Skip to main content
  • Submit
  • About
    • Editorial Board
    • PNAS Staff
    • FAQ
    • Rights and Permissions
    • Site Map
  • Contact
  • Journal Club
  • Subscribe
    • Subscription Rates
    • Subscriptions FAQ
    • Open Access
    • Recommend PNAS to Your Librarian
  • Log in
  • Log out
  • My Cart

Main menu

  • Home
  • Articles
    • Current
    • Latest Articles
    • Special Features
    • Colloquia
    • Collected Articles
    • PNAS Classics
    • Archive
  • Front Matter
  • News
    • For the Press
    • Highlights from Latest Articles
    • PNAS in the News
  • Podcasts
  • Authors
    • Information for Authors
    • Purpose and Scope
    • Editorial and Journal Policies
    • Submission Procedures
    • For Reviewers
    • Author FAQ
  • Submit
  • About
    • Editorial Board
    • PNAS Staff
    • FAQ
    • Rights and Permissions
    • Site Map
  • Contact
  • Journal Club
  • Subscribe
    • Subscription Rates
    • Subscriptions FAQ
    • Open Access
    • Recommend PNAS to Your Librarian

User menu

  • Log in
  • Log out
  • My Cart

Search

  • Advanced search
Home
Home

Advanced Search

  • Home
  • Articles
    • Current
    • Latest Articles
    • Special Features
    • Colloquia
    • Collected Articles
    • PNAS Classics
    • Archive
  • Front Matter
  • News
    • For the Press
    • Highlights from Latest Articles
    • PNAS in the News
  • Podcasts
  • Authors
    • Information for Authors
    • Purpose and Scope
    • Editorial and Journal Policies
    • Submission Procedures
    • For Reviewers
    • Author FAQ

New Research In

Physical Sciences

Featured Portals

  • Physics
  • Chemistry
  • Sustainability Science

Articles by Topic

  • Applied Mathematics
  • Applied Physical Sciences
  • Astronomy
  • Computer Sciences
  • Earth, Atmospheric, and Planetary Sciences
  • Engineering
  • Environmental Sciences
  • Mathematics
  • Statistics

Social Sciences

Featured Portals

  • Anthropology
  • Sustainability Science

Articles by Topic

  • Economic Sciences
  • Environmental Sciences
  • Political Sciences
  • Psychological and Cognitive Sciences
  • Social Sciences

Biological Sciences

Featured Portals

  • Sustainability Science

Articles by Topic

  • Agricultural Sciences
  • Anthropology
  • Applied Biological Sciences
  • Biochemistry
  • Biophysics and Computational Biology
  • Cell Biology
  • Developmental Biology
  • Ecology
  • Environmental Sciences
  • Evolution
  • Genetics
  • Immunology and Inflammation
  • Medical Sciences
  • Microbiology
  • Neuroscience
  • Pharmacology
  • Physiology
  • Plant Biology
  • Population Biology
  • Psychological and Cognitive Sciences
  • Sustainability Science
  • Systems Biology
Research Article

A common rule for decision making in animal collectives across species

Sara Arganda, Alfonso Pérez-Escudero, and Gonzalo G. de Polavieja
PNAS December 11, 2012 109 (50) 20508-20513; https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1210664109
Sara Arganda
Instituto Cajal, Consejo Superior de Investigaciones Científicas, 28002 Madrid, Spain
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Alfonso Pérez-Escudero
Instituto Cajal, Consejo Superior de Investigaciones Científicas, 28002 Madrid, Spain
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Gonzalo G. de Polavieja
Instituto Cajal, Consejo Superior de Investigaciones Científicas, 28002 Madrid, Spain
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
  • For correspondence: gonzalo.polavieja@cajal.csic.es
  1. Edited by Simon A. Levin, Princeton University, Princeton, NJ, and approved November 2, 2012 (received for review June 21, 2012)

This article has a Correction. Please see:

  • Correction for Arganda et al., A common rule for decision making in animal collectives across species
  • Article
  • Figures & SI
  • Info & Metrics
  • PDF
Loading

