Skip to main content

Main menu

  • Home
  • Articles
    • Current
    • Special Feature Articles - Most Recent
    • Special Features
    • Colloquia
    • Collected Articles
    • PNAS Classics
    • List of Issues
  • Front Matter
    • Front Matter Portal
    • Journal Club
  • News
    • For the Press
    • This Week In PNAS
    • PNAS in the News
  • Podcasts
  • Authors
    • Information for Authors
    • Editorial and Journal Policies
    • Submission Procedures
    • Fees and Licenses
  • Submit
  • Submit
  • About
    • Editorial Board
    • PNAS Staff
    • FAQ
    • Accessibility Statement
    • Rights and Permissions
    • Site Map
  • Contact
  • Journal Club
  • Subscribe
    • Subscription Rates
    • Subscriptions FAQ
    • Open Access
    • Recommend PNAS to Your Librarian

User menu

  • Log in
  • My Cart

Search

  • Advanced search
Home
Home
  • Log in
  • My Cart

Advanced Search

  • Home
  • Articles
    • Current
    • Special Feature Articles - Most Recent
    • Special Features
    • Colloquia
    • Collected Articles
    • PNAS Classics
    • List of Issues
  • Front Matter
    • Front Matter Portal
    • Journal Club
  • News
    • For the Press
    • This Week In PNAS
    • PNAS in the News
  • Podcasts
  • Authors
    • Information for Authors
    • Editorial and Journal Policies
    • Submission Procedures
    • Fees and Licenses
  • Submit
Research Article

Testosterone administration impairs cognitive empathy in women depending on second-to-fourth digit ratio

Jack van Honk, Dennis J. Schutter, Peter A. Bos, Anne-Wil Kruijt, Eef G. Lentjes, and Simon Baron-Cohen
  1. aDepartment of Experimental Psychology, Utrecht University, 3584 CS, Utrecht, The Netherlands;
  2. bDepartment of Psychiatry, University of Cape Town, Cape Town 7925, South Africa;
  3. cDepartment of Clinical Psychology, Leiden University, 2300 RC, Leiden, The Netherlands;
  4. dDepartment of Clinical Chemistry, Utrecht University, 3584 CX, Utrecht, The Netherlands; and
  5. eAutism Research Centre, Department of Psychiatry, Cambridge University, Cambridge CB2 8AH, United Kingdom

See allHide authors and affiliations

PNAS February 22, 2011 108 (8) 3448-3452; https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1011891108
Jack van Honk
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
  • For correspondence: j.vanhonk@uu.nl
Dennis J. Schutter
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Peter A. Bos
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Anne-Wil Kruijt
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Eef G. Lentjes
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Simon Baron-Cohen
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
  1. Edited by Bruce S. McEwen, The Rockefeller University, New York, NY, and approved January 3, 2011 (received for review August 10, 2010)

  2. ↵2D.J.S. and P.A.B. contributed equally to this work.

  • Article
  • Figures & SI
  • Info & Metrics
  • PDF
Loading

Abstract

During social interactions we automatically infer motives, intentions, and feelings from bodily cues of others, especially from the eye region of their faces. This cognitive empathic ability is one of the most important components of social intelligence, and is essential for effective social interaction. Females on average outperform males in this cognitive empathy, and the male sex hormone testosterone is thought to be involved. Testosterone may not only down-regulate social intelligence organizationally, by affecting fetal brain development, but also activationally, by its current effects on the brain. Here, we show that administration of testosterone in 16 young women led to a significant impairment in their cognitive empathy, and that this effect is powerfully predicted by a proxy of fetal testosterone: the right-hand second digit-to-fourth digit ratio. Our data thus not only demonstrate down-regulatory effects of current testosterone on cognitive empathy, but also suggest these are preprogrammed by the very same hormone prenatally. These findings have importance for our understanding of the psychobiology of human social intelligence.

  • prenatal priming
  • steroid hormones
  • mind reading

In human social environments, the ability to make sense of and predict other people's behavior is crucial for physical and social survival (1). To meet this adaptive challenge, humans have a set of evolved cognitive-empathetic mechanisms, enabling them to accurately infer motives, intentions, thoughts, and emotions of others, largely from subtle bodily cues (2–4). Cognitive empathy is central to social intelligence and occurs automatically and mostly unconsciously (4). A major source of information providing cues for cognitive empathy is the eye region of the face, which contains subtle facial expression. The ability to “read the mind from the eyes” is sexually dimorphic, with females on average typically outperforming males (4–6). The androgen (sex steroid) hormone testosterone is thought to be involved, as testosterone represents the biggest hormonal difference between the sexes and affects sociality (7, 8). However, testosterone's action in the brain is both organizational and activational: first, the hormone preprograms the brain during early development, and, in later life, it selectively modifies brain processing to facilitate or inhibit behaviors depending on social context (9). In humans, the fetal period of prenatal development is considered critical for testosterone's effects on brain organization (between weeks 12 and 19 of gestation), whereas the hormone's activational effects come into prominence in adolescence and adulthood (9, 10).

