (an example of interaction between serotonin receptor gene and culture in cognitive style is discussed in ref. 33). The present findings provide further evidence to the old adage that people are products of both nurture and nature, and our approach provides a potential framework for understanding how these influences interact.

Materials and Methods

Participants were 134 Koreans (63 female and 71 male; 54 community members and 80 college students; mean age = 25.06 y) and 140 Americans (108 European Americans and 32 Korean Americans; 79 females and 61 males; 44 community members and 96 college students; mean age = 24.54 y). (Sex, age, and student/community status did not significantly differ between two cultural groups. We conducted all reported analyses also controlling for these variables, and in all cases, they did not change the significance levels.) The Korean participants were recruited in Korea, and their ethnicity was confirmed by their indication of Korea as the country of birth as well as by their name at the recruitment. Participants were recruited in the United States based on their self-categorized ethnicity. Participants were allowed to choose only one ethnic category among six ethnic groups (e.g., Asian American, European American, African American, Latino American, Native American, Native Pacific Islander), but an “Other” category was provided for those who did not clearly fit into these categories or for those with mixed ethnicities. Asian Americans were asked to further specify their ethnicity. Only those who indicated that they are European American or Korean American and that they were born and raised in the United States were recruited to participate in the study. Student participants were recruited through class announcements, and community participants were recruited among campus employees and from adult classes in both countries. Participants received either course credit or payment ($10 or 10,000Embedded Image for students and $20 or 20,000Embedded Image for community members) for their participation.

Procedure and Measures.

Participants completed questionnaires assessing their level of psychological distress and their tendency to seek social support. A bilingual assistant translated the scales developed in English into Korean, and another independent bilingual assistant translated the Korean scales back to English to ensure the accuracy of the translation for Korean participants. After completion of the questionnaires and demographics, participants provided saliva samples for genetic analyses.

Psychological distress.

As measures of current psychological distress, participants completed the Perceived Stress Scale (PSS) (25) and the Brief Symptoms Inventory (BSI) (26). The PSS is a 10-item scale that assessed the level of stress experienced over the last month on a scale of 0 (never) to 4 (very often) (α = 0.77 among Koreans and α = 0.85 among Americans; e.g., In the last month, how often have you felt nervous and “stressed”?). The BSI included a list of 53 psychological symptoms (e.g., feeling so restless you couldn't sit still or feeling easily annoyed or irritated) for which participants rated their experiences in the last 7 d on scale of 0 (not at all) to 4 (extremely) (α = 0.97 among Koreans and α = 0.96 among Americans). Responses to these two scales (intercorrelation r = 0.65, P < 0.001 for Americans and r = 0.57, P < 0.001 for Koreans) were standardized and averaged to yield an index of psychological distress.

Emotional support seeking.

To prompt participants to think about specific stressful experiences and how they coped with them, participants were asked to describe their biggest stressor of the last 3 mo. The participants then reported how they coped with the stressor that they just described, by completing a standardized coping inventory (35) used in our prior research (20, 21). Emotional social support seeking was measured by four emotional coping items (α = 0.83 among Koreans and α = 0.90 among Americans) (“I tried to get emotional support from friends or relatives.” “I discussed my feelings with someone.” “I talked to someone about how I felt.” “I got comfort and understanding from someone.”). Participants reported how much they used each strategy on a scale from 0 (not at all) to 6 (very much).

Genotyping.

After the completion of questionnaire measures, participants provided saliva or cheek swab samples collected with the OraSure oral specimen collection device (OraSure Technologies for cheek swab samples) or Oragene collection device, for saliva samples (Genotek). The Oragene samples were kept at room temperature, and the OraSure samples were stored at −20 °C for 3–4 mo until processed. DNA was extracted using the Puregene DNA purification kit (Gentra Systems). Concentrations were determined on a spectraphotometer and equalized across samples by diluting the high-concentration samples with water.

The genotype of the OXTR rs53576 polymorphism was then assessed using a commercially available TaqMan SNP genotyping assay. The SNP assay contains forward and reverse PCR primers as well as two allele-specific probes conjugated with either VIC or FAM fluorescent marker. Each PCR mixture consisted of DNA templates, the SNP-specific genotyping assay, and Taqman Genotype master mix (ABI). PCR amplification was carried out on an ABI 7500 real-time PCR System following the PCR conditions recommended by the manufacturer of the SNP probe. After the PCR reactions, the allelic discrimination program (ABI) generated a genotype plot in which samples were separated into four clusters, representing the GG, GA, AA, and undetermined genotypes for OXTR. All samples were run in duplicate, which, in all cases, were confirmed to be consistent.

There was a higher proportion of A allele of OXTR among Koreans (57 AA, 55 AG, and 22 GG) than among Americans [29 AA, 54 AG, and 57 GG), χ2(2, n = 274) = 24.51, P < 0.001]. Americans included European Americans (13 AA, 41 AG, and 54 GG) and Korean Americans (16 AA, 13 AG, and 3 GG). European Americans’ distribution significantly differed from that of Koreans [χ2(2, n = 242) = 40.85, P < 0.001], but Korean Americans’ distribution did not [χ2(2, n = 166) = 1.18, P = 0.56]. Korean Americans’ distribution significantly differed from European Americans [χ2(2, n = 140) = 27.23, P < 0.001], showing that, at least in terms of OXTR genotype distribution, Korean Americans were more similar to Koreans than to European Americans. These genotypes did not deviate from Hardy–Weinberg equilibrium in any cultural group [χ2(2, n = 134) = 1.90, P = 0.17 for Koreans; χ2(2, n = 32) = 0.02, P = 0.88 for Korean Americans; and χ2(2, n = 108) = 1.38, P = 0.24 for European Americans]. These distributions are comparable to the OXTR genotypic distributions from previous studies with ethnically similar samples (11, 13).

Acknowledgments

This research was supported by National Science Foundation Human Social Dynamic Grants BCS-0729532 and BCS-0734230.

Footnotes

  • 1To whom correspondence may be addressed. E-mail: kim{at}psych.ucsb.edu or taylors{at}psych.ucla.edu.
  • Author contributions: H.S.K., D.K.S., J.X., and S.E.T. designed research; H.S.K., D.K.S., J.Y.S., T.Q.C., C.R., and E.M.S. performed research; H.S.K., D.K.S., J.Y.S., J.X., T.Q.C., and K.G. analyzed data; and H.S.K., D.K.S., and S.E.T. wrote the paper.

  • The authors declare no conflict of interest.

    References

    1. ↵
      1. Way BM,
      2. Taylor SE
      (2010) Social influences on health: Is serotonin a critical mediator? Psychosom Med 72:107–112.
      OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
    2. ↵
      1. Caspi A,
      2. et al.
      (2003) Influence of life stress on depression: Moderation by a polymorphism in the 5-HTT gene. Science 301:386–389.
      OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
    3. ↵
      1. Taylor SE,
      2. et al.
      (2006) Early family environment, current adversity, the serotonin transporter promoter polymorphism, and depressive symptomatology. Biol Psychiatry 60:671–676.
      OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
    4. ↵
      1. Caspi A,
      2. et al.
      (2002) Role of genotype in the cycle of violence in maltreated children. Science 297:851–854.
      OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
    5. ↵
      1. Baskermans-Kranenburg MJ,
      2. Ilzendoorn MH
      (2006) Gene-environment interaction of the dopamine D4 receptor (DRD4) and observed maternal insensitivity predicting externalizing behavior in preschoolers. Dev Psychol 48:406–409.
      OpenUrlCrossRef
    6. ↵
      1. Meaney MJ,
      2. Szyf M
      (2005) Environmental programming of stress responses through DNA methylation: Life at the interface between a dynamic environment and a fixed genome. Dialogues Clin Neurosci 7:103–123.
      OpenUrlPubMed
    7. ↵
      1. Moffitt TE,
      2. Caspi A,
      3. Rutter M
      (2006) Measured gene-environment interactions in psychopathology: Concepts, research strategies and implications for research, intervention and public understanding of genetics. Perspect Psychol Sci 1:5–27.
      OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
    8. ↵
      1. Obradović J,
      2. Boyce WT
      (2009) Individual differences in behavioral, physiological, and genetic sensitivities to contexts: Implications for development and adaptation. Dev Neurosci 31:300–308.
      OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
    9. ↵
      1. Gimpl G,
      2. Fahrenholz F
      (2001) The oxytocin receptor system: Structure, function, and regulation. Physiol Rev 81:629–683.
      OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
    10. ↵
      1. Bakermans-Kranenburg MJ,
      2. van Ijzendoorn MH
      (2008) Oxytocin receptor (OXTR) and serotonin transporter (5-HTT) genes associated with observed parenting. Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci 3:128–134.
      OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
    11. ↵
      1. Lucht MJ,
      2. et al.
      (2009) Associations between the oxytocin receptor gene (OXTR) and affect, loneliness and intelligence in normal subjects. Prog Neuropsychopharmacol Biol Psychiatry 33:860–866.
      OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
    12. ↵
      1. Rodrigues SM,
      2. Saslow LR,
      3. Garcia N,
      4. John OP,
      5. Keltner D
      (2009) Oxytocin receptor genetic variation relates to empathy and stress reactivity in humans. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 106:21437–21441.
      OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
    13. ↵
      1. Wu S,
      2. et al.
      (2005) Positive association of the oxytocin receptor gene (OXTR) with autism in the Chinese Han population. Biol Psychiatry 58:74–77.
      OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
    14. ↵
      1. Domes G,
      2. Heinrichs M,
      3. Michel A,
      4. Berger C,
      5. Herpertz SC
      (2007) Oxytocin improves “mind-reading” in humans. Biol Psychiatry 61:731–733.
      OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
    15. ↵
      1. Kosfeld M,
      2. Heinrichs M,
      3. Zak PJ,
      4. Fischbacher U,
      5. Fehr E
      (2005) Oxytocin increases trust in humans. Nature 435:673–676.
      OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
    16. ↵
      1. Eisenberger NI,
      2. Taylor SE,
      3. Gable SL,
      4. Hilmert CJ,
      5. Lieberman MD
      (2007) Neural pathways link social support to attenuated neuroendocrine stress responses. Neuroimage 35:1601–1612.
      OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
    17. ↵
      1. House JS,
      2. Landis KR,
      3. Umberson D
      (1988) Social relationships and health. Science 241:540–545.
      OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
    18. ↵
      1. Busjahn A,
      2. Faulhaber HD,
      3. Freier K,
      4. Luft FC
      (1999) Genetic and environmental influences on coping styles: A twin study. Psychosom Med 61:469–475.
      OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
    19. ↵
      1. Kim HS,
      2. Sherman DK,
      3. Taylor SE
      (2008) Culture and social support. Am Psychol 63:518–526.
      OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
    20. ↵
      1. Taylor SE,
      2. et al.
      (2004) Culture and social support: Who seeks it and why? J Pers Soc Psychol 87:354–362.
      OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
    21. ↵
      1. Kim HS,
      2. Sherman DK,
      3. Ko D,
      4. Taylor SE
      (2006) Pursuit of comfort and pursuit of harmony: Culture, relationships, and social support seeking. Pers Soc Psychol Bull 32:1595–1607.
      OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
    22. ↵
      1. Kitayama S
      (2002) Culture and basic psychological processes—toward a system view of culture: Comment on Oyserman et al. (2002) Psychol Bull 128:89–96.
      OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
    23. ↵
      1. Taylor SE,
      2. Welch WT,
      3. Kim HS,
      4. Sherman DK
      (2007) Cultural differences in the impact of social support on psychological and biological stress responses. Psychol Sci 18:831–837.
      OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
    24. ↵
      1. Costa B,
      2. et al.
      (2009) Oxytocin receptor polymorphisms and adult attachment style in patients with depression. Psychoneuroendocrinology 34:1506–1514.
      OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
    25. ↵
      1. Cohen S,
      2. Kamarck T,
      3. Mermelstein R
      (1983) A global measure of perceived stress. J Health Soc Behav 24:385–396.
      OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
    26. ↵
      1. Derogatis LR,
      2. Spencer PM
      (1982) Brief Symptom Inventory: Administration, Scoring, and Procedures Manual-I (Clinical Psychometric Research, Baltimore).
    27. ↵
      1. Kessler RC,
      2. Kendler KS,
      3. Heath A,
      4. Neale MC,
      5. Eaves LJ
      (1992) Social support, depressed mood, and adjustment to stress: A genetic epidemiologic investigation. J Pers Soc Psychol 62:257–272.
      OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
    28. ↵
      1. Strobel A,
      2. et al.
      (2003) Allelic variation in 5-HT1A receptor expression is associated with anxiety- and depression-related personality traits. J Neural Transm 110:1445–1453.
      OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
    29. ↵
      1. Gelernter J,
      2. Kranzler H,
      3. Cubells JF
      (1997) Serotonin transporter protein (SLC6A4) allele and haplotype frequencies and linkage disequilibria in African- and European-American and Japanese populations and in alcohol-dependent subjects. Hum Genet 101:243–246.
      OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
    30. ↵
      1. Boyd R,
      2. Richerson P
      (1985) Culture and the Evolutionary Process (University of Chicago Press, Chicago).
    31. ↵
      1. Chiao JY,
      2. Blizinsky KD
      (2010) Culture-gene coevolution of individualism-collectivism and the serotonin transporter gene. Proc Biol Sci 277:529–537.
      OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
    32. ↵
      1. Lumsden CJ,
      2. Wilson EO
      (1981) Genes, Mind, & Culture (Harvard University Press, Cambridge, MA).
    33. ↵
      1. Kim HS,
      2. et al.
      (2010) Culture, serotonin receptor polymorphism and locus of attention. Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci 5:212–218.
      OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
    34. ↵
      1. Cole SW
      (2009) Social regulation of human gene expression. Curr Dir Psychol Sci 18:132–137.
      OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
    35. ↵
      1. Carver CS,
      2. Scheier MF,
      3. Weintraub JK
      (1989) Assessing coping strategies: A theoretically based approach. J Pers Soc Psychol 56:267–283.
      OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
    PreviousNext
    Back to top
    Article Alerts
    Email Article

    Thank you for your interest in spreading the word on PNAS.

    NOTE: We only request your email address so that the person you are recommending the page to knows that you wanted them to see it, and that it is not junk mail. We do not capture any email address.

    Enter multiple addresses on separate lines or separate them with commas.
    Culture, distress, and oxytocin receptor polymorphism (OXTR) interact to influence emotional support seeking
    (Your Name) has sent you a message from PNAS
    (Your Name) thought you would like to see the PNAS web site.
    CAPTCHA
    This question is for testing whether or not you are a human visitor and to prevent automated spam submissions.
    Citation Tools
    Culture, distress, and oxytocin receptor polymorphism (OXTR) interact to influence emotional support seeking
    Heejung S. Kim, David K. Sherman, Joni Y. Sasaki, Jun Xu, Thai Q. Chu, Chorong Ryu, Eunkook M. Suh, Kelsey Graham, Shelley E. Taylor
    Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences Sep 2010, 107 (36) 15717-15721; DOI: 10.1073/pnas.1010830107

    Citation Manager Formats

    • BibTeX
    • Bookends
    • EasyBib
    • EndNote (tagged)
    • EndNote 8 (xml)
    • Medlars
    • Mendeley
    • Papers
    • RefWorks Tagged
    • Ref Manager
    • RIS
    • Zotero
    Request Permissions
    Share
    Culture, distress, and oxytocin receptor polymorphism (OXTR) interact to influence emotional support seeking
    Heejung S. Kim, David K. Sherman, Joni Y. Sasaki, Jun Xu, Thai Q. Chu, Chorong Ryu, Eunkook M. Suh, Kelsey Graham, Shelley E. Taylor
    Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences Sep 2010, 107 (36) 15717-15721; DOI: 10.1073/pnas.1010830107
    del.icio.us logo Digg logo Reddit logo Twitter logo CiteULike logo Facebook logo Google logo Mendeley logo
    • Tweet Widget
    • Facebook Like
    • Mendeley logo Mendeley

    Article Classifications

    • Social Sciences
    • Psychological and Cognitive Sciences
    Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences: 107 (36)
    Table of Contents

    Submit

    Sign up for Article Alerts

    Jump to section

    • Article
      • Abstract
      • Results
      • Discussion
      • Materials and Methods
      • Acknowledgments
      • Footnotes
      • References
    • Figures & SI
    • Info & Metrics
    • PDF

    You May Also be Interested in

    Water from a faucet fills a glass.
    News Feature: How “forever chemicals” might impair the immune system
    Researchers are exploring whether these ubiquitous fluorinated molecules might worsen infections or hamper vaccine effectiveness.
    Image credit: Shutterstock/Dmitry Naumov.
    Reflection of clouds in the still waters of Mono Lake in California.
    Inner Workings: Making headway with the mysteries of life’s origins
    Recent experiments and simulations are starting to answer some fundamental questions about how life came to be.
    Image credit: Shutterstock/Radoslaw Lecyk.
    Cave in coastal Kenya with tree growing in the middle.
    Journal Club: Small, sharp blades mark shift from Middle to Later Stone Age in coastal Kenya
    Archaeologists have long tried to define the transition between the two time periods.
    Image credit: Ceri Shipton.
    Illustration of groups of people chatting
    Exploring the length of human conversations
    Adam Mastroianni and Daniel Gilbert explore why conversations almost never end when people want them to.
    Listen
    Past PodcastsSubscribe
    Panda bear hanging in a tree
    How horse manure helps giant pandas tolerate cold
    A study finds that giant pandas roll in horse manure to increase their cold tolerance.
    Image credit: Fuwen Wei.

    Similar Articles

    Site Logo
    Powered by HighWire
    • Submit Manuscript
    • Twitter
    • Facebook
    • RSS Feeds
    • Email Alerts

    Articles

    • Current Issue
    • Special Feature Articles – Most Recent
    • List of Issues

    PNAS Portals

    • Anthropology
    • Chemistry
    • Classics
    • Front Matter
    • Physics
    • Sustainability Science
    • Teaching Resources

    Information

    • Authors
    • Editorial Board
    • Reviewers
    • Subscribers
    • Librarians
    • Press
    • Cozzarelli Prize
    • Site Map
    • PNAS Updates
    • FAQs
    • Accessibility Statement
    • Rights & Permissions
    • About
    • Contact

    Feedback    Privacy/Legal

    Copyright © 2021 National Academy of Sciences. Online ISSN 1091-6490