Skip to main content
  • Submit
  • About
    • Editorial Board
    • PNAS Staff
    • FAQ
    • Accessibility Statement
    • Rights and Permissions
    • Site Map
  • Contact
  • Journal Club
  • Subscribe
    • Subscription Rates
    • Subscriptions FAQ
    • Open Access
    • Recommend PNAS to Your Librarian
  • Log in
  • My Cart

Main menu

  • Home
  • Articles
    • Current
    • Special Feature Articles - Most Recent
    • Special Features
    • Colloquia
    • Collected Articles
    • PNAS Classics
    • List of Issues
  • Front Matter
  • News
    • For the Press
    • This Week In PNAS
    • PNAS in the News
  • Podcasts
  • Authors
    • Information for Authors
    • Editorial and Journal Policies
    • Submission Procedures
    • Fees and Licenses
  • Submit
  • About
    • Editorial Board
    • PNAS Staff
    • FAQ
    • Accessibility Statement
    • Rights and Permissions
    • Site Map
  • Contact
  • Journal Club
  • Subscribe
    • Subscription Rates
    • Subscriptions FAQ
    • Open Access
    • Recommend PNAS to Your Librarian

User menu

  • Log in
  • My Cart

Search

  • Advanced search
Home
Home

Advanced Search

  • Home
  • Articles
    • Current
    • Special Feature Articles - Most Recent
    • Special Features
    • Colloquia
    • Collected Articles
    • PNAS Classics
    • List of Issues
  • Front Matter
  • News
    • For the Press
    • This Week In PNAS
    • PNAS in the News
  • Podcasts
  • Authors
    • Information for Authors
    • Editorial and Journal Policies
    • Submission Procedures
    • Fees and Licenses

New Research In

Physical Sciences

Featured Portals

  • Physics
  • Chemistry
  • Sustainability Science

Articles by Topic

  • Applied Mathematics
  • Applied Physical Sciences
  • Astronomy
  • Computer Sciences
  • Earth, Atmospheric, and Planetary Sciences
  • Engineering
  • Environmental Sciences
  • Mathematics
  • Statistics

Social Sciences

Featured Portals

  • Anthropology
  • Sustainability Science

Articles by Topic

  • Economic Sciences
  • Environmental Sciences
  • Political Sciences
  • Psychological and Cognitive Sciences
  • Social Sciences

Biological Sciences

Featured Portals

  • Sustainability Science

Articles by Topic

  • Agricultural Sciences
  • Anthropology
  • Applied Biological Sciences
  • Biochemistry
  • Biophysics and Computational Biology
  • Cell Biology
  • Developmental Biology
  • Ecology
  • Environmental Sciences
  • Evolution
  • Genetics
  • Immunology and Inflammation
  • Medical Sciences
  • Microbiology
  • Neuroscience
  • Pharmacology
  • Physiology
  • Plant Biology
  • Population Biology
  • Psychological and Cognitive Sciences
  • Sustainability Science
  • Systems Biology
Research Article

Temporal coding of odor mixtures in an olfactory receptor neuron

Chih-Ying Su, Carlotta Martelli, Thierry Emonet, and John R. Carlson
PNAS March 22, 2011 108 (12) 5075-5080; https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1100369108
Chih-Ying Su
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Carlotta Martelli
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Thierry Emonet
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
John R. Carlson
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
  • For correspondence: john.carlson@yale.edu
  1. Edited by David L. Denlinger, The Ohio State University, Columbus, OH, and approved February 11, 2011 (received for review January 10, 2011)

  • Article
  • Figures & SI
  • Info & Metrics
  • PDF
Loading

Abstract

Most natural odors are mixtures and often elicit percepts distinct from those elicited by their constituents. This emergence of a unique odor quality has long been attributed to central processing. Here we show that sophisticated integration of olfactory information begins in olfactory receptor neurons (ORNs) in Drosophila. Odor mixtures are encoded in the temporal dynamics as well as in the magnitudes of ORN responses. ORNs can respond to an inhibitory odorant with different durations depending on the level of background excitation. ORNs respond to mixtures with distinctive temporal dynamics that reflect the physicochemical properties of the constituent odorants. The insect repellent DEET (N,N-diethyl-m-toluamide), which attenuates odor responses of multiple ORNs, differs from an ORN-specific inhibitor in its effects on temporal dynamics. Our analysis reveals a means by which integration of information from odor mixtures begins in ORNs and provides insight into the contribution of inhibitory stimuli to sensory coding.

A fascinating aspect of the sense of smell is that odor mixtures often have distinctive emergent qualities, and their individual constituents are difficult to identify (1). How the emergent quality of an odor mixture arises is unknown. It is believed to originate mainly from information processing in the central nervous system, as in the insect antennal lobe (2–4) and the vertebrate olfactory bulb (5, 6). However, olfactory receptor neurons (ORNs) also contribute to the integration of olfactory information. For example, some individual rat ORNs respond to a binary odor mixture with response magnitudes, i.e., firing frequencies, that cannot be predicted simply from the responses to its components (7, 8). In addition, studies in moths (9–14) and beetles (15) have provided evidence that information about odorants in a mixture can be integrated in the periphery. However, little is known about how information is processed in ORNs. It is unclear, for example, whether individual ORNs are capable of integrating information via means other than simple alterations of their response magnitudes.

Equally intriguing in sensory coding is the role of inhibitory stimuli. In taste, certain stimuli inhibit vertebrate sweet receptors (16, 17). In olfaction, inhibitory odorants have been identified for olfactory receptors of vertebrates (18–21) and invertebrates (13, 22–26). When delivered as a monomolecular odor stimulus, inhibitors reduce the spontaneous activity of ORNs. However, the low spontaneous activities of most ORNs (13, 22, 23, 27) limit the dynamic range of inhibition, and inhibitory odorants may have greater functional significance when paired with excitatory odorants. Indeed, an inhibitory odorant reduced the intensity of a concurrent excitatory odor stimulus in a human psychophysical study (28), and odorants that inhibited CO2-sensing neurons prevented CO2-mediated avoidance behaviors in Drosophila (24). However, it is not clear whether a binary mixture of an excitatory stimulus and an inhibitory stimulus can be distinguished from an excitatory stimulus alone at a lower intensity. How is a mixture encoded by the primary sensory neuron?