Interestingly, the androgen theory of autism proposes that fetal programming of the brain by testosterone negatively affects social intelligence (11). Both cognitive empathy deficits typically seen in autism, and the male-bias of autism, are indirect evidence consistent with the theory. Moreover, recent studies in which fetal testosterone was sampled from the amniotic fluid of pregnant women provide for more direct evidence: in young typically developing children, fetal testosterone is inversely correlated with eye contact at 12 mo (12), social cognition at age 4 y (13), and social intelligence including reading the mind from the eyes at age 6 to 8 y (14).

Fetal testosterone is associated with a fixed somatic marker that can be indexed after birth: the length ratio of the right hand's second (i.e., index) to fourth (i.e., ring) finger (2D:4D ratio). Males on average have a significantly lower 2D:4D ratio on their right hand and fetal testosterone is thought to underlie this sex difference, including its variability within the sexes (15, 16). The reliability of 2D:4D ratio as a marker of fetal testosterone is substantiated by a large amount of correlational evidence in animals and humans (15–17). Moreover, meta-analytic data show that 2D:4D ratio is unaffected by later testosterone fluctuations or circulating levels of testosterone in adulthood. The ratio is therefore considered a useful, noninvasive marker of fetal testosterone (16). A recent hormone manipulation demonstrated the validity of the marker in animals: experimental testosterone elevation in pregnant rats lowered the 2D:4D ratio of the right paw of their offspring measured in adulthood (18). Further strong evidence in humans comes from a study showing that androgen receptor (AR) polymorphisms (i.e., increasing CAG repeats in exon 1), which produce less effective AR protein, predict less masculine 2D:4D ratios (19). Furthermore, higher (i.e., more feminized) 2D:4D ratios are observed in women with genetic mutations that disrupt AR function as seen in androgen insensitivity syndrome (20). In light of the interrelations between AR polymorphisms and digit ratios, Breedlove recently suggested that, although 2D:4D ratio is typically discussed in terms of its relationship to prenatal levels of androgens, digit ratio more accurately reflects total androgen stimulation in terms of prenatal androgen levels and androgen sensitivity (17). Finally, with respect to the androgen theory of autism, it is important to note that lower (i.e., more masculinized) 2D:4D ratios have been observed in children with autism or Asperger syndrome, and also in their first-degree relatives (21).

There is also strong evidence for the activational effects of testosterone on human social and emotional behavior. Placebo-controlled testosterone administration studies in typical young adult women have shown reductions in mimicry and conscious recognition of emotional facial expressions (22, 23). However, to our knowledge, there is no such direct evidence for down-regulatory effects of testosterone administration on social intelligence or cognitive empathy in particular. Moreover, fetal testosterone might critically mediate in the activational effects of testosterone on human social behavior. In nonhuman animals, it has been shown that early organizational effects of testosterone strongly facilitate the activational effects of the very same hormone in adulthood (9, 10). Testosterone administration in adult humans might thus impair social intelligence, but especially in those most primed by the same hormone prenatally. Accordingly, we conducted an experiment to test whether testosterone administration impairs cognitive empathy, and whether predicted testosterone-induced impairments in cognitive empathy varied according to the 2D:4D ratio marker of fetal testosterone.

Results

To investigate effects of testosterone on cognitive empathy, we temporarily elevated the levels of testosterone in young adult females by using a validated sublingual 0.5-mg single-dose testosterone administration technique. We used a crossover, double-blind, placebo-controlled, within-subjects design with a computerized adaptation of the validated reading the mind in the eyes task (RMET; http://www.autismresearchcentre.com/tests/eyes_test_adult.asp) as the behavioral measure of social intelligence (5, 6, 24). To enable measurement of 2D:4D ratio, subjects’ right hands were scanned and the 2D:4D ratio was computed from these scans using Adobe Photoshop as a measurement-precision tool. This was carried out by two experienced raters, who used the Millet and de Witte procedure (25), and who remained blind to the experiment. Statistical analyses are based on nonparametric tests (Wilcoxon rank tests and Spearman correlations), but additional parametric statistics are applied for insight in explained variances. The 2D:4D ratios measured by the two raters were highly correlated [Spearman ρ(14) = 0.96; P < 0.001].

First, we investigated potential activational effects of testosterone on cognitive empathy. As can be seen from Fig. 1A, compared with placebo, testosterone administration significantly impaired the ability to read the mind from the eyes [Wilcoxon repeated-measures nonparametric test, Z(1, 16) = −2.24; P = 0.013, one-tailed], with 75% of the subjects showing a decrease in performance on the RMET after testosterone administration. Next, we addressed the relation between fetal testosterone and cognitive empathy, first at baseline by relating 2D:4D ratio to mind-reading performance after placebo: nonparametric Spearman correlations over these variables were not significant [ρ(14) = 0.30; P > 0.25]. However, Spearman correlations showed that the relation between 2D:4D ratio and the impairment in cognitive empathy induced by testosterone administration was highly significant [ρ(14) = 0.85; P < 0.0001].