Abstract

A diversity of decision-making systems has been observed in animal collectives. In some species, choices depend on the differences of the numbers of animals that have chosen each of the available options, whereas in other species on the relative differences (a behavior known as Weber’s law), or follow more complex rules. We here show that this diversity of decision systems corresponds to a single rule of decision making in collectives. We first obtained a decision rule based on Bayesian estimation that uses the information provided by the behaviors of the other individuals to improve the estimation of the structure of the world. We then tested this rule in decision experiments using zebrafish (Danio rerio), and in existing rich datasets of argentine ants (Linepithema humile) and sticklebacks (Gasterosteus aculeatus), showing that a unified model across species can quantitatively explain the diversity of decision systems. Further, these results show that the different counting systems used by animals, including humans, can emerge from the common principle of using social information to make good decisions.

  • collective behavior
  • public information
  • probability matching

Sensory data always has some degree of ambiguity, so animals need to make decisions by estimating the properties of the environment from uncertain sensory data (1⇓⇓⇓–5). This estimation has been shown to be close to optimal in many cases, making optimal Bayesian decision making a successful framework shared by behavioral, neurobiological, and psychological studies (1⇓⇓⇓⇓⇓–7).

A richer scenario for decision making takes place when animals move in groups. In this case, the behaviors of other animals are an extra source of information (6⇓⇓⇓⇓⇓⇓⇓⇓⇓⇓⇓⇓⇓⇓⇓⇓⇓⇓⇓⇓⇓⇓⇓⇓⇓⇓⇓–34). Animals of different species have been observed to incorporate this extra information in their decisions in different ways. Some species make decisions that can be explained using the differences of the numbers of animals taking each option (21, 22), others according to the relative differences (Weber’s law) (23, 24) or using other rules (25⇓⇓⇓⇓⇓⇓⇓⇓–34). This diversity of decision schemes has translated into a diversity of models (21, 22, 24⇓⇓⇓⇓⇓⇓⇓⇓⇓–34).

To search for a unified framework having the diversity of decision-making schemes as particular cases, we generalized Bayesian decision making to the case of animal collectives. Our previous attempt at building such a theory predicted that the only relevant social information is the difference of the numbers of individuals already choosing each available option, and not the numbers themselves or the relative differences (or Weber’s law) (22). However, this theory was limited to the particular case in which only one of the options could be a good option (22). We have now generalized the theory, allowing all available options to be good or bad options. We found that this generalization explains the diversity of decision rules observed in collectives, maintaining the same conceptual and mathematical simplicity, and containing our previous theory as a particular case. We have tested the theory experimentally in decision experiments using zebrafish (Danio rerio), but to cover the diversity of decision systems, we have also tested it using rich datasets of decision making in argentine ants (Linepithema humile) (24) and three-spined sticklebacks (Gasterosteus aculeatus) (25, 26). We found a quantitative match between the theory and the different decision systems of these representative species.

Results

We studied how the behaviors of others should be taken into account to improve the estimations of the structure of the world and make decisions in animal collectives. For a situation with two identical options to choose from (Fig. 1A), we looked for the probability that one option, say x, is a good option given that nx and ny animals have already chosen options x and y, respectively. We used Bayesian theory to find an approximated analytic expression for this probability as (SI Text)

Embedded Image
Fig. 1.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Fig. 1.

A general decision-making rule in animal collectives. (A) Decision making between two sites when nx and ny animals have already chosen sites x and y, respectively. (B) The probability of choosing x in the general rule (Eq. 3), plotted as a function of the animals that have already chosen between the two sites, nx and ny. The theory predicts very different structure in the probability for the case of low and high numbers of animals, separated by point Graphic. The rate of change of Px in the transition regions depends on the reliability parameter s, with the width of these regions proportional to Graphic. (C) Same as B but for three different values of parameter k: k = 0 (Left), 0 < k < 1 (Center), and k = 1 (Right).

Parameter a measures the quality of nonsocial information available to the deciding individual, and s measures how reliably an individual that has chosen x indicates to the deciding individual that x is a good option. According to Eq. 1, the higher the number of individuals that chose option x, nx, the higher the probability that option x is good for the deciding individual, and more so the higher the reliability s of the information from the individuals that already chose x. However, each individual that chooses y decreases the probability that x is a good option. Parameter k measures the relative impact of these two opposing effects. Individuals need to decide based on the estimated probabilities in Eq. 1. A common decision rule in animals, from insects to humans, is probability matching, according to which the probability of choosing a behavior is proportional to the estimated probability (35⇓⇓⇓⇓⇓⇓⇓⇓–44),

Embedded Image

This rule is known to be optimal when there is competition for resources (39, 40) and when the estimated probabilities change in time (41⇓⇓–44). Probability matching in Eq. 2, together with the estimation in Eq. 1, gives that the probability of choosing x is

Embedded Image

and Graphic is the probability of choosing y. The main implications of Eq. 3 are apparent in its plot (Fig. 1B). First, decision making in collectives is predicted to be different for low and high numbers of individuals. For low numbers, there is a fast transition between preferring one side over the other, whereas for high numbers the transition has an intermediate region with no preference in which the probability has a plateau of value of one-half. There is a clear separation between the low- and high- numbers regimes at the point Graphic in which the plateau starts (Fig. 1B; SI Text). Second, in the high-numbers regime, the isoprobability curves are straight lines of slope k. We can use this slope to classify three very different scenarios we found to correspond to different experimental datasets: Graphic, Graphic, and Graphic (Fig. 1C).

For k = 0, the animals at one option do not impact negatively on the estimated quality of the other option; this can take place, for example, when animals at one option do not seem to have information about the other option. An important prediction for this case is that for high numbers of animals there is a large plateau of probability one-half of choosing each of the two options (Fig. 1C, Left). To have a significantly higher probability of choosing one option, say x, it is then needed not only that Graphic but also to be outside of the large plateau, which means that very few animals have chosen the other option y, Graphic. A second prediction is that there is a finite number of animals that need to be distinguished; to see this, consider that the probability that option x is a good one (Eq. 1) for k = 0 increases monotonically with nx and converges to 1. The number of animals nx needed to reach a high probability of 0.95 is given by Graphic (Fig. S1). Beyond α the probability changes very little, thus in practice it is not necessary to count beyond that number. For a wide range of parameters a and s, α has low values, corresponding to counting up to a low number of animals (Fig. S1).

We have found that wild-type zebrafish (D. rerio) in a two-choice setup used for tests of sociability (45, 46) make choices that quantitatively correspond to the predictions of the k = 0 case. The setup has three chambers separated by transparent walls; a central chamber with the zebrafish we monitor, and two lateral chambers with different numbers of zebrafish acting as social stimuli (Fig. 2A; Materials and Methods). An interesting feature of this setup is that it measures the behavior of a single individual when presented with social stimuli, allowing a direct test of the individual decision rule in Eq. 3. Specifically, we measured the probability that the focal fish chooses each of the two options for a range of configurations (Fig. 2B; each dot is the mean of typically n = 15 animals). We found that these experimental results correspond to Eq. 3 for a = 11.2, s = 5, and k = 0 (Fig. 2B, blue surface) with a robust fit (Fig. S2). To make a more quantitative comparison between theory and experiment, we highlighted several lines on the theoretical surface, using different colors to indicate different numbers of fish at option y. Fig. 2C compares the probability values for these five lines with the experimental data, showing a close match. The model offers both a quantitative fit to data and a simple explanation of the experimental result. Fish do not choose directly according to the number of other fish, but to how these numbers indicate that a place is a good option, giving a rule of “counting up to 3.”

Fig. 2.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Fig. 2.

Zebrafish choices correspond to the general rule of decisions in collectives. (A) Focal fish choosing between two sites with different number of zebrafish, separated from the focal fish by glass. (B) Probability of choosing option x for different numbers of zebrafish at sites x and y, nx and ny. Theoretical probabilities for a = 11.2 and s = 5, and k = 0 in Eq. 3 represented as a surface and experimental data represented as dots indicating the mean value of typically 15 animals at each configuration. Different dot colors correspond to different values of ny and bars are SEM. (C) Same as B but plotted only as a function of nx and different colors representing the value of ny.

The close match between experimental data and the decision-making model supports that zebrafish behavior corresponds to probabilistic estimations about the quality of sites using social information. However, the processing steps made by the fish brain need not have a one-to-one correspondence with the computational steps in the theory. Instead, a likely option is that zebrafish use simple behavioral rules that approximate good estimations. We found mechanistic models with simple probabilistic attraction rules for individual fish that approximate well the decision-making model and the data (Figs. S3 and S4).

The second case we consider has parameter k in the range from 0 to 1. For this range, the estimation that x is a good option increases with how many animals have already chosen x and decreases, although at a slower rate, with how many have chosen option y. This situation might be common, for example, in food search. Animals choosing one option can indicate that there is a food source in that direction, but also that there might not be a food source at the other option. In this case, the probability of choosing x has a plateau in which both options are equally likely, but increasing the number of animals that have chosen x, nx, reaches a transition region of rapid increase in probability (Fig. 1B). This transition region follows a straight line of slope k in the probability plot (Fig. 1B). This line obeys for high number of animals that Graphic. This is a Weber law (23, 24), according to which the just-noticeable difference between two groups is proportional to the total number of individuals. Indeed, if we substitute Graphic into Graphic we obtain a constant of value Graphic. A second prediction of the model is that decisions should deviate from Weber behavior at low numbers (below the transition point τ in Fig. 1B).

We have found that decisions made by the Argentine ant (L. humile) correspond to the case Graphic. Ants′ choices to turn left and right have been recorded by Perna et al. (24), and we found that they have choice probabilities well described by Eq. 3, except that experimental probabilities do not reach values as close to 0 or 1 as the theory. This difference might be due simply to the fact that ants are not always making turn decisions based on pheromones, but responding to other factors, such as roughness of terrain or collisions with other ants. We therefore considered that ants choose at random with a given probability and otherwise make a decision according to Eq. 3 (Eq. 4). This modification only introduces an overall rescaling in the probabilities, so all structural features described below are present in Eq. 3 (Fig. S5). We obtain a good correspondence with data for high (Fig. 3A) and low numbers of animals (Fig. 3B) with a fit that is robust (Fig. S6). The experimental data are smoother than the theory, without a central plateau, but still with a close correspondence, as also shown in the following analysis. According to Weber’s law, isoprobability curves should be horizontal lines in the Graphic vs. Graphic plane, and this is true both for the theory and experiments for high numbers of total animals N (Fig. 3C). The advantage of this plot is that it magnifies the region of low N, where the data deviate from Weber’s law similarly to the theoretical prediction. A further quantitative analysis revealing the close correspondence between theory and data are shown in Fig. 3D. We performed a linear fit to the experimental probability along the lines of constant nx + ny depicted in Fig. 3D Inset. The slope of each linear fit was then plotted against the total number of animals Graphic (Fig. 3D, blue dots). The experimental data has a very close correspondence with the theoretical values in this plot (Fig. 3D, red line). For a high number of animals, both theory and data show Weber behavior, corresponding in this logarithmic plot to a straight line with slope −1 (Fig. 3D, black line) (24). Interestingly, for low numbers of animals, the theoretical prediction of a deviation from Weber behavior corresponds to the data.

Fig. 3.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Fig. 3.

Ant choices correspond to the general rule of decisions in collectives. (A) Probability of choosing option x as a function of how many ants have previously been at locations x and y, nx and ny, for theory (Left) using Eq. 4 with a = 2.5, s = 1.07, k = 0.53, prand = 0.39, and experiments (Right) from Perna et al. (24). (B) Detail of A. (C) Same as A but represented as a function of Graphic and Graphic. (D) Slope of the probability of choosing x in A as obtained from a linear fit along the lines depicted in Inset. Experimental values (blue dots; error bars are 95% confidence interval), theory (red line), and Weber’s law (black line).

The last case we consider has Graphic, for which Eq. 3 depends only on the variable Graphic. This situation could take place when there is a high probability that only one of the options is good, and those animals choosing x indicate that x may be the good one in a similar way that those choosing y may indicate that x might not be the good one. We have previously shown (22) that the simple decision rule Graphic explains well a large dataset of collective decisions in sticklebacks, G. aculeatus (25, 26). In these experiments, animal groups were made to choose in two-choice setups with different combinations of social and nonsocial information (Fig. 4A, Far Left). Interestingly, Eq. 3 has the simple rule Graphic as a particular case for k = 1 (SI Text). Indeed, all experimental results (blue histograms in Fig. 4A and Fig. S7) are fit using Eq. 3 with parameters s = 2.5, k = 1 (Fig. 4A, red lines). Additionally, for low numbers of animals (up to τ in Fig. 1B), an approximated ΔN rule can also be found for any value of k but with different values of the nonsocial reliability parameter a (SI Text). Therefore, the stickleback data can be fit with any value of k (green and blue lines in Fig. 4A and Fig. S7 for k = 0.5 and k = 0, respectively), with robust fits (Fig. S8). The reason why in this case k can have any value is that its main effect is to control the slope of the boundaries of the plateau of probability 0.5, which is not present in the experimentally explored region of the stickleback dataset (Fig. 4B, white triangle). Still, all these fits have in common an effective ΔN rule for the experimental region (Fig. 4B), giving strong support to this rule in this dataset.

Fig. 4.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Fig. 4.

Stickleback choices correspond to the general rule of decisions in collectives. (A) Probability of finding a final proportion of sticklebacks choosing option x (blue histograms are experimental results from refs. 25 and 26 and theoretical values as lines for k = 1, k = 0.5, and k = 0) for different group sizes (two, four, and eight fish) and for three types of setups: a symmetric setup with different numbers of replica fish going to x and y (Top), a setup with a replica predator at x and different replica fish going to x (Middle), and a symmetric setup with modified replica fish (Bottom). See model parameters and 68 additional experiments with fits in Fig. S7. (B) Theoretical Px for k = 1, a = 1 (Left), k = 0.5, a = 5 (Center), and k = 0, a = 224 (Right), and s = 2.5 in the three cases. All models require an effective ΔN rule to compare with data for the number of animals used in experiments (triangle).

Discussion

Our results support that estimation by the brain using social information to counteract the ambiguity of sensory data is a fundamental principle in collective decision making. The theory explains also the diversity in number discrimination schemes used in collective decisions, including counting up to a given number of animals, counting the difference of animals choosing among options, Graphic, or the relative difference, Graphic, as well as observed deviations from these ideal cases and the existence of different counting regimes for high and low numbers as observed in many species, including humans (47, 48). A single mathematical rule contains all these cases and can be used as a first-principles approach to quantitatively study decisions in animal collectives.

One important ingredient of our theory is the use of probability matching (Eq. 2); for symmetric decisions, it implies a functional form of the type Graphic. Our model in Eq. 3 is a particular case of this function, with Graphic derived from an approximation to Bayesian estimation. Interestingly, many previous approaches derive from the form Graphic (21, 22, 27, 28), which is also a particular case of Graphic, and therefore compatible with probability matching. In other cases, the basic form Graphic has been modified by adding constant terms (29, 30) or an extra function (25), as Graphic, with k a constant when animals have access to a single choice (31, 32, 34). Weber behavior can also be seen as a particular case. It has been previously described using a function (24) that can be expressed as Graphic, with δ between 0 and 1/2. This function obeys Graphic, so in this case Graphic, following Weber behavior.

These previous functions are very useful when applied to particular datasets because they may use few parameters in these conditions. In particular, our previous model (22), a particular case of Eq. 3 (SI Text), used only one parameter in the symmetric experiments with sticklebacks, and a model with two parameters described the ants dataset (24). However, these two models cannot fit the three datasets or even two of them (Fig. S9 A and D and S10). For the zebrafish data in Fig. 2, none of the previously proposed functions (21, 22, 24, 27⇓–29) give a good fit of the plateau in the data (Fig. S9). Our approach has been developed to be applied in very different species and conditions, here tested for three large datasets in three different species. One important factor in this ability to describe different datasets is that our basic function Graphic has a term Graphic that captures how the estimated quality of an option depends not only on the animals choosing that option but also on the animals choosing the other option. These two sources of information are balanced by parameter k, and different datasets are found to correspond to different balances k.

Previous functions describing ant foraging include a constant term that represents a threshold of pheromone concentration below which ants do not react (24, 27, 28). In this way, these functions can describe the deviation from Weber’s law at low pheromone concentration (24). In our case, the theory naturally shows this behavior as one more particular case of the predicted difference between a low and high number of animals. Comparing the two approaches, it is interesting to consider that the behavior for low numbers that is predicted from estimation theory can be achieved in ants using a threshold of pheromone concentration.

An advantage of our approach is that the form of the function f is derived for any type of setup simply from estimation given nonsocial sensory data and the behaviors of others (SI Text). For example, we predict for a symmetric setup with N options a generalization of Eq. 1 of the form

Embedded Image

with Graphic the total number of animals choosing any option except x (see Eq. S10 for the more general case of asymmetric choices).

A further advantage is that the parameters a, s, and k are not only fitting parameters but have expressions, Eqs. S4, S9, and S16, respectively, which give additional predictions. For example, the social reliability parameter is given by

Embedded Image

with β a given animal behavior. This expression means that the social reliability parameter s is higher for a behavior β that is produced with high probability when x is a good option, and with very low probability when it is not a good option. Among all behaviors, those with higher s allow an individual to obtain a higher probability that option x is a good one (Eq. 1), so we expect them to have a larger effect on collective decision making.

Another advantage of an approach based on a theory of estimation is that generalizations of the theoretical expressions can be envisaged deriving models using fewer assumptions. For example, including dependencies in the behaviors of the other individuals and explicit space and time variables should be natural extensions of the theory.

Materials and Methods

Experimental Protocol for Zebrafish.

All procedures met with European Economic Community (EEC) guidelines for animal experiments under Directive 86/609/EEC. Experimental procedures were approved by the Bioethics Subcommittee of Consejo Superior de Investigaciones Científicas. We used wild-type adult zebrafish, D. rerio, of both sexes. Fish were acclimatized to the setup water for 1 d before the experiments (Fig. S11). At 1 h before the experiment, each fish was isolated and fed to ensure uniform nutritional status across individuals. A focal fish entered the setup and swam freely in a central chamber between two social chambers with different number of fish and separated from the choice chamber by glass. Once a fish had been recorded for 5 min it could be placed in one of the lateral chambers as a social stimulus for another fish. The fish in the lateral chambers were interchanged between trials to ensure uniformity, and sides were randomized. The central chamber of the setup was washed between trials to remove odor traces. We computed the probability Px as the fraction of time the focal fish spent on the black region close to one of the social chambers, x. This fraction of time converges to Px for a fish that makes repeated decisions choosing x (y) with probability Px (Py = 1 − Px). A total of 238 fish were tested only once. To test the effect of previous experience, another 233 trials were performed with fish that were tested several times. We found no significant difference between the two groups in the mean times spent at each side (Fig. S12), so all data were pooled for Fig. 2.

Model with Noise Added to the Decision Rule.

The model in Eq. 3 has a good agreement with data from experiments using the Argentine ant, L. humile (24), except that experimental probabilities do not reach values as close to 0 or 1 as the theory. To account for the experimental data, we made a simple modification of the model by assuming that the ant has some probability prand of making the decision at random motivated by unknown factors. Then, with probability (1 − prand), the ant makes the decision according to Eq. 3. Therefore, the probability of turning toward x is

Embedded Image

The parameters that best fit the ant data are a = 2.5, s = 1.07, k = 0.53, and prand = 0.39. This same model can be applied to the zebrafish and stickleback datasets, but in these cases the best fit is obtained for Graphic, which corresponds to Eq. 3.

Analysis of the Ants Dataset.

Both the raw dataset and preprocessing routines were provided by Perna et al. (24), and we used their data assuming no evaporation of pheromone (this assumption does not change the results significantly). We calculated from the data the probability of turning right or left, not a continuous angle, to compare directly to our predicted probabilities. To reduce the noise in the experimental maps of Fig. 3, we symmetrized the data so that the probability shown at point (nx,ny) is obtained as (Px(nx,ny) + (1 − Px(ny,nx)))/2.

Experimental data from Perna et al. (24) measures a quantity that is proportional to the number of ants previously at the left/right of the deciding ant, not directly the numbers, so the number of ants (nx, ny) used in the plots are related to the actual number of ants that count for the decision (nx,true, ny,true) by nx = λ nx,true, ny = λ ny,true, where λ is an unknown proportionality constant. This relation means that the model still applies but with s = strueλ, where strue is the actual value of the reliability parameter.

Asymmetric models.

For the case of sticklebacks deciding in the presence of a predator (Fig. 4A, Middle), we derived the more general asymmetric version of Eq. 1, Eq. S22 (Figs. S13 and S14). We also derived Eq. S23 for the case with two different types of replicas (Fig. 4A, Bottom).

Fitting Procedures.

To fit the model’s parameters to the data, we performed 2D exhaustive searches in the space of parameters. For functions with more than two parameters, we performed the search successively with all possible pairs of parameters. In these cases we repeated the fit several times starting from different initial conditions, always getting the same final result.

Acknowledgments

We thank Andrea Perna for sharing the raw ant data before publication; David Sumpter, Jacques Gautrais, Raphael Jeanson, Matthieu Moreau, and members of G.G.d.P.’s laboratory for fruitful discussions; and two reviewers for constructive comments. Funding for this work was provided by the Ministerio de Ciencia e Innovacion (MICINN, Spain) as Plan Nacional and as partners of the ERASysBio+ initiative (G.G.d.P.) supported under the European Union European Research Area Networks (ERA-NET) Plus scheme in Framework Program 7, including contracts to S.A and A.P.E. A.P.E. is the recipient of a Formación de Profesorado Universitario (FPU) fellowship from MICINN (Spain).

Footnotes

  • ↵1Present address: Centre de Recherches sur la Cognition Animale, Centre National de la Recherche Scientifique Unité Mixte de Recherche 5169, Université Paul Sabatier, 31062, Toulouse, France.

  • ↵2S.A. and A.P-E contributed equally to this work.

  • ↵3To whom correspondence should be addressed. E-mail: gonzalo.polavieja{at}cajal.csic.es.
  • Author contributions: S.A., A.P.-E., and G.G.d.P. designed research, performed research, analyzed data, and wrote the paper.

  • The authors declare no conflict of interest.

  • This article is a PNAS Direct Submission.

  • This article contains supporting information online at www.pnas.org/lookup/suppl/doi:10.1073/pnas.1210664109/-/DCSupplemental.

References

  1. ↵
    1. Knill DC,
    2. Pouget A
    (2004) The Bayesian brain: The role of uncertainty in neural coding and computation. Trends Neurosci 27(12):712–719.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  2. ↵
    1. Gold JI,
    2. Shadlen MN
    (2007) The neural basis of decision making. Annu Rev Neurosci 30:535–574.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  3. ↵
    1. Tenenbaum JB,
    2. Kemp C,
    3. Griffiths TL,
    4. Goodman ND
    (2011) How to grow a mind: Statistics, structure, and abstraction. Science 331(6022):1279–1285.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  4. ↵
    1. Biernaskie JM,
    2. Walker SC,
    3. Gegear RJ
    (2009) Bumblebees learn to forage like Bayesians. Am Nat 174(3):413–423.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  5. ↵
    1. McNamara JM,
    2. Green RF,
    3. Olsson O
    (2006) Bayes theorem and its applications in animal behaviour. Oikos 112(2):243–251.
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  6. ↵
    1. Banerjee AV
    (1992) A simple model of herd behavior. