Skip to main content

Main menu

  • Home
  • Articles
    • Current
    • Special Feature Articles - Most Recent
    • Special Features
    • Colloquia
    • Collected Articles
    • PNAS Classics
    • List of Issues
  • Front Matter
    • Front Matter Portal
    • Journal Club
  • News
    • For the Press
    • This Week In PNAS
    • PNAS in the News
  • Podcasts
  • Authors
    • Information for Authors
    • Editorial and Journal Policies
    • Submission Procedures
    • Fees and Licenses
  • Submit
  • Submit
  • About
    • Editorial Board
    • PNAS Staff
    • FAQ
    • Accessibility Statement
    • Rights and Permissions
    • Site Map
  • Contact
  • Journal Club
  • Subscribe
    • Subscription Rates
    • Subscriptions FAQ
    • Open Access
    • Recommend PNAS to Your Librarian

User menu

  • Log in
  • My Cart

Search

  • Advanced search
Home
Home
  • Log in
  • My Cart

Advanced Search

  • Home
  • Articles
    • Current
    • Special Feature Articles - Most Recent
    • Special Features
    • Colloquia
    • Collected Articles
    • PNAS Classics
    • List of Issues
  • Front Matter
    • Front Matter Portal
    • Journal Club
  • News
    • For the Press
    • This Week In PNAS
    • PNAS in the News
  • Podcasts
  • Authors
    • Information for Authors
    • Editorial and Journal Policies
    • Submission Procedures
    • Fees and Licenses
  • Submit
Research Article

Selectively altering belief formation in the human brain

Tali Sharot, Ryota Kanai, David Marston, Christoph W. Korn, Geraint Rees, and Raymond J. Dolan
  1. aDepartment of Cognitive, Perceptual, and Brain Sciences,
  2. bInstitute of Cognitive Neuroscience, and
  3. fWellcome Trust Centre for Neuroimaging, Institute of Neurology, University College London, London WC1 H0AP, United Kingdom;
  4. cDepartment of Psychology, University of Pennsylvania, Philadelphia, PA 19104-6241;
  5. dDepartment of Education and Psychology, Freie Universität, 14195 Berlin, Germany; and
  6. eBerlin School of Mind and Brain, Humboldt Universität zu Berlin, 10099 Berlin, Germany

See allHide authors and affiliations

PNAS October 16, 2012 109 (42) 17058-17062; https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1205828109
Tali Sharot
aDepartment of Cognitive, Perceptual, and Brain Sciences,
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
  • For correspondence: t.sharot@ucl.ac.uk
Ryota Kanai
bInstitute of Cognitive Neuroscience, and
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
David Marston
cDepartment of Psychology, University of Pennsylvania, Philadelphia, PA 19104-6241;
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Christoph W. Korn
dDepartment of Education and Psychology, Freie Universität, 14195 Berlin, Germany; and
eBerlin School of Mind and Brain, Humboldt Universität zu Berlin, 10099 Berlin, Germany
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Geraint Rees
bInstitute of Cognitive Neuroscience, and
fWellcome Trust Centre for Neuroimaging, Institute of Neurology, University College London, London WC1 H0AP, United Kingdom;
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Raymond J. Dolan
fWellcome Trust Centre for Neuroimaging, Institute of Neurology, University College London, London WC1 H0AP, United Kingdom;
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
  1. Edited by Paul W. Glimcher, New York University, New York, NY, and accepted by the Editorial Board August 15, 2012 (received for review April 6, 2012)

  • Article
  • Figures & SI
  • Info & Metrics
  • PDF
Loading

Abstract

Humans form beliefs asymmetrically; we tend to discount bad news but embrace good news. This reduced impact of unfavorable information on belief updating may have important societal implications, including the generation of financial market bubbles, ill preparedness in the face of natural disasters, and overly aggressive medical decisions. Here, we selectively improved people’s tendency to incorporate bad news into their beliefs by disrupting the function of the left (but not right) inferior frontal gyrus using transcranial magnetic stimulation, thereby eliminating the engrained “good news/bad news effect.” Our results provide an instance of how selective disruption of regional human brain function paradoxically enhances the ability to incorporate unfavorable information into beliefs of vulnerability.

  • cognition
  • learning
  • optimism

We are constantly flooded with information that helps form our beliefs about reality (e.g., via the Web, advertising, colleagues, and friends). Understanding how beliefs are formed is critical, as beliefs drive our actions and decisions. Normative theories assume beliefs are adjusted according to Bayes’ Rule (1). Indeed, this assumption often holds when people incorporate favorable news into their existing beliefs (2). However, for unfavorable news people show an aversion to incorporating new information (3). Specifically, they discount the strength of the new information leading to noisy posterior beliefs (2). This tendency to selectively ignore negative information is known as the “good news/bad news effect” (2).

For example, people adjust their beliefs regarding their level of intelligence and physical attractiveness when they receive information indicating they are more intelligent and attractive than they had assumed. However, they relatively fail to adjust their beliefs in response to information suggesting they rate lower on these attributes than they had previously thought (3). In addition, when learning that their risk of experiencing future negative events, such as cancer, is higher than they had expected, people are less likely to integrate these data into their prior beliefs relative to a situation when they learn that their risk is lower than expected (4).

The consequences of readily integrating good news into our beliefs while underweighting bad news are likely to be considerable for an individual and for society. The bias is thought to help explain financial market bubbles (5), ill preparedness in the face of natural disasters (6), overly aggressive medical decisions (7), overconfidence (8), and optimism (4).

Here, we tested whether the tendency to discount bad news could be reversed by altering brain functions thought to mediate such discounting, thereby eliminating the good news/bad news effect. A candidate brain region is the inferior frontal gyrus (IFG), suggested to play a key role in two mechanisms likely to be involved in generating the good news/bad news effect: updating beliefs and inhibition.

First, the IFG is important for flexibly altering beliefs. For example, it plays an important role in reversal learning (9) and has been shown to track and integrate information into prior beliefs (4). Specifically, as the left IFG tracks desirable errors in estimation triggered by information that is better than expected, the right IFG tracks undesirable errors (4). In optimistic individuals the left IFG shows greater fidelity in tracking errors relative to the right, such that adjustment of beliefs in response to bad news is less likely than in response to good news (4). Previous studies have suggested similar hemispheric asymmetry in processing positive and negative information (10). If relatively efficient function of left IFG (compared with the right) leads to selective updating, than interfering with its function should reduce the bias.

Second, the IFG is thought to mediate different forms of inhibition (for review, see ref. 11). These forms include response/action inhibition (12), task-set switching (13), inhibition of unwanted memories (14), and inhibition of working memory to resolve interference from previous trials (15). Although response inhibition has been suggested to involve primarily the right IFG (11, but see refs. 13 and 16), other forms of inhibition are thought to be lateralized to the left IFG (14) or bilateral IFG (15). If reduced learning from unfavorable news involves active inhibition mediated by the IFG (either left, right, or both), then disruption of its function will release this inhibition, improve learning from bad news, and thus eliminate the bias.

To test these hypotheses we used a causal intervention, using off-line repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS) to disrupt the function of either left IFG, right IFG, or a control region (Oz) in three different groups of human participants who then completed a belief formation updating task (4). Participants estimated their likelihood of experiencing 40 adverse life events [e.g., Alzheimer’s disease, robbery (4)]. After each trial, participants were presented with the actual frequency of that event in their representative population (Fig. 1A). We assessed whether participants used this factual information to revise their predictions by subsequently asking them, in a second session, to re-estimate their likelihoods for all life events. Participants then rated all stimuli on vividness, familiarity, prior experience, arousal, controllability, and negativity, and were tested for memory of the information presented.

Fig. 1.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Fig. 1.

(A) In each trial participants were presented with a short description of one of 40 adverse life events and asked to estimate how likely this event was to occur to them. They were then presented with the average probability of that event occurring to a person living in the same sociocultural environment. The second session was the same as the first session, except that the average probability of the event to occur was not presented again. (B and C) Examples of trials for which the participant’s estimate was (B) higher or (C) lower than the average probability. Here, for illustration purposes only, the blue and red frames denote the participant’s response (either an overestimation or underestimation, respectively) and the blue and red filled boxes denote information that calls for an adjustment in a (B) favorable (good news) or (C) unfavorable (bad news) direction.

Results

Trials were subsequently divided into ones in which participants received good news (i.e., the probability presented was better than the estimate of their own probability) (Fig. 1B) or bad news (i.e., the probability presented was worse) (Fig. 1C). For each participant, update scores corresponding to the difference between the first and second estimates were calculated for each trial. Then two average update scores were calculated for each participant: one for the good news condition and one for the bad news condition. We entered these average score values into a three (TMS group: right IFG, left IFG, control site) by two (valence: good news/bad news) ANOVA.

Our analysis revealed a significant interaction between group and valence [F(2,27) = 6.5, P < 0.005]. Participants who received TMS to the control site or right IFG showed the typical good news/bad news effect; they updated their beliefs more when presented with good news relative to bad news [right IFG: t(9) = 3.4, P < 0.01, 100% of participants showed the effect; control: t(9) = 4.2, P < 0.01, 90% of participants showed the effect]. In contrast, the good news/bad news effect was eliminated in participants who received TMS to the left IFG [t(9) = 0.9, P > 0.3, 40% of participants showed the effect] (Fig. 2), and was significantly reduced compared with right IFG [F(1,18) = 3.2, P < 0.005] and control group [F(1,18) = 2.5, P < 0.02]. The result suggests a causal link between left IFG function and the good news/bad news effect.

Fig. 2.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Fig. 2.

(A) Bar graphs plotting the good news/bad news effect (= update in response to good news − update in response to bad news) for every participant in every group, rank-ordered by effect size. Positive numbers indicate greater belief update in response to good news relative to bad news, negative numbers indicate the reverse. The effect was observed in only 40% of participants who received TMS to the left IFG, in 90% of those who received TMS to the control site, and in 100% of participants who received TMS to the right IFG. (B) Mean update for all participants in each TMS condition when receiving good or bad news. Error bars are SEM; *indicates statistical significance at a threshold of P < 0.05, two tailed. Participants whose left IFG is disrupted with TMS update their estimates more after receiving bad news. As a consequence, they do not show the good news/bad news effect observed in the other two groups (this reduction is significant relative to the other two groups).

Note that the critical group by valance interaction was not affected by whether the participant was asked to decide the likelihood of the event happening to them in the future or not happening to them in the future. Specifically, entering average update values into a two (frame: happen, not happen) by three (TMS group: right IFG, left IFG, control site) by two (valence: good news/bad news) ANOVA did not reveal a significant three-way interaction [F(2,27 = 0.58, P > 0.5].

Next, we examined whether elimination of the good news/bad news effect following TMS to the left IFG was the result of a reduction of belief updating in response to good news, an enhancement in response to bad news, or both. We found that the good news/bad news effect was abolished following TMS to the left IFG because a disposition to adjust beliefs when receiving bad news was enhanced relative to controls [t(18) = 2.6, P < 0.05]. In contrast, updating in response to good news was preserved [t(18) = 0.068 P > 0.9]. This finding is consistent with the hypothesis that the left IFG inhibits updating in response to bad news.

Importantly, there were no significant effects on any other measures elicited from the participants. Specifically, no group by valence interaction or group effect emerged when testing participants’ ratings of familiarity with stimuli, prior experience, vividness, arousal, negativity, controllability, and reaction times (see Table 1 for scores and statistics). Participants were also asked at the end of the session to provide the actual probability previously presented of each event. Memory errors were calculated as the absolute difference between the probability previously presented and the participants’ recollection of that statistic. These errors did not differ between groups and there was no interaction with valence (Table 1). We also calculated the Pearson correlation coefficients between update and the participants’ initial estimate for all good news trials and all bad news trials, to test whether TMS was effecting these correlations. These data did not differ between groups and there was no interaction with valence (all P > 0.1). Nonetheless, we entered all differential scores (average rating for good news trials minus average rating for bad news trials) of all of the measures above as covariates into an ANCOVA testing update scores with TMS group (right IFG, left IFG, control site) as random between subject effects and valence (good news, bad news) as within-subject effect. This analysis showed that the interaction between group and valence on update scores remained significant even after controlling for all these additional variables [F(2,17) = 6.43, P < 0.01].

View this table:
  • View inline
  • View popup
Table 1.

Participants’ ratings of familiarity with stimuli, prior experience, vividness, arousal, negativity, controllability, memory, update, initial estimates, and reaction times

Finally, we examined if initial estimates for either good news trials or bad news trials were at ceiling or floor. One-sample t tests revealed that the average ratings in each group for each valence condition were significantly different from both the minimum and maximum probabilities (see methods, all P < 0.0001). Thus, initial estimates were neither at floor nor ceiling. There were no significant difference between groups on initial estimates and no interaction of group and valence (Table 1).

Discussion

Disruption of the left IFG abolished a deep-rooted bias in belief formation known as the good news/bad news effect. This effect refers to the reduced tendency to alter beliefs in response to unfavorable information compared with favorable information. Specifically, interfering with activity of the left IFG using TMS paradoxically enhanced participants’ ability to alter beliefs in response to unfavorable information regarding the risk of encountering negative events. The effect on belief formation following TMS to the left IFG was highly selective and did not affect the evaluation of the stimuli presented (including emotional evaluation of the stimuli, sense of familiarity, and vividness) or memory for the information presented.

The enhancement in the inclination to adjust one’s beliefs when confronted with bad news following TMS of the left IFG, suggests that this region normally inhibits this ability and that its disruption releases such inhibition. This finding is consistent with the known inhibitory role to the IFG (11). The left IFG has been previously reported to mediate inhibition of unwanted memories (14), inhibition of working memory to reduce interference (14), and inhibition essential for task switching (13) and shifting (17). The present results suggest that the left IFG also plays a role in inhibiting unwanted news from altering beliefs. Suppression of the left IFG may also conceivably render other regions task-active; we speculate that this may include the contralateral right IFG by releasing it from interhemispheric inhibition (18).

Belief change following TMS to the left IFG was specific to adjustments in response to bad news in so far as updating in response to good news was unaltered. A previous functional MRI study using the same update task used here reported error-related activity in response to good news in the left IFG as well as in several other brain regions, including the right and left medial frontal cortex/superior frontal gyrus, and right cerebellum (4). Activity of these latter regions predicted the amount of update in response to good news (4). Thus, although the left IFG plays a role in tracking positive information (4), the present results suggest that disruption of just this one region is insufficient to significantly influence favorable updates.

Our results should not be interpreted as suggesting that the disruption of left IFG function improves learning or decision-making in general. Rather, our study provides an interesting instance of how a selective disruption to a specific brain region may enhance the tendency to integrate negative information into beliefs regarding vulnerability. Paradoxically, this enhancement may be suboptimal, as underestimating our susceptibility to negative events has adaptive benefits that include increasing explorative behavior and reducing stress and anxiety, a factor that has links with physical and mental well-being (19). However, any advantage arising out of a reduced tendency to learn from bad news is likely to come at a cost. A pertinent example is the discounting of warning signs regarding financial risk, which is widely perceived as a contributing factor to the 2008 global economic collapse (20).

Materials and Methods

Participants.

Thirty healthy right-handed participants were recruited via the University College London psychology subject pool (age range, 20–35 y). All subjects gave written informed consent and were paid for their participation. The study was approved by the University College London Research Ethics Committee. Participants were randomly assigned to one of three groups: right IFG TMS, left IFG TMS, or Control Site TMS (10 subjects in each group).

Stimuli.

Forty short descriptions of negative life events (e.g., car theft, Alzheimer’s disease) were presented in random order. Stimuli were split into two lists of 20 events each.

For each adverse event the average probability of that event occurring at least once to a person living in the same sociocultural environment as the participants was determined from on-line resources (Office for National Statistics, Eurostat, PubMed). Very rare or very common events were not included; all events probabilities lay between 10% and 70%. To ensure that the range of possible overestimation equaled the range of possible underestimation, participants were told that the range of probabilities lay between 3% and 77%.

Procedure.

Participants completed the task 5 min after the 40-s off-line continuous θ-burst stimulation (cTBS) protocol (see details below).

The paradigm was adapted from our previous study (4). On each trial participants were presented with 1 of 40 adverse life events for 4 s. Then participants estimated how likely the event was to happen to them in the future. Participants had up to 10 s to respond. After a fixation cross (1.2 s), they were presented with the actuarial frequency of the event, for a demographically similar population, for 3 s, followed by fixation for 1.2 s (Fig. 1). In a second session participants were asked to again provide estimates of their likelihood of encountering the events, so that we could assess how they used the information provided. Update was calculated for trials in which participants received good news (when a participant received an estimate that was better than expected; update = first estimate − second estimate) and trials in which they received bad news (when a participant received an estimate that was worse than expected; update = second estimate − first estimate). Thus, positive updates indicate a change toward the actuarial frequency and negative updates a change away from the actuarial frequency.

The extent of the good news/bad news effect is equal to the difference between update in respond to good news minus the amount of update in respond to bad news:

Embedded Image

The second session was the same as the first, except that the average probability of the event to occur was not presented again.

Trials were divided into two blocks. Each block consisted two sessions, with each session involving 20 trials. In one block (counterbalanced) participants estimated their likelihood as described above. In the other block they estimated their likelihood of not encountering the adverse events and received the frequency of the events not happening, such that results could not be attributed to differential processing of high or low numbers. All statistical percentages and all responses in the “not happen” sessions were transformed into the corresponding numbers of the “happen” sessions by subtracting the respective number from 100.

To test memory for the information presented, subjects were asked at the end of the session to provide the actual probability previously presented of each event. Memory errors were calculated as the absolute difference between the probability previously presented and the participants’ recollection of that statistic.

Embedded Image

Participants then rated all stimuli on: vividness (How vividly could you imagine this event? From 1 = not vivid to 6 = very vivid); familiarity (Regardless if this event has happened to you before, how familiar do you feel it is to you from TV, friends, movies and so on? From 1 = not at all familiar to 6 very familiar); prior experience (Has this event happened to you before? From 1 = never to 6 = very often); arousal (When you imagine this event happening to you how emotionally arousing is the image in your mind? From 1 = not arousing at all to 6 = very arousing); negativity (How negative would this event be for you? From 1 = not negative at all to 6 = very negative); and controllability (Is this event under your control? From 1 = not at all to 6 = very much).

TMS.

TMS pulses were delivered by a Magstim Rapid2 Stimulator (Magstim) at 40% of the maximum stimulator output using a small TMS coil (figure-of-eight shape, 50-mm diameter). We used an off-line continuous cTBS protocol, which consisted of three pulses at 50 Hz repeated at 200-ms intervals for a total duration of 40 s. A 5-min rest period was implemented after the termination of cTBS before participants started the task.

Target sites were selected based on a previous functional MRI paper (4). Specifically, we targeted the peak voxel in a region where blood oxygen-level dependent signals correlate with participant’s estimation error in response to negative information about the future (right IFG) and the region in which activity correlated with estimation errors in response to positive information (left IFG). The target sites for individual participants were identified by converting the Talairach coordinates (x/y/z = ± 4 8/18/16) into real space for individual participants. In this procedure, structural MRI scans were first transformed to a standard Montreal Neurological Institute (MNI) template using the FMRIB Software Library. The coordinates for IFG sites were marked in the MNI space and then were inverse transformed to the original image space. The markers for the target sites were used to guide frameless stereotaxy with the Brainsight system (Rogue Research). In addition to the two IFG sites, Oz position of the International 10-20 system was used as a control site for nonspecific effects of TMS.

Behavioral Analysis.

Two average update scores were calculated for each participant; one for the good news condition and one for the bad news condition. Those average scores were entered into a two (valence: good news, bad news) by three (group: left IFG, control, right IFG) ANOVA.

We then proceeded to conduct ANOVAs for each additional measure elicited (i.e., memory, reaction times, and the six stimuli ratings: vividness, familiarity, controllability, experience, arousal, negativity) to assure that they did not show an interaction. We also calculated for each subject the correlation between initial estimates and update for good news trials and bad news trials and entered those into an ANOVA.

Finally, to control for all these variables, we conducted an additional analysis in which the differential scores of all additional ratings described above were added as covariates in an ANCOVA entering update scores as a dependent variable, with valence as a within subject variable (valence: good news, bad news) and group as a between subject random effects variable (TMS group: IFG left, IFG right, control site).

Acknowledgments

This work was supported by Wellcome Trust programme Grant 078865/Z/05/Z (to R.J.D.); a Wellcome Trust Career Development Fellowship (to T.S.); a Wellcome Trust Senior Clinical Fellowship (to G.R.); and the Japan Society for the Promotion of Science (R.K.). The Wellcome Trust Centre for Neuroimaging is supported by core funding from the Wellcome Trust (091593/Z/10/Z).

Footnotes

  • ↵1T.S. and R.K. contributed equally to this work.

  • ↵2To whom correspondence should be addressed. E-mail: t.sharot{at}ucl.ac.uk.
  • Author contributions: T.S., R.K., and C.W.K. designed research; T.S., R.K., and D.M. performed research; T.S., D.M., and C.W.K. analyzed data; and T.S., R.K., C.W.K., G.R., and R.J.D. wrote the paper.

  • The authors declare no conflict of interest.

  • This article is a PNAS Direct Submission. P.W.G. is a guest editor invited by the Editorial Board.

Freely available online through the PNAS open access option.

References

  1. ↵
    1. Green D-M,
    2. Swets J-A
    (1966) Signal Detection Theory and Psychophysics (John Wiley and Sons, New York).
  2. ↵
    1. Eli D,
    2. Rao J-M
    (2012) The Good News-Bad News Effect: Asymmetric processing of objective information about yourself. American Economic Journal: Microeconomics 3:114138.
    OpenUrl
  3. ↵
    1. Koszegi B
    (2006) Ego utility, overconfidence, and task choice. J Eur Econ Assoc 4:673–707.
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  4. ↵
    1. Sharot T,
    2. Korn C-W,
    3. Dolan R-J
    (2011) How unrealistic optimism is maintained in the face of reality. Nat Neurosci 14:1475–1479.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  5. ↵
    1. Farmer R
    (1999) The Macroeconomics of Self-fulfilling Prophecies (MIT Press, Cambridge, MA).
  6. ↵
    1. Paton D
    (2003) Disaster preparedness: A social-cognitive perspective. Disaster Prevention and Management 12(Iss: 3):210–216.
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  7. ↵
    1. Paling J
    (2003) Strategies to help patients understand risks. BMJ 327:745–748.
    OpenUrlFREE Full Text
  8. ↵
    1. Johnson D-D,
    2. Fowler J-H
    (2011) The evolution of overconfidence. Nature 477:317–320.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  9. ↵
    1. Cools R,
    2. Clark L,
    3. Owen AM,
    4. Robbins TW
    (2002) Defining the neural mechanisms of probabilistic reversal learning using event-related functional magnetic resonance imaging. J Neurosci 22:4563–4567.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  10. ↵
    1. Davidson RJ,
    2. Hugdahl K
    1. Davidson RJ
    (1995) Cerebral asymmetry, emotion and affective style. Brain Asymmetry, eds Davidson RJ, Hugdahl K (MIT Press, Cambridge, MA), pp 361–387.
  11. ↵
    1. Aron AR,
    2. Robbins TW,
    3. Poldrack RA
    (2004) Inhibition and the right inferior frontal cortex. Trends Cogn Sci 8:170–177.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  12. ↵
    1. Bunge SA,
    2. Dudukovic NM,
    3. Thomason ME,
    4. Vaidya CJ,
    5. Gabrieli JD
    (2002) Immature frontal lobe contributions to cognitive control in children: Evidence from fMRI. Neuron 33:301–311.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  13. ↵
    1. Dove A,
    2. Pollmann S,
    3. Schubert T,
    4. Wiggins CJ,
    5. von Cramon DY
    (2000) Prefrontal cortex activation in task switching: An event-related fMRI study. Brain Res Cogn Brain Res 9:103–109.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  14. ↵
    1. Anderson MC,
    2. et al.
    (2004) Neural systems underlying the suppression of unwanted memories. Science 303:232–235.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  15. ↵
    1. D’Esposito M,
    2. Postle BR,
    3. Jonides J,
    4. Smith EE
    (1999) The neural substrate and temporal dynamics of interference effects in working memory as revealed by event-related functional MRI. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 96:7514–7519.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  16. ↵
    1. Konishi S,
    2. et al.
    (1999) Common inhibitory mechanism in human inferior prefrontal cortex revealed by event-related functional MRI. Brain 122:981–991.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  17. ↵
    1. Nakahara K,
    2. Hayashi T,
    3. Konishi S,
    4. Miyashita Y
    (2002) Functional MRI of macaque monkeys performing a cognitive set-shifting task. Science 295:1532–1536.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  18. ↵
    1. Suppa A,
    2. et al.
    (2008) Theta burst stimulation induces after-effects on contralateral primary motor cortex excitability in humans. J Physiol 586:4489–4500.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  19. ↵
    1. Taylor S-E,
    2. Kemeny M-E,
    3. Reed G-M,
    4. Bower J-E,
    5. Gruenewald T-L
    (2000) Psychological resources, positive illusions, and health. Am Psychol 55:99–109.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  20. ↵
    1. Siegel LB
    1. Shefrin H
    (2010) in Voices of Wisdom: Understanding the Global Financial Crisis, ed Siegel LB, (Research foundation of CFA Institute, Charlottesville, VA) Available at http://ssrn.com/abstract=1523931, pp 224–256.
PreviousNext
Back to top
Article Alerts
Email Article

Thank you for your interest in spreading the word on PNAS.

NOTE: We only request your email address so that the person you are recommending the page to knows that you wanted them to see it, and that it is not junk mail. We do not capture any email address.

Enter multiple addresses on separate lines or separate them with commas.
Selectively altering belief formation in the human brain
(Your Name) has sent you a message from PNAS
(Your Name) thought you would like to see the PNAS web site.
CAPTCHA
This question is for testing whether or not you are a human visitor and to prevent automated spam submissions.
Citation Tools
Altering belief formation in the human brain
Tali Sharot, Ryota Kanai, David Marston, Christoph W. Korn, Geraint Rees, Raymond J. Dolan
Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences Oct 2012, 109 (42) 17058-17062; DOI: 10.1073/pnas.1205828109

Citation Manager Formats

  • BibTeX
  • Bookends
  • EasyBib
  • EndNote (tagged)
  • EndNote 8 (xml)
  • Medlars
  • Mendeley
  • Papers
  • RefWorks Tagged
  • Ref Manager
  • RIS
  • Zotero
Request Permissions
Share
Altering belief formation in the human brain
Tali Sharot, Ryota Kanai, David Marston, Christoph W. Korn, Geraint Rees, Raymond J. Dolan
Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences Oct 2012, 109 (42) 17058-17062; DOI: 10.1073/pnas.1205828109
del.icio.us logo Digg logo Reddit logo Twitter logo CiteULike logo Facebook logo Google logo Mendeley logo
  • Tweet Widget
  • Facebook Like
  • Mendeley logo Mendeley

Article Classifications

  • Biological Sciences
  • Neuroscience
  • Social Sciences
  • Psychological and Cognitive Sciences
Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences: 109 (42)
Table of Contents

Submit

Sign up for Article Alerts

Jump to section

  • Article
    • Abstract
    • Results
    • Discussion
    • Materials and Methods
    • Acknowledgments
    • Footnotes
    • References
  • Figures & SI
  • Info & Metrics
  • PDF

You May Also be Interested in

Setting sun over a sun-baked dirt landscape
Core Concept: Popular integrated assessment climate policy models have key caveats
Better explicating the strengths and shortcomings of these models will help refine projections and improve transparency in the years ahead.
Image credit: Witsawat.S.
Model of the Amazon forest
News Feature: A sea in the Amazon
Did the Caribbean sweep into the western Amazon millions of years ago, shaping the region’s rich biodiversity?
Image credit: Tacio Cordeiro Bicudo (University of São Paulo, São Paulo, Brazil), Victor Sacek (University of São Paulo, São Paulo, Brazil), and Lucy Reading-Ikkanda (artist).
Syrian archaeological site
Journal Club: In Mesopotamia, early cities may have faltered before climate-driven collapse
Settlements 4,200 years ago may have suffered from overpopulation before drought and lower temperatures ultimately made them unsustainable.
Image credit: Andrea Ricci.
Steamboat Geyser eruption.
Eruption of Steamboat Geyser
Mara Reed and Michael Manga explore why Yellowstone's Steamboat Geyser resumed erupting in 2018.
Listen
Past PodcastsSubscribe
Birds nestling on tree branches
Parent–offspring conflict in songbird fledging
Some songbird parents might improve their own fitness by manipulating their offspring into leaving the nest early, at the cost of fledgling survival, a study finds.
Image credit: Gil Eckrich (photographer).

Similar Articles

Site Logo
Powered by HighWire
  • Submit Manuscript
  • Twitter
  • Facebook
  • RSS Feeds
  • Email Alerts

Articles

  • Current Issue
  • Special Feature Articles – Most Recent
  • List of Issues

PNAS Portals

  • Anthropology
  • Chemistry
  • Classics
  • Front Matter
  • Physics
  • Sustainability Science
  • Teaching Resources

Information

  • Authors
  • Editorial Board
  • Reviewers
  • Subscribers
  • Librarians
  • Press
  • Site Map
  • PNAS Updates
  • FAQs
  • Accessibility Statement
  • Rights & Permissions
  • About
  • Contact

Feedback    Privacy/Legal

Copyright © 2021 National Academy of Sciences. Online ISSN 1091-6490