Fig. 1.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Fig. 1.

(A) Effect of testosterone administration on cognitive empathy in young women: mean and SEM of the percentage correct responses on the RMET after administration of testosterone and placebo (P = 0.013, one-tailed). Testosterone administration impairs the ability to accurately infer motives, intentions, thoughts, and emotions from the eye region of the face of others. (B) Fetal testosterone exposure (inferred from 2D:4D ratio) predicts the effect of testosterone administration on cognitive empathy: scatter plot shows the interaction between the 2D:4D ratio fetal testosterone marker and the effect of testosterone (T) administration on cognitive empathy (P < 0.001). The group effect of testosterone administration on cognitive empathy varies strongly according to individual 2D:4D ratios. (C) Effect of testosterone (T) administration on cognitive empathy in subjects with high and low fetal testosterone exposure (inferred 2D:4D ratio): Mean and SEM of the effect of testosterone administration on cognitive empathy in subjects with relatively low and high 2D:4D ratios, based on median split. Substantial effects of testosterone on cognitive empathy are observed in subjects with high fetal testosterone exposure (P = 0.006, one-tailed), and no effects are seen in subjects with low fetal testosterone exposure (P = 1).

As can be seen from Fig. 1B, applied as a regressor (i.e., parametrically), fetal testosterone exposure (as inferred from 2D:4D ratios) explains more than 50% of the variance in the effect of testosterone administration on cognitive empathy. To qualify this effect, we applied a median split on the individual 2D:4D ratio measures to create groups of high and low fetal testosterone. Wilcoxon repeated-measures analyses (Fig. 1C) showed no effects of testosterone administration on cognitive empathy whatsoever in subjects with low fetal testosterone exposure [i.e., high 2D:4D ratio; Z(1,8) = 0.00; P = 1]. However, in line with expectations, subjects with high fetal testosterone exposure (i.e., low 2D:4D ratio) showed a strongly significant reduction in cognitive empathy after testosterone administration [Z(1,8) = −2.54; P = 0.006, one-tailed].

In addition, consistent with the down-regulatory effects of testosterone administration on mind reading, there was a significant negative correlation between salivary testosterone and cognitive empathy in the placebo condition [ρ(14) = −0.45; P = 0.041, one-tailed]. Importantly, however, testosterone levels measured from saliva (before administration of testosterone or placebo) did not differ between the experimental conditions, were unrelated to 2D:4D ratio, and did not mediate in our critical effect of testosterone administration on cognitive empathy (all P values > 0.40). Finally, none of these effects were caused by secondary-generated mood changes (all P values > 0.18), and our data were not confounded by expectations or subjective biases of the participants with respect to the effects of testosterone in general, or effects of testosterone on cognitive empathy in particular (all P values > 0.30; Experimental Procedures).

Discussion

We have shown that a single administration of testosterone in female subjects leads to a significant impairment in the ability to infer emotions, intentions, and other mental states from the eye region of the face. Our data provide causal evidence for the hypothesis that testosterone levels negatively influence social intelligence (26, 27). Moreover, the 2D:4D ratio fetal testosterone marker predicted more than 50% of the variance in this effect; i.e., sizable effects of testosterone on cognitive empathy were seen in only subjects who were highly prenatally primed by testosterone, inferred from low 2D:4D ratios. Thus, our data convincingly show effects of testosterone administration on cognitive empathy, and these may depend on fetal testosterone priming.

There was no relation between the fetal testosterone marker and performance on the RMET at baseline, which is consistent with recent correlational data from large groups of adult humans (5, 6), but not with data from children (14). Although correlational studies, which usually include larger subject samples, report associations between the fetal testosterone marker and economic and social behavior in adult humans (5, 28), the chance of finding such effects might theoretically be greater in young children as their brains have not yet been reorganized by the testosterone surges of adolescence (9). Crucially, however, and consistent with rodent research, we show that fetal testosterone comes into prominence when its priming is experimentally activated by testosterone administration in adulthood (10). Indeed, at present, there is also a negative relation between baseline salivary testosterone levels and social intelligence, but, consistent with the animal data (9, 10), it was testosterone's early organizational effect indexed by 2D:4D ratio that predicted the effects of administration of the very same hormone on behavior.

Recently, some researchers have expressed doubt over the sensitivity of 2D:4D ratio as an individual marker for differences in prenatal androgen exposure (20). Certainly there is variance in 2D:4D ratio that cannot be attributed to prenatal testosterone alone, and sex or certain phenotypes cannot be predicted from individual digit ratios (17, 29). However, 2D:4D ratio is useful for predicting human behavior when comparing groups, and has proven to be a valuable marker for individual differences in prenatal androgen exposure in correlational studies (15–17). This is substantiated by the present findings wherein digit ratio explained more than 50% of the individual variance in the effects of testosterone on cognitive empathy. One study into 2D:4D ratio variance examined the relationship between fetal steroid hormone levels measured in amniocentesis fluid and the 2D:4D ratio. The relation between fetal testosterone and 2D:4D ratio only became significant when fetal estradiol was taken into account (i.e., fetal testosterone:estradiol ratio) (30), suggesting fetal estradiol and testosterone interactively contribute to 2D:4D ratio (31). This is interesting, but seems inconsistent with the evidence from rodents showing that testosterone and estradiol are involved in masculinizing the brain; i.e., in rodents, brain masculinization depends on circulating testosterone acting on AR receptors and conversely on testosterone converted by the enzyme aromatase into estradiol acting on estrogen receptors (32). Crucially, however, in primates, including humans, brain masculinization evidently is accomplished primarily via androgens acting directly on ARs. The stimulatory role of estrogen receptors in masculinizing the human brain is negligible, because individuals with complete androgen insensitivity syndrome (i.e., nonfunctional ARs) show feminized behavior (33), whereas masculine behavior can be observed in men with dysfunctional aromatase (34).

In sum, we show that 2D:4D ratio has strong predictive power in estimating effects of testosterone administration on cognitive empathy in humans. This finding is consistent with animal data (9, 10) and establishes that 2D:4D ratio might be a useful marker for differing effects of testosterone administration in humans.

Opposite effects (i.e., improvements in cognitive empathy) have been shown after administration of the “female-type” peptide hormone oxytocin in healthy young males (24). Furthermore, improvements in cognitive empathy after oxytocin administration were recently also observed in young males diagnosed with autism or Asperger syndrome (35), which suggests that oxytocin may hold some potential for intervention in autism spectrum disorders conditions. It seems that, depending on conditions, testosterone works antagonistically on the neuroendocrine system that expresses oxytocin (36, 37). Steroid and peptide hormones are known to act interdependently in the brain, and as hyperfunction of testosterone and hypofunction of oxytocin has been suggested in autism (36, 38), an integrative approach might add to our understanding of this neurodevelopmental condition (26).

In conclusion, we have shown that administration of testosterone in humans leads to significant impairment in the cognitive empathic ability to infer emotions, intentions, feelings, and other mental states from the eye region of the face. Moreover, a proxy of subjects’ fetal testosterone, the right-hand 2D:4D ratio, suggests that prenatal testosterone priming is crucial in this effect. Here we provide evidence in humans that activational effects of testosterone on adult social cognition may depend on early, prenatal organization by the same hormone, testosterone. Further research is necessary to establish whether this finding is specific to social intelligence or can be generalized to other human behaviors in which testosterone plays a role, such as social dominance or sexual motivation (26, 39). Nonetheless, our data provide unique insights into the psychobiology of social intelligence and open up opportunities for further research in human social neuroendocrinology.

Experimental Procedures

Subjects.

The medical ethics committee of the University Medical Centre Utrecht, The Netherlands, approved the protocol of the study. Sixteen typical female volunteers (mean age, 21 y; age range, 20–25 y) participated in this double-blind, crossover, within-subjects study. They received a single dose of 0.5 mg sublingual testosterone in one session and a single dose of placebo in the other session, with a 48-h latency between sessions. Subjects had no (history of) psychiatric disorders or neurological or endocrine abnormalities, did not smoke, and used no medication other than contraceptive agents. We exclusively recruited women because the parameters (quantity and time course) for inducing neurophysiological effects after a single sublingual administration of 0.5 mg of testosterone are known in women but not in men. We controlled for influences of hormonal change related to menstrual cycle by including only women who used single-phase contraceptives, and testing them during the 3-wk period they were on these contraceptives and not during menstruation (40). In this 3-wk contraceptive period, menstrual cycle influences are virtually absent. This method is vital in our repeated design, because emotion recognition is highly sensitive to variations in the menstrual cycle (41, 42). There is no evidence for single-phase contraceptives influencing the recognition of emotion from faces or mind reading specifically. Moreover, any effects of the contraceptives would be equal during the placebo or testosterone condition.

Drug Administration Procedure.

The drug samples consisted of 0.5 mg of testosterone, 5 mg of cyclodextrin (carrier), 5 mg of ethanol, and 5 mL of water. Testosterone was omitted from the placebo samples, and testosterone and placebo were administered sublingually. Previous experimental research established the time course of changes in blood levels of testosterone and physiological responsiveness in typical young women after a single sublingual administration of 0.5 mg of testosterone. A 10-fold increase in total testosterone was observed 15 min after intake, with testosterone levels returning to baseline within 1.5 h (39). It was also shown that this single administration of testosterone significantly elevates vaginal pulse amplitude in healthy young women after approximately 4 h (43). Thus, physiological effects after single sublingual administrations of 0.5 mg testosterone peak 2.5 h after the testosterone level in the blood has returned to baseline. Note that vaginal pulse amplitude is a centrally driven response evoked by erotic material, and the only physiological measure known to possess a nonhabitual nature, thus allowing multiple measurements throughout the day (39, 44). There is no method available to assess the time course of effects of testosterone administration in human males, whereas in females the present time-course method may have unique applicability in the treatment of sexual dysfunction (44, 45). More important, the reliability and generalizability of behavioral effects after a 4-h delay has been successfully established in more than 20 studies that addressed sexual, social, and emotional behaviors in young typical women (e.g., refs. 22, 39, 44, 46–48). Therefore, in the present protocol, a 4-h delay between testosterone administration and measurement of mood and mind reading was again used.

Experimental Paradigm.

We used a computerized adaptation of the validated RMET (http://www.autismresearchcentre.com/tests/eyes_test_adult.asp) as the behavioral measure of social intelligence (5, 6, 24). The RMET measures subtle variations in the ability to infer other people's mental states from the eye region of the face. The RMET is presented on a computer screen as 36 pictures of the eye region from different faces and a forced choice is required from four alternatives, each of which is a word that describes a possible feeling or thought this person might have. These words were presented in both the original English and in Dutch to keep it as close as possible to the original RMET. Dutch students in general use English as a second language because much of the teaching is in English. The RMET has no time constraints and the explanatory booklets that accompany the RMET were also available to the subjects in English and Dutch.

Digit Ratio Measurement.

Digit ratio was measured from a scan of the right hand of the subjects. The use of scanned images is a valid method to measure finger lengths. When conducting this scan, we ensured that details of major creases could be seen. Lengths of the second and fourth digits were measured from the ventral proximal crease of the digit to the fingertip by using Adobe Photoshop. When there was a band of creases at the base of the digit, measurement was taken from the most proximal crease.

Salivary Testosterone Measurement.

Saliva samples were collected just before administration of testosterone and placebo, and testosterone was measured in saliva. This was done after diethyl ether extraction with a competitive radioimmunoassay using a polyclonal antitestosterone antibody (AZG 3290; gift from J. H. Pratt, Department of Medicine, Indiana University School of Medicine, Indianapolis, IN). [1,2,6,7-3H]–testosterone (TRK402; Amersham) was used as a tracer following chromatographic verification of its purity. Interassay variation ranged from 9% to 16% at 20 to 400 pmol/L (n = 25).

Mood Measurement.

The shortened version of the Profile of Mood States (49) was used to index possible effects of testosterone on anger, anxiety, fatigue, vigor, and depression. Wilcoxon rank tests detected no significant differences in mood between the testosterone and placebo conditions (all P > 0.18), replicating earlier studies that used the same methodology (22, 23, 27, 46–48). Given that testosterone had no effects on mood, the observed effects of testosterone on cognitive empathy cannot be attributed to secondary mood- generated response biases.

Control of Belief Effects and Subjective Biases.

Recent research has established that beliefs about the effects of the hormone testosterone can influence the performance of human subjects in experimental conditions in which these subjects think they have been administered the hormone (46). After the two sessions of the experiment, subjects were asked to indicate (by forced choice) in which sessions they think they received testosterone and placebo. Performance was at chance level (binomial P = 0.80), confirming that subjects were unaware of condition. Furthermore, we asked them about the possible influences of testosterone on the RMET. Only one subject guessed the hypothesis correctly, but was wrong about her testosterone and placebo conditions. The other subjects had no idea about the rationale of the experiment or thought it involved perceptions of anger or aggression.

Acknowledgments

We thank Aimee Capello for her assistance in this research. This work was supported by research grants from Utrecht University (High-Potential) (to J.v.H.); Hope for Depression Research Foundation Grant RGA 9-015 (to J.v.H.); Netherlands Society of Scientific Research Innovational Research Grant 452-07-012 (to D.J.S.); Netherlands Society of Scientific Research Brain and Cognition Grant 056-24-010 (to J.v.H.); the United Kingdom Medical Research Council (S.B.-C.); and the Nancy Lurie Marks Family Foundation (S.B.-C.).

Footnotes

  • 1To whom correspondence should be addressed. E-mail: j.vanhonk{at}uu.nl.
  • Author contributions: J.v.H. and S.B.-C. designed research; P.A.B. and A.-W.K. performed research; E.G.L. contributed new reagents/analytic tools; J.v.H., D.J.S., and A.-W.K. analyzed data; and J.v.H., D.J.S., P.A.B., E.G.L., and S.B.-C. wrote the paper.

  • The authors declare no conflict of interest.

  • This article is a PNAS Direct Submission.

References

  1. ↵
    1. Frith CD,
    2. Frith U
    (1999) Interacting minds—a biological basis. Science 286:1692–1695.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  2. ↵
    1. Hill EL,
    2. Frith U
    (2003) Understanding autism: Insights from mind and brain. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci 358:281–289.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  3. ↵
    1. Realo A,
    2. et al.
    (2003) Mind-reading ability: Beliefs and performance. J Res Personality 37:420–445.
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  4. ↵
    1. Baron-Cohen S
    (2003) The Essential Difference (Basic Books, New York).
  5. ↵
    1. Sapienza P,
    2. Zingales L,
    3. Maestripieri D
    (2009) Gender differences in financial risk aversion and career choices are affected by testosterone. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 106:15268–15273.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  6. ↵
    1. Voracek M,
    2. Dressler SG
    (2006) Lack of correlation between digit ratio (2D: 4D) and Baron-Cohen's “Reading the Mind in the Eyes” test, empathy, systemising, and autism-spectrum quotients in a general population sample. Person Individ Differ 41:1481–1491.
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  7. ↵
    1. Pennebaker JW,
    2. Groom CJ,
    3. Loew D,
    4. Dabbs JM
    (2004) Testosterone as a social inhibitor: two case studies of the effect of testosterone treatment on language. J Abnorm Psychol 113:172–175.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  8. ↵
    1. Baron-Cohen S,
    2. Knickmeyer RC,
    3. Belmonte MK
    (2005) Sex differences in the brain: implications for explaining autism. Science 310:819–823.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  9. ↵
    1. Sisk CL,
    2. Zehr JL
    (2005) Pubertal hormones organize the adolescent brain and behavior. Front Neuroendocrinol 26:163–174.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  10. ↵
    1. deCantazaro DA
    (1998) Motivation and Emotion (Prentice Hall, New Jersey).
  11. ↵
    1. Baron-Cohen S
    (2002) The extreme male brain theory of autism. Trends Cogn Sci 6:248–254.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  12. ↵
    1. Lutchmaya S,
    2. Baron-Cohen S,
    3. Raggatt P
    (2002) Foetal testosterone and eye contact in 12-month-old human infants. Infant Behav Dev 25:327–335.
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  13. ↵
    1. Knickmeyer R,
    2. Baron-Cohen S,
    3. Raggatt P,
    4. Taylor K
    (2005) Foetal testosterone, social relationships, and restricted interests in children. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 46:198–210.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  14. ↵
    1. Chapman E,
    2. et al.
    (2006) Fetal testosterone and empathy: evidence from the empathy quotient (EQ) and the “reading the mind in the eyes” test. Soc Neurosci 1:135–148.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  15. ↵
    1. Manning JT,
    2. et al.
    (2000) The 2nd:4th digit ratio, sexual dimorphism, population differences, and reproductive success. evidence for sexually antagonistic genes? Evol Hum Behav 21:163–183.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  16. ↵
    1. Hönekopp J,
    2. Bartholdt L,
    3. Beier L,
    4. Liebert A
    (2007) Second to fourth digit length ratio (2D:4D) and adult sex hormone levels: new data and a meta-analytic review. Psychoneuroendocrinology 32:313–321.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  17. ↵
    1. Breedlove SM
    (2010) Minireview: Organizational hypothesis: Instances of the fingerpost. Endocrinology 151:4116–4122.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  18. ↵
    1. Talarovicová A,
    2. Krsková L,
    3. Blazeková J
    (2009) Testosterone enhancement during pregnancy influences the 2D:4D ratio and open field motor activity of rat siblings in adulthood. Horm Behav 55:235–239.
    OpenUrlPubMed
  19. ↵
    1. Manning JT,
    2. Bundred PE,
    3. Newton DJ,
    4. Flanagan BF
    (2003) The second to fourth digit ratio and variation in the androgen receptor gene. Evol Hum Behav 24:399–405.
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  20. ↵
    1. Berenbaum SA,
    2. Bryk KK,
    3. Nowak N,
    4. Quigley CA,
    5. Moffat S
    (2009) Fingers as a marker of prenatal androgen exposure. Endocrinology 150:5119–5124.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  21. ↵
    1. Manning JT,
    2. Baron-Cohen S,
    3. Wheelwright S,
    4. Sanders G
    (2001) The 2nd to 4th digit ratio and autism. Dev Med Child Neurol 43:160–164.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  22. ↵
    1. Hermans EJ,
    2. Putman P,
    3. van Honk J
    (2006) Testosterone administration reduces empathetic behavior: A facial mimicry study. Psychoneuroendocrinology 31:859–866.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  23. ↵
    1. van Honk J,
    2. Schutter DJLG
    (2007) Testosterone reduces conscious detection of signals serving social correction: Implications for antisocial behavior. Psychol Sci 18:663–667.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  24. ↵
    1. Domes G,
    2. Heinrichs M,
    3. Michel A,
    4. Berger C,
    5. Herpertz SC
    (2007) Oxytocin improves “mind-reading” in humans. Biol Psychiatry 61:731–733.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  25. ↵
    1. Millet K,
    2. Dewitte S
    (2006) Second to fourth digit ratio and cooperative behavior. Biol Psychol 71:111–115.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  26. ↵
    1. Ingudomnukul E,
    2. Baron-Cohen S,
    3. Wheelwright S,
    4. Knickmeyer R
    (2007) Elevated rates of testosterone-related disorders in women with autism spectrum conditions. Horm Behav 51:597–604.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  27. ↵
    1. Harmon-Jones E,
    2. Beer JS
    1. van Honk J
    (2009) in Methods in Social Neuroscience, Neuroendocrine manipulation of the sexually-dimorphic social human brain, eds Harmon-Jones E, Beer JS (Guilford Press, New York), pp 45–69.
  28. ↵
    1. Coates JM,
    2. Gurnell M,
    3. Rustichini A
    (2009) Second-to-fourth digit ratio predicts success among high-frequency financial traders. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 106:623–628.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  29. ↵
    1. Hönekopp J,
    2. Watson S
    (2010) Meta-analysis of digit ratio 2D:4D shows greater sex difference in the right hand. Am J Hum Biol 22:619–630.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  30. ↵
    1. Lutchmaya S,
    2. Baron-Cohen S,
    3. Raggatt P,
    4. Knickmeyer R,
    5. Manning JT
    (2004) 2nd to 4th digit ratios, fetal testosterone and estradiol. Early Hum Dev 77:23–28.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  31. ↵
    1. Manning JT,
    2. Scutt D,
    3. Wilson J,
    4. Lewis-Jones DI
    (1998) The ratio of 2nd to 4th digit length: A predictor of sperm numbers and concentrations of testosterone, luteinizing hormone and oestrogen. Hum Reprod 13:3000–3004.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  32. ↵
    1. Zuloaga DG,
    2. Puts DA,
    3. Jordan CL,
    4. Breedlove SM
    (2008) The role of androgen receptors in the masculinization of brain and behavior: What we've learned from the testicular feminization mutation. Horm Behav 53:613–626.
    OpenUrlPubMed
  33. ↵
    1. Imperato-McGinley J,
    2. et al.
    (1982) Hormonal evaluation of a large kindred with complete androgen insensitivity: evidence for secondary 5 alpha-reductase deficiency. J Clin Endocrinol Metab 54:931–941.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  34. ↵
    1. Grumbach MM,
    2. Auchus RJ
    (1999) Estrogen: Consequences and implications of human mutations in synthesis and action. J Clin Endocrinol Metab 84:4677–4694.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  35. ↵
    1. Guastella AJ,
    2. et al.
    (2010) Intranasal oxytocin improves emotion recognition for youth with autism spectrum disorders. Biol Psychiatry 67:692–694.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  36. ↵
    1. Carter CS
    (2007) Sex differences in oxytocin and vasopressin: Implications for autism spectrum disorders? Behav Brain Res 176:170–186.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  37. ↵
    1. Geary DC,
    2. Flinn MV
    (2002) Sex differences in behavioral and hormonal response to social threat: commentary on Taylor et al. (2000) Psychol Rev 109:745–750.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  38. ↵
    1. Baron-Cohen S
    (2009) Autism: The empathizing-systemizing (E-S) theory. Ann N Y Acad Sci 1156:68–80.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  39. ↵
    1. Tuiten A,
    2. et al.
    (2000) Time course of effects of testosterone administration on sexual arousal in women. Arch Gen Psychiatry 57:149–153.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  40. ↵
    1. Aarts H,
    2. van Honk J
    (2009) Testosterone and unconscious positive priming increase human motivation separately. Neuroreport 20:1300–1303.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  41. ↵
    1. Derntl B,
    2. Kryspin-Exner I,
    3. Fernbach E,
    4. Moser E,
    5. Habel U
    (2008) Emotion recognition accuracy in healthy young females is associated with cycle phase. Horm Behav 53:90–95.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  42. ↵
    1. Derntl B,
    2. et al.
    (2008) Facial emotion recognition and amygdala activation are associated with menstrual cycle phase. Psychoneuroendocrinology 33:1031–1040.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  43. ↵
    1. Postma A,
    2. et al.
    (2000) Effects of testosterone administration on selective aspects of object-location memory in healthy young women. Psychoneuroendocrinology 25:563–575.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  44. ↵
    1. van der Made F,
    2. et al.
    (2009) The influence of testosterone combined with a PDE5-inhibitor on cognitive, affective, and physiological sexual functioning in women suffering from sexual dysfunction. J Sex Med 6:777–790.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  45. ↵
    1. van der Made F,
    2. et al.
    (2009) Childhood sexual abuse, selective attention for sexual cues and the effects of testosterone with or without vardenafil on physiological sexual arousal in women with sexual dysfunction: A pilot study. J Sex Med 6:429–439.
    OpenUrlPubMed
  46. ↵
    1. Eisenegger C,
    2. Naef M,
    3. Snozzi R,
    4. Heinrichs M,
    5. Fehr E
    (2010) Prejudice and truth about the effect of testosterone on human bargaining behaviour. Nature 463:356–359.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  47. ↵
    1. van Honk J,
    2. Peper JS,
    3. Schutter DJ
    (2005) Testosterone reduces unconscious fear but not consciously experienced anxiety: Implications for the disorders of fear and anxiety. Biol Psychiatry 58:218–225.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  48. ↵
    1. van Honk J,
    2. et al.
    (2004) Testosterone shifts the balance between sensitivity for punishment and reward in healthy young women. Psychoneuroendocrinology 29:937–943.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  49. ↵
    1. Shacham S
    (1983) A shortened version of the Profile of Mood States. J Pers Assess 47:305–306.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
PreviousNext
Back to top
Article Alerts
Email Article

Thank you for your interest in spreading the word on PNAS.

NOTE: We only request your email address so that the person you are recommending the page to knows that you wanted them to see it, and that it is not junk mail. We do not capture any email address.

Enter multiple addresses on separate lines or separate them with commas.
Testosterone administration impairs cognitive empathy in women depending on second-to-fourth digit ratio
(Your Name) has sent you a message from PNAS
(Your Name) thought you would like to see the PNAS web site.
CAPTCHA
This question is for testing whether or not you are a human visitor and to prevent automated spam submissions.
Citation Tools
Testosterone administration impairs cognitive empathy in women depending on second-to-fourth digit ratio
Jack van Honk, Dennis J. Schutter, Peter A. Bos, Anne-Wil Kruijt, Eef G. Lentjes, Simon Baron-Cohen
Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences Feb 2011, 108 (8) 3448-3452; DOI: 10.1073/pnas.1011891108

Citation Manager Formats

  • BibTeX
  • Bookends
  • EasyBib
  • EndNote (tagged)
  • EndNote 8 (xml)
  • Medlars
  • Mendeley
  • Papers
  • RefWorks Tagged
  • Ref Manager
  • RIS
  • Zotero
Request Permissions
Share
Testosterone administration impairs cognitive empathy in women depending on second-to-fourth digit ratio
Jack van Honk, Dennis J. Schutter, Peter A. Bos, Anne-Wil Kruijt, Eef G. Lentjes, Simon Baron-Cohen
Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences Feb 2011, 108 (8) 3448-3452; DOI: 10.1073/pnas.1011891108
del.icio.us logo Digg logo Reddit logo Twitter logo CiteULike logo Facebook logo Google logo Mendeley logo
  • Tweet Widget
  • Facebook Like
  • Mendeley logo Mendeley

Article Classifications

  • Biological Sciences
  • Psychological and Cognitive Sciences
Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences: 108 (8)
Table of Contents

Submit

Sign up for Article Alerts

Jump to section

  • Article
    • Abstract
    • Results
    • Discussion
    • Experimental Procedures
    • Acknowledgments
    • Footnotes
    • References
  • Figures & SI
  • Info & Metrics
  • PDF

You May Also be Interested in

Setting sun over a sun-baked dirt landscape
Core Concept: Popular integrated assessment climate policy models have key caveats
Better explicating the strengths and shortcomings of these models will help refine projections and improve transparency in the years ahead.
Image credit: Witsawat.S.
Model of the Amazon forest
News Feature: A sea in the Amazon
Did the Caribbean sweep into the western Amazon millions of years ago, shaping the region’s rich biodiversity?
Image credit: Tacio Cordeiro Bicudo (University of São Paulo, São Paulo, Brazil), Victor Sacek (University of São Paulo, São Paulo, Brazil), and Lucy Reading-Ikkanda (artist).
Syrian archaeological site
Journal Club: In Mesopotamia, early cities may have faltered before climate-driven collapse
Settlements 4,200 years ago may have suffered from overpopulation before drought and lower temperatures ultimately made them unsustainable.
Image credit: Andrea Ricci.
Steamboat Geyser eruption.
Eruption of Steamboat Geyser
Mara Reed and Michael Manga explore why Yellowstone's Steamboat Geyser resumed erupting in 2018.
Listen
Past PodcastsSubscribe
Birds nestling on tree branches
Parent–offspring conflict in songbird fledging
Some songbird parents might improve their own fitness by manipulating their offspring into leaving the nest early, at the cost of fledgling survival, a study finds.
Image credit: Gil Eckrich (photographer).

Similar Articles

Site Logo
Powered by HighWire
  • Submit Manuscript
  • Twitter
  • Facebook
  • RSS Feeds
  • Email Alerts

Articles

  • Current Issue
  • Special Feature Articles – Most Recent
  • List of Issues

PNAS Portals

  • Anthropology
  • Chemistry
  • Classics
  • Front Matter
  • Physics
  • Sustainability Science
  • Teaching Resources

Information

  • Authors
  • Editorial Board
  • Reviewers
  • Subscribers
  • Librarians
  • Press
  • Site Map
  • PNAS Updates
  • FAQs
  • Accessibility Statement
  • Rights & Permissions
  • About
  • Contact

Feedback    Privacy/Legal

Copyright © 2021 National Academy of Sciences. Online ISSN 1091-6490