To analyze how ORNs integrate excitatory and inhibitory odor information, we have carried out in vivo single-unit recordings in the relatively simple and well-defined Drosophila olfactory system. Our studies reveal an unexpected capacity of individual ORNs to encode odor mixtures. The results indicate how ORNs may contribute to the generation of unique percepts for odor mixtures. More generally, our findings reveal means by which inhibitory stimuli can shape sensory responses. In addition to reducing the magnitudes of excitatory responses, inhibitors may alter their temporal dynamics. Our results identify degrees of freedom by which primary sensory neurons may encode complex sensory information.

Results and Discussion

Excitatory and Inhibitory Responses to Single Odorants.

We first compared the responses to single excitatory and inhibitory odorants in a well-characterized class of ORN, ab2A, which expresses the Or59b receptor (23, 29, 30). Excitatory responses are elicited in ab2A by several structurally similar esters and ketones (31). The magnitude of the excitatory response increased with increasing odorant concentrations (Fig. S1, Left panels). However, the width of the response, defined as the time interval between the half-maximal points, i.e., between the time points in the rising and falling phases at which the response magnitudes are half of the peak response, remained largely constant (Fig. 1A and Fig. S1). At all concentrations, the excitatory response terminated shortly after the end of the stimulus.

Fig. 1.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Fig. 1.

Comparison of excitatory and inhibitory olfactory responses to single odorants. Single-unit recordings of ab2A ORNs in response to a single excitatory odorant, ethyl acetate (A) (n = 9) or an inhibitory odorant, linalool (B) (n = 24). (C) Excitatory responses of ab7A ORNs to linalool (n = 12). (Left) Raster plots of individual ORN responses to a 500-ms pulse of odorant (gray bar). Each short vertical line represents one spike. (Center) Corresponding peri-stimulus time histograms (PSTHs), spikes binned at 50 ms (shaded area represents SEM but, for ethyl acetate, is too small to be visible). (Right) Response width, defined as the time interval between half-maximal responses. Linalool at 10−3 or lower concentrations did not elicit any inhibitory response. Error bars indicate variations in width due to SEM (Materials and Methods), which are too small to be visible in A and C. Gray dotted lines (Right panels) denote the 500-ms stimulus duration.

Inhibitory responses are elicited in ab2A by linalool, a terpene that lowers the spontaneous activity of the ab2A ORN (22) (Fig. 1B). This inhibition depends on the receptor of the ab2A cell; it is observed following ablation of the neighboring ab2B ORN, and we verified that ectopic expression of the ab2A receptor (Or59b) in a mutant “empty neuron” (23, 32) conferred sensitivity to inhibition by linalool (Fig. S2). In measurements from the endogenous ab2A cell, we observed robust inhibition with linalool at 5 × 10−3 dilutions or at higher concentrations, although not at lower concentrations. In contrast to the ab2A excitatory responses, the width of inhibitory responses increased with higher odorant concentrations (Fig. 1B, Right). We also observed prolonged inhibition by a brief pulse of another ab2A inhibitory odorant, α-terpineol (Fig. S3A). Thus, the inhibitory responses of ab2A lasted much longer than the excitatory responses. We note that when linalool was used as an excitatory odorant, against a different ORN (ab7A), the responses did not extend beyond the length of the odor stimulus (Fig. 1C). Taken together, these results suggest a role for response duration in the coding of inhibitory stimuli in ab2A. In summary, responses of an individual ORN to individual excitatory and inhibitory odorants can differ not only in polarity but also in temporal dynamics.

Inhibition in an Excitatory Background.

Having examined the responses to excitatory and inhibitory odorants individually, we next asked how ORNs encode a pulse of inhibitory odorant in the course of a response to an excitatory background odorant. We examined the response of ab2A to a 500-ms pulse of linalool, superimposed on a constant stream of background excitatory odorant, methyl acetate. Higher concentrations of the background excitatory odorant produced higher levels of background firing rates (Fig. 2A). Interestingly, the duration of inhibition decreased with increasing background excitation. At the maximum background firing levels tested, the duration of the period of inhibition was similar to the duration of the inhibitory stimulus [Fig. 2A, 1ac (5 × 10−5), and Fig. 2B, Upper]. Similar results were observed with another background excitatory odorant, ethyl acetate, suggesting that changes in the duration of inhibition are determined primarily by the degree of background activation, rather than by the identity of the excitatory odorant. One interpretation of the decline in duration of inhibition is that higher levels of the background excitatory odorant may accelerate the recovery from inhibition.

Fig. 2.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Fig. 2.

Changes in the duration of inhibition with background excitation. (A) (Left) Raster plots and the corresponding PSTHs of ab2A spike activity to a 500-ms pulse of linalool (10−1) (gray bar), with increasing concentrations of a background excitatory odorant, methyl acetate (1ac). Recordings were made when excitation was at steady state. (Right) Gray traces represent solvent control, a pulse of paraffin oil; blue traces represent a pulse of linalool (10−1). The mean prestimulus background firing rates are indicated in parentheses (n = 24). As a control, we confirmed, using a photoionization detector, that the presence of the background excitatory odorant had little if any effect on the time course of individual linalool stimulus. (B) Average width of the inhibitory responses and magnitude of inhibition, plotted as a function of average background firing rate. Error bars: ±SEM except for “Width,” which indicates variations in width due to SEM.

The concentration of linalool is encoded not only by the temporal dynamics of inhibition, but also by the magnitude of the inhibitory effect. Even at high levels of background excitation, linalool inhibited ab2A firing nearly completely (Fig. 2A). Thus, the magnitude of inhibition, defined here as the reduction in number of spikes, increased proportionally with the background firing rate (Fig. 2B, Lower). An important corollary of these results is that the response to a mixture of an inhibitory odorant and an excitatory odorant cannot be determined simply by summing the effects each has individually on the magnitude of the ORN firing rate.

We note finally that other receptor-odorant combinations, one including a mosquito olfactory receptor, AgOr8 (33–35), also showed a reduction in the duration of inhibition and an increase in the magnitude of inhibition following increases in the level of background excitation (Fig. S3). These results suggest that the use of these parameters to represent odor mixtures in insects may be a common principle.

Sharpened ORN Response to an Odor Mixture.

Above we have analyzed the effects of superimposing a pulse of one odor upon a prolonged stimulus of another odor. Next we considered a pulse of a binary mixture of two odorants intended to simulate a blend of components in a food source. We compared the response to a pulse of a mixture of an excitatory odorant (methyl butyrate) and an inhibitory odorant (linalool) to the response to a pulse of the excitatory odorant and the diluent (paraffin oil). At lower concentrations, methyl butyrate elicited small excitatory responses from ab2A (peak response <50 spikes), which could be completely abolished by the inhibitory odorant, linalool (Fig. S4A). These results are consistent with a previous report that inhibition can override excitation in a binary odor mixture (36).

With higher methyl butyrate concentrations, however, linalool could only attenuate, not abolish, methyl butyrate-elicited excitatory responses (Fig. S4A). Interestingly, the attenuation was more pronounced during the falling phase of the response to the mixture, and the resulting response appeared “sharpened” (Fig. 3 A and B and Fig. S4A). In fact, linalool nearly halved the width of methyl butyrate-elicited responses to ~200 ms (Fig. 3C, Upper Left).

Fig. 3.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Fig. 3.

Sharpening of the excitatory response by an inhibitory odorant in a mixture. (A) Responses of ab2A ORNs to an odor mixture containing equal parts of an excitatory odorant, methyl butyrate (1bu), and either an inhibitory odorant, linalool (Lin, 10−1), or a solvent control, paraffin oil (PO) (n = 9). (B) PSTHs (Upper Left and Right) shown in A and the respective derivatives (Lower Left and Right), showing the maximum rising (solid arrows, Vmax_rise) and first maximum falling speed (open arrows, Vmax_fall). (C) Comparison of the excitatory response dynamics of methyl butyrate (10−5 to 5 × 10−2) with either paraffin oil (red) or an inhibitory odorant, linalool (10−1) (blue). Width: the time interval of the half-maximal responses; t_fall50: the time point when the response falls to its half-maximal magnitude. Linalool at a lower concentration (10−2) caused a similar but smaller transformation in response dynamics. Recordings in A–C were conducted in the same ORNs. Values are plotted as a function of peak response. (D) Normalized PSTHs of ab2A to a 500-ms pulse of a methyl butyrate/linalool mixture and a 200-ms pulse of methyl butyrate (n = 9). (E–G) Similar to A–C, showing ab2A responses to methyl butyrate alone or as a mixture containing equal parts of methyl butyrate and DEET (100%, 50 μL), which is not soluble in paraffin oil (n = 9). (H) Photoionization detector (PID) measurements of odor stimuli, presented as 500-ms puffs of methyl butyrate alone or methyl butyrate mixed with DEET (n = 12). The photoionization potential of DEET is outside the detection range of the PID, and thus the PID signals represent only methyl butyrate concentrations in the gaseous phase.

Notably, the fast-falling dynamics of the response to a 500-ms pulse of the mixture also differed from those of the response to a 200-ms pulse of methyl butyrate alone over a variety of concentrations (Fig. 3D and Fig. S4B). Thus, in the periphery, an ORN is capable of encoding a binary odor mixture with unique temporal dynamics that distinguish the mixture from the excitatory odorant alone, even when the excitatory odorant is presented at different concentrations or with different stimulus durations.

We reasoned that the unique response dynamics elicited by linalool may arise from a different effect on the rising and falling phases of the response. Indeed, the time required for the response to fall from its peak to its half-maximal magnitude (t_fall50) was much shorter in the presence of linalool (Fig. 3C, Upper Right), whereas no difference was observed in the time required for the response to rise to its half-maximal magnitude. We examined the derivative of the response to quantify the rate at which responses rise and fall (Fig. 3B, Lower Left and Right). The analysis showed that linalool did not affect the rising process, but accelerated the falling process (Fig. 3C, Lower Left and Right), as measured by the maximum rising rate and the first maximum falling rate (solid arrowheads and open arrowheads, respectively, in Fig. 3B, Lower Left and Right).

In addition, to visualize the “net effect” of the inhibitory odorant in a mixture, we subtracted the control responses (methyl butyrate mixed with paraffin oil) from the responses to the binary odor mixture containing methyl butyrate and linalool (Fig. S5). Consistent with the notion that the response to a mixture of an inhibitory odorant and an excitatory odorant cannot be determined simply by summing the effects each has individually on the magnitude of the ORN firing rate (Fig. 2), the net inhibitory effect of linalool in a binary mixture varied depending on the concentration of the concurrent excitatory odorant, methyl butyrate (Fig. S5).

DEET Attenuation Does Not Change Response Dynamics.

Having found that linalool, an ORN-specific inhibitor, affects the temporal dynamics of the response to a concurrent excitatory stimulus, we next asked whether a putative inhibitor of special interest, DEET (N,N-diethyl-m-toluamide), has similar effects on response dynamics. DEET attenuates odor responses in multiple ORN classes by blocking the common coreceptor Or83b (37) and/or by reducing the level of the concurrent excitatory odorant vapor (38), referred to as a fixative effect. We compared methyl butyrate-elicited responses with or without DEET. Indeed, DEET attenuated the responses elicited by methyl butyrate at higher concentrations (Fig. 3E). However, such attenuation resulted in little if any change in the response dynamics (Fig. 3 F and G). These results suggested that DEET differs from linalool in the mechanism by which it attenuates the activation of ab2A by methyl butyrate. Photoionization detector (PID) measurements showed that DEET reduced methyl butyrate concentration in the gaseous phase (Fig. 3H), supporting the notion that DEET has a fixative effect on other odorants (38). Our results do not exclude the possibility of additional inhibitory effects of DEET.

Odor Stimuli Differ in Their Temporal Dynamics.

Why does linalool predominantly affect the falling phase of the excitatory response? PID measurements suggested that, although linalool and methyl butyrate were premixed in solution and carried via the same puff of air, their vapors may reach the fly's antenna at different rates (Fig. 4A). At all concentrations tested, levels of methyl butyrate vapor rose faster than linalool, as reflected by the smaller rising time constants (Fig. 4B; for τon, see Materials and Methods). Consequently, methyl butyrate-elicited excitatory responses also rose faster than linalool-elicited inhibitory responses, which may explain why the falling phase of methyl butyrate-elicited excitatory response was preferentially inhibited by linalool.

Fig. 4.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Fig. 4.

Temporal dynamics of excitatory and inhibitory odor stimuli. (A and C) Photoionization detector (PID) measurements of odor stimuli, presented as 500-ms pulses (gray bars) (n = 12). Odor dilutions are indicated on the right in each panel. Information about saturated vapor pressure (VP, at 25 °C) and boiling point (bp, at 760 mmHg) of individual compounds was obtained from http://www.chemspider.com and is indicated in parentheses. (B and D) (Left) Normalized PID measurements of methyl butyrate (10−2) and linalool (10−1) (B) and of 4-methylphenol (5 × 10−2) and 6-methyl-5-hepten-2-one (10−3) (D). (Right in B and D) Comparisons of the odorant arrival kinetics. PID signals were normalized to the respective peak responses in A and C. The rising time constant (τon) was derived from PID measurements of each individual odorant.

To verify this interpretation, we paired linalool with three other ab2A excitatory odorants (methyl acetate, ethyl acetate, and 2,3-butanedione), all of which demonstrate fast-rising PID profiles like that of methyl butyrate (Fig. S6). The physicochemical similarity shared by these strong ab2A excitatory odorants may contribute to the similarity of their PID profiles. As expected, we observed comparable asymmetric sharpening of the response dynamics by linalool in all three combinations (Fig. S6). Thus, in an odor mixture, a slow-arriving inhibitory odorant can sharpen the response elicited by a fast-arriving excitatory odorant. Our findings support the notions that ORN responses can reflect the relative differences in stimulus dynamics and that insect olfactory systems can respond to odor stimuli with high temporal precision (39).

Biphasic Response to an Odor Mixture.

We next considered the reciprocal situation: the pairing of a fast-arriving inhibitory odorant (6-methyl-5-hepten-2-one) with a slow-arriving excitatory odorant (4-methylphenol), which act on AgOr1 (34, 35, 40). For all concentrations tested, 6-methyl-5-hepten-2-one vapor rose faster than 4-methylphenol (Fig. 4 C and D). Interestingly, at a low concentration of the excitatory odorant 4-methylphenol, addition of the inhibitor 6-methyl-5-hepten-2-one resulted in a biphasic response (Fig. 5A). At a higher concentration of the excitatory odorant, a single, prolonged plateau was observed (Fig. 5B). At either concentration, the temporal dynamics differed from those observed for the excitatory odorant alone. We note that, in addition to rising slowly, 4-methylphenol vapor fell slowly (Fig. 4B), which may contribute to the long duration of the excitatory responses in AgOr1 (Fig. 5). Taken together, in addition to attenuating the excitatory responses, an inhibitory odorant in a mixture can generate distinctive response dynamics, depending on the properties and concentrations of the two odorants. Thus, an individual ORN may respond with distinctive dynamics that serve as a unique signature for a given blend of odorants.

Fig. 5.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Fig. 5.

Unique temporal dynamics elicited by an inhibitory odorant in a mixture of 4-methylphenol and 6-methyl-5-hepten-2-one are excitatory and inhibitory odorants for a mosquito receptor, AgOr1, respectively. Responses of AgOr1-expressing ORNs in response to a 500-ms pulse of an odor mixture containing equal parts of 4-methylphenol, 10−5 in A and 10−4 in B, and either 6-methyl-5-hepten-2-one (6m5h2on, 10−1) or solvent control (paraffin oil). Recordings were conducted in the same ORNs (n = 9).

Finally, we note that the relative differences in the PID profiles of fast and slow odorants were observed consistently with different odor delivery methods (Fig. S7 and SI Text). The relatively slow kinetics of linalool emission was also observed using a method in which odor delivery tubes were not used (41). Most “fast” odorants in our study have higher saturated vapor pressures and lower boiling points than the “slow” odorants, which is indicative of higher volatilities (Fig. 4).

Functional Implications for Higher-Order Neurons.

As the temporal dynamics of the postsynaptic projection neurons (PNs) largely follow the temporal dynamics of the presynaptic ORNs (42), the unique temporal features of ORN responses to a given odor mixture are likely to be preserved in the PN responses. To address the effects of ORN inhibition on PN response, we used a formula that predicts the postsynaptic PN responses on the basis of the presynaptic ORN responses (43). We found a striking inhibitory effect of linalool on the PN responses (Fig. 6).

Fig. 6.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Fig. 6.

Predicted responses of the postsynaptic projection neurons (PNs). The responses of the PNs receiving direct inputs from ab2A ORNs were estimated using the formula derived by Olsen et al. (43) using parameters for PNs in the DM4 glomerulus. ORN responses were average responses during the 500-ms odor stimulus. Dose–response relationships of two ab2A excitatory odorants are shown: methyl butyrate (A) and methyl acetate (B) (n = 9). Dose–response relationships were fitted with the Hill equation. Hill coefficients are indicated in parentheses.

A particularly dramatic effect is predicted at low odorant concentrations. The greater effect at lower concentrations may be explained by the nonlinear amplification observed at the ORN–PN synapse (42, 44): because weak ORN inputs are amplified more than strong ORN inputs in the PNs, inhibition of ORNs at lower concentrations caused greater inhibition in the corresponding range in the PN dose–response curve. Consequently, the PN dose–response curve appears steeper, characteristic of a curve with a greater Hill coefficient (PN panels). Thus, our analysis supports the notion that inhibition of ORNs is translated into inhibition of PNs; in fact, at low concentrations the inhibitory effects are likely to be even more pronounced in PNs than in ORNs.

Conclusions.

We have found that ORNs are capable of sophisticated information processing. The emergent quality of an odor mixture has long been believed to arise from integration of olfactory information in the CNS. We have found that olfactory information is integrated in individual ORNs in the form of response dynamics as well as response magnitude.

We have analyzed how individual ORNs integrate olfactory information. We found that, when an inhibitory odorant is delivered in the presence of a background excitatory odorant, the duration and the magnitude of the inhibitory response reflect the concentrations of the inhibitory and excitatory odorants. ORNs also respond with distinctive temporal dynamics to premixed combinations of inhibitory and excitatory odorants. We show that the temporal dynamics of odor stimuli differ markedly, presumably due to differences in their physicochemical properties. The dynamics of the response to an odor mixture are likely to be shaped by the dynamics of its component stimuli. Taken together, our findings provide evidence that ORNs encode odor mixtures not only by altering their response magnitudes, but also by varying their response dynamics.

Do the temporal dynamics of ORNs contribute to odor identification and discrimination? A recent study showed that temporal heterogeneity in ORN firing patterns is crucial for generating spatiotemporal neural codes for odors in the locust antennal lobe (45), which represents odor identity in a concentration-independent manner (46). In the antennal lobe of moths, precise temporal activation patterns unique to certain odor blends provide a means for triggering innate behaviors (47). Moreover, mutant flies with only one functional type of Or-expressing ORN can be trained to discriminate different odorants, suggesting that odors could be discriminated by virtue of the temporal dynamics that they elicited in the ORN (48). Finally, we note that an emerging body of evidence supports a role for temporal dynamics in the coding of individual odorants in vertebrates (49–51), Thus, ORN response dynamics distinctive for an odor mixture seem likely to make a major contribution to the emergent quality of the mixture, binding features of the individual constituents into a unifying odor percept.

In perfumery, odorants of high, moderate, and lower volatility are commonly blended together to create the top, middle, and base notes, respectively, of a perfume. The composition and ratio of these notes endow the perfume with a signature scent (52). Human psychophysical studies have shown that odorants in a mixture are processed and perceived in series (53). As shown by our PID measurements (Fig. 4), inhibitory and excitatory odorants may also arrive in series and may interact so as to give rise to unique, mixture-specific ORN response dynamics. In this manner, inhibitory odorants may influence not only the perception of odor intensity, but also the perception of odor identity. Inhibitory stimuli could influence the perception of stimulus identity in other sensory modalities as well.

Finally, we note that, for many years, analysis of odors that drive animal and human behavior has focused primarily on their excitatory constituents. Our results invite a greater emphasis on the identification and analysis of their inhibitory constituents.

Materials and Methods

Drosophila Stocks.

All experiments were performed on adult female flies, 5–7 d after eclosion, except for “empty neuron” recordings, in which both male and female flies were used. Flies were reared at 25 °C in an incubator with a 12-h light–dark cycle. Wild-type flies are Canton-S. “Empty neuron” recordings were from flies of genotype w; Δhalo/Δhalo; Or22a-GAL4/UAS-Or. The ab3A mutant flies and Or22a-GAL4 and UAS-Or transgenic lines were described previously (23, 32, 34, 40). Table S1 lists genotypes of the flies used in all experiments.

Electrophysiology and Data Analysis.

Extracellular single-unit recordings were performed essentially as described previously (23). All data were acquired using Axoscope 10.2 (Molecular Devices), and ORN spikes were detected using routines in Igor Pro-6.01 (Wavemetrics) (44). Peri-stimulus time histograms (PSTHs) were obtained by averaging spike activities in 50-ms bins (44) and smoothed using a binomial algorithm (Igor Pro-6.01, Wavemetrics). Response width was calculated on the basis of average PSTHs, as the time interval between half-maximal responses in the case of excitation (Max50, excitation) or half-minimal responses in the case of inhibition (Min50, inhibition). Two additional PSTH traces were generated as PSTH + SEM and PSTH − SEM, respectively. The time intervals of Max50 or Min50 for the two additional PSTH traces determined the ranges for error bars of the response widths.

Odor Stimuli.

For short odor pulses, odor stimuli (50 μL) were delivered from a Pasteur pipette via a pulse of air (200 mL/min) into the main air stream (2,000 mL/min) as described previously (23, 34), except for binary odor mixture experiments (Figs. 3 and 5) in which 100 μL of a premixed odor dilution was added to two filter discs, placed adjacently along the long axis of the Pasteur pipette (54). We observed no difference in the temporal dynamics of odor stimuli using one or two filter discs in our PID measurements. Background odor stimuli were delivered from a 125-mL flask containing 3 mL of odor dilutions directly downstream of the main airflow (2,000 mL/min).

PID Measurements.

A fast response mini-PID (200a, Aurora Scientific) was positioned between the head of a fly and the mouth of the odor delivery tube as described previously (55, 56). Fig. S7 and SI Text show the PID setup in relation to the odor delivery system. The rising phase of the PID response was fitted with an exponential curve y(t) = A*exp(−t/τon). Parameters of the fits are listed in Table S2.

Acknowledgments

We thank W. van der Goes van Naters for help with the electrophysiology setup, and V. Bhandawat and M. T. H. Do for assistance with data analyses. We thank K. Menuz, J. Reisert, C. A. Yao, L. Zwiebel, and G. M. Thomas for comments on the manuscript. We also thank B. J. Dickson for the Or85b-GAL4 transgenic line. C.M. and T.E. acknowledge support from The Raymond and Beverly Sackler Institute for Biological, Physical and Engineering Sciences and from an Alfred P. Sloan Foundation Fellowship. This work was funded by National Institutes of Health grants to J.R.C. and by a grant from the Foundation for the National Institutes of Health through the Grand Challenges in Global Health Initiative (GCGH no. 121).

Footnotes

  • ↵1To whom correspondence may be addressed. E-mail: john.carlson{at}yale.edu.
  • Author contributions: C.-Y.S. designed research; C.-Y.S. and C.M. performed research; C.-Y.S. analyzed data; and C.-Y.S., C.M., T.E., and J.R.C. wrote the paper.

  • The authors declare no conflict of interest.

  • This article is a PNAS Direct Submission.

  • This article contains supporting information online at www.pnas.org/lookup/suppl/doi:10.1073/pnas.1100369108/-/DCSupplemental.

References

  1. ↵
    1. Laing DG,
    2. Francis GW
    (1989) The capacity of humans to identify odors in mixtures. Physiol Behav 46:809–814.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  2. ↵
    1. Deisig N,
    2. Giurfa M,
    3. Lachnit H,
    4. Sandoz JC
    (2006) Neural representation of olfactory mixtures in the honeybee antennal lobe. Eur J Neurosci 24:1161–1174.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  3. ↵
    1. Silbering AF,
    2. Galizia CG
    (2007) Processing of odor mixtures in the Drosophila antennal lobe reveals both global inhibition and glomerulus-specific interactions. J Neurosci 27:11966–11977.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  4. ↵
    1. Broome BM,
    2. Jayaraman V,
    3. Laurent G
    (2006) Encoding and decoding of overlapping odor sequences. Neuron 51:467–482.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  5. ↵
    1. Tabor R,
    2. Yaksi E,
    3. Weislogel JM,
    4. Friedrich RW
    (2004) Processing of odor mixtures in the zebrafish olfactory bulb. J Neurosci 24:6611–6620.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  6. ↵
    1. Johnson BA,
    2. Ong J,
    3. Leon M
    (2010) Glomerular activity patterns evoked by natural odor objects in the rat olfactory bulb are related to patterns evoked by major odorant components. J Comp Neurol 518:1542–1555.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  7. ↵
    1. Duchamp-Viret P,
    2. Duchamp A,
    3. Chaput MA
    (2003) Single olfactory sensory neurons simultaneously integrate the components of an odour mixture. Eur J Neurosci 18:2690–2696.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  8. ↵
    1. Rospars JP,
    2. Lansky P,
    3. Chaput M,
    4. Duchamp-Viret P
    (2008) Competitive and noncompetitive odorant interactions in the early neural coding of odorant mixtures. J Neurosci 28:2659–2666.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  9. ↵
    1. Den Otter CJ
    (1977) Single sensillum responses in the male moth Adoxophyes orana (F.v.R.) to female sex pheromone components and their geometrical isomers. J Comp Physiol A Neuroethol Sens Neural Behav Physiol 121:205–222.
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  10. ↵
    1. van der Pers JNC,
    2. Thomas G,
    3. Den Otter CJ
    (1980) Interactions between plant odours and pheromone reception in small ermine moths (Lepidoptera: Yponomeutidae) Chem Senses 5:367–371.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  11. ↵
    1. Ochieng SA,
    2. Park KC,
    3. Baker TC
    (2002) Host plant volatiles synergize responses of sex pheromone-specific olfactory receptor neurons in male Helicoverpa zea. J Comp Physiol A Neuroethol Sens Neural Behav Physiol 188:325–333.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  12. ↵
    1. Lee SG,
    2. Baker TC
    (2008) Incomplete electrical isolation of sex-pheromone responsive olfactory receptor neurons from neighboring sensilla. J Insect Physiol 54:663–671.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  13. ↵
    1. Kaissling KE,
    2. Meng LZ,
    3. Bestmann HJ
    (1989) Responses of bombykol receptor cells to (Z,E)-4,6-hexadecadiene and linalool. J Comp Physiol A Neuroethol Sens Neural Behav Physiol 165:147–154.
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  14. ↵
    1. Kramer E
    (1992) Attractivity of pheromone surpassed by time-patterned application of two nonpheromone compounds. J Insect Behav 5:83–97.
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  15. ↵
    1. Nikonov AA,
    2. Leal WS
    (2002) Peripheral coding of sex pheromone and a behavioral antagonist in the Japanese beetle, Popillia japonica. J Chem Ecol 28:1075–1089.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  16. ↵
    1. Xu H,
    2. et al.
    (2004) Different functional roles of T1R subunits in the heteromeric taste receptors. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 101:14258–14263.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  17. ↵
    1. Galindo-Cuspinera V,
    2. Winnig M,
    3. Bufe B,
    4. Meyerhof W,
    5. Breslin PA
    (2006) A TAS1R receptor-based explanation of sweet ‘water-taste’ Nature 441:354–357.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  18. ↵
    1. Araneda RC,
    2. Kini AD,
    3. Firestein S
    (2000) The molecular receptive range of an odorant receptor. Nat Neurosci 3:1248–1255.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  19. ↵
    1. Spehr M,
    2. et al.
    (2003) Identification of a testicular odorant receptor mediating human sperm chemotaxis. Science 299:2054–2058.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  20. ↵
    1. Oka Y,
    2. Omura M,
    3. Kataoka H,
    4. Touhara K
    (2004) Olfactory receptor antagonism between odorants. EMBO J 23:120–126.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  21. ↵
    1. Oka Y,
    2. Nakamura A,
    3. Watanabe H,
    4. Touhara K
    (2004) An odorant derivative as an antagonist for an olfactory receptor. Chem Senses 29:815–822.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  22. ↵
    1. de Bruyne M,
    2. Foster K,
    3. Carlson JR
    (2001) Odor coding in the Drosophila antenna. Neuron 30:537–552.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  23. ↵
    1. Hallem EA,
    2. Ho MG,
    3. Carlson JR
    (2004) The molecular basis of odor coding in the Drosophila antenna. Cell 117:965–979.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  24. ↵
    1. Turner SL,
    2. Ray A
    (2009) Modification of CO(2) avoidance behaviour in Drosophila by inhibitory odorants. Nature 461:477–481.
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  25. ↵
    1. Michel WC,
    2. McClintock TS,
    3. Ache BW
    (1991) Inhibition of lobster olfactory receptor cells by an odor-activated potassium conductance. J Neurophysiol 65:446–453.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  26. ↵
    1. Shields VD,
    2. Hildebrand JG
    (2000-2001) Responses of a population of antennal olfactory receptor cells in the female moth Manduca sexta to plant-associated volatile organic compounds. J Comp Physiol A Neuroethol Sens Neural Behav Physiol 186:1135–1151.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  27. ↵
    1. Reisert J,
    2. Restrepo D
    (2009) Molecular tuning of odorant receptors and its implication for odor signal processing. Chem Senses 34:535–545.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  28. ↵
    1. Brodin M,
    2. Laska M,
    3. Olsson MJ
    (2009) Odor interaction between Bourgeonal and its antagonist undecanal. Chem Senses 34:625–630.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  29. ↵
    1. Couto A,
    2. Alenius M,
    3. Dickson BJ
    (2005) Molecular, anatomical, and functional organization of the Drosophila olfactory system. Curr Biol 15:1535–1547.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  30. ↵
    1. Fishilevich E,
    2. Vosshall LB
    (2005) Genetic and functional subdivision of the Drosophila antennal lobe. Curr Biol 15:1548–1553.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  31. ↵
    1. Hallem EA,
    2. Carlson JR
    (2006) Coding of odors by a receptor repertoire. Cell 125:143–160.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  32. ↵
    1. Dobritsa AA,
    2. van der Goes van Naters W,
    3. Warr CG,
    4. Steinbrecht RA,
    5. Carlson JR
    (2003) Integrating the molecular and cellular basis of odor coding in the Drosophila antenna. Neuron 37:827–841.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  33. ↵
    1. Lu T,
    2. et al.
    (2007) Odor coding in the maxillary palp of the malaria vector mosquito Anopheles gambiae. Curr Biol 17:1533–1544.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  34. ↵
    1. Carey AF,
    2. Wang G,
    3. Su CY,
    4. Zwiebel LJ,
    5. Carlson JR
    (2010) Odorant reception in the malaria mosquito Anopheles gambiae. Nature 464:66–71.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  35. ↵
    1. Wang G,
    2. Carey AF,
    3. Carlson JR,
    4. Zwiebel LJ
    (2010) Molecular basis of odor coding in the malaria vector mosquito Anopheles gambiae. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 107:4418–4423.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  36. ↵
    1. Schuckel J,
    2. Torkkeli PH,
    3. French AS
    (2009) Two interacting olfactory transduction mechanisms have linked polarities and dynamics in Drosophila melanogaster antennal basiconic sensilla neurons. J Neurophysiol 102:214–223.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  37. ↵
    1. Ditzen M,
    2. Pellegrino M,
    3. Vosshall LB
    (2008) Insect odorant receptors are molecular targets of the insect repellent DEET. Science 319:1838–1842.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  38. ↵
    1. Syed Z,
    2. Leal WS
    (2008) Mosquitoes smell and avoid the insect repellent DEET. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 105:13598–13603.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  39. ↵
    1. Vickers NJ,
    2. Christensen TA,
    3. Baker TC,
    4. Hildebrand JG
    (2001) Odour-plume dynamics influence the brain's olfactory code. Nature 410:466–470.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  40. ↵
    1. Hallem EA,
    2. Nicole Fox A,
    3. Zwiebel LJ,
    4. Carlson JR
    (2004) Olfaction: Mosquito receptor for human-sweat odorant. Nature 427:212–213.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  41. ↵
    1. Noe SM,
    2. Ciccioli P,
    3. Brancaleoni E,
    4. Loreto F,
    5. Niinemets U
    (2006) Emissions of monoterpenes linalool and ocimene respond differently to environmental changes due to differences in physico-chemical characteristics. Atmos Environ 40:4649–4662.
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  42. ↵
    1. Olsen SR,
    2. Bhandawat V,
    3. Wilson RI
    (2007) Excitatory interactions between olfactory processing channels in the Drosophila antennal lobe. Neuron 54:89–103.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  43. ↵
    1. Olsen SR,
    2. Bhandawat V,
    3. Wilson RI
    (2010) Divisive normalization in olfactory population codes. Neuron 66:287–299.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  44. ↵
    1. Bhandawat V,
    2. Olsen SR,
    3. Gouwens NW,
    4. Schlief ML,
    5. Wilson RI
    (2007) Sensory processing in the Drosophila antennal lobe increases reliability and separability of ensemble odor representations. Nat Neurosci 10:1474–1482.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  45. ↵
    1. Raman B,
    2. Joseph J,
    3. Tang J,
    4. Stopfer M
    (2010) Temporally diverse firing patterns in olfactory receptor neurons underlie spatiotemporal neural codes for odors. J Neurosci 30:1994–2006.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  46. ↵
    1. Stopfer M,
    2. Jayaraman V,
    3. Laurent G
    (2003) Intensity versus identity coding in an olfactory system. Neuron 39:991–1004.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  47. ↵
    1. Riffell JA,
    2. Lei H,
    3. Christensen TA,
    4. Hildebrand JG
    (2009) Characterization and coding of behaviorally significant odor mixtures. Curr Biol 19:335–340.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  48. ↵
    1. DasGupta S,
    2. Waddell S
    (2008) Learned odor discrimination in Drosophila without combinatorial odor maps in the antennal lobe. Curr Biol 18:1668–1674.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  49. ↵
    1. Spors H,
    2. Wachowiak M,
    3. Cohen LB,
    4. Friedrich RW
    (2006) Temporal dynamics and latency patterns of receptor neuron input to the olfactory bulb. J Neurosci 26:1247–1259.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  50. ↵
    1. Carey RM,
    2. Verhagen JV,
    3. Wesson DW,
    4. Pírez N,
    5. Wachowiak M
    (2009) Temporal structure of receptor neuron input to the olfactory bulb imaged in behaving rats. J Neurophysiol 101:1073–1088.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  51. ↵
    1. Junek S,
    2. Kludt E,
    3. Wolf F,
    4. Schild D
    (2010) Olfactory coding with patterns of response latencies. Neuron 67:872–884.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  52. ↵
    1. Aftel M
    (2001) Essence and Alchemy: A Natural History of Perfume (North Point Press, New York).
  53. ↵
    1. Laing DG,
    2. Eddy A,
    3. Francis GW,
    4. Stephens L
    (1994) Evidence for the temporal processing of odor mixtures in humans. Brain Res 651:317–328.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  54. ↵
    1. de Brito Sanchez MG,
    2. Kaissling KE
    (2005) Inhibitory and excitatory effects of iodobenzene on the antennal benzoic acid receptor cells of the female silk moth Bombyx mori L. Chem Senses 30:435–442.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  55. ↵
    1. Vetter RS,
    2. Sage AE,
    3. Justus KA,
    4. Cardé RT,
    5. Galizia CG
    (2006) Temporal integrity of an airborne odor stimulus is greatly affected by physical aspects of the odor delivery system. Chem Senses 31:359–369.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  56. ↵
    1. Schuckel J,
    2. Meisner S,
    3. Torkkeli PH,
    4. French AS
    (2008) Dynamic properties of Drosophila olfactory electroantennograms. J Comp Physiol A Neuroethol Sens Neural Behav Physiol 194:483–489.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
PreviousNext
Back to top
Article Alerts
Email Article

Thank you for your interest in spreading the word on PNAS.

NOTE: We only request your email address so that the person you are recommending the page to knows that you wanted them to see it, and that it is not junk mail. We do not capture any email address.

Enter multiple addresses on separate lines or separate them with commas.
Temporal coding of odor mixtures in an olfactory receptor neuron
(Your Name) has sent you a message from PNAS
(Your Name) thought you would like to see the PNAS web site.
CAPTCHA
This question is for testing whether or not you are a human visitor and to prevent automated spam submissions.
Citation Tools
Temporal coding of odor mixtures in an olfactory receptor neuron
Chih-Ying Su, Carlotta Martelli, Thierry Emonet, John R. Carlson
Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences Mar 2011, 108 (12) 5075-5080; DOI: 10.1073/pnas.1100369108

Citation Manager Formats

  • BibTeX
  • Bookends
  • EasyBib
  • EndNote (tagged)
  • EndNote 8 (xml)
  • Medlars
  • Mendeley
  • Papers
  • RefWorks Tagged
  • Ref Manager
  • RIS
  • Zotero
Request Permissions
Share
Temporal coding of odor mixtures in an olfactory receptor neuron
Chih-Ying Su, Carlotta Martelli, Thierry Emonet, John R. Carlson
Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences Mar 2011, 108 (12) 5075-5080; DOI: 10.1073/pnas.1100369108
Digg logo Reddit logo Twitter logo Facebook logo Google logo Mendeley logo
  • Tweet Widget
  • Facebook Like
  • Mendeley logo Mendeley
Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences: 108 (12)
Table of Contents

Submit

Sign up for Article Alerts

Article Classifications

  • Biological Sciences
  • Neuroscience

Jump to section

  • Article
    • Abstract
    • Results and Discussion
    • Materials and Methods
    • Acknowledgments
    • Footnotes
    • References
  • Figures & SI
  • Info & Metrics
  • PDF

You May Also be Interested in

Surgeons hands during surgery
Inner Workings: Advances in infectious disease treatment promise to expand the pool of donor organs
Despite myriad challenges, clinicians see room for progress.
Image credit: Shutterstock/David Tadevosian.
Setting sun over a sun-baked dirt landscape
Core Concept: Popular integrated assessment climate policy models have key caveats
Better explicating the strengths and shortcomings of these models will help refine projections and improve transparency in the years ahead.
Image credit: Witsawat.S.
Double helix
Journal Club: Noncoding DNA shown to underlie function, cause limb malformations
Using CRISPR, researchers showed that a region some used to label “junk DNA” has a major role in a rare genetic disorder.
Image credit: Nathan Devery.
Steamboat Geyser eruption.
Eruption of Steamboat Geyser
Mara Reed and Michael Manga explore why Yellowstone's Steamboat Geyser resumed erupting in 2018.
Listen
Past PodcastsSubscribe
Multi-color molecular model
Enzymatic breakdown of PET plastic
A study demonstrates how two enzymes—MHETase and PETase—work synergistically to depolymerize the plastic pollutant PET.
Image credit: Aaron McGeehan (artist).

Similar Articles

Site Logo
Powered by HighWire
  • Submit Manuscript
  • Twitter
  • Facebook
  • RSS Feeds
  • Email Alerts

Articles

  • Current Issue
  • Special Feature Articles – Most Recent
  • List of Issues

PNAS Portals

  • Anthropology
  • Chemistry
  • Classics
  • Front Matter
  • Physics
  • Sustainability Science
  • Teaching Resources

Information

  • Authors
  • Editorial Board
  • Reviewers
  • Librarians
  • Press
  • Site Map
  • PNAS Updates

Feedback    Privacy/Legal

Copyright © 2021 National Academy of Sciences. Online ISSN 1091-6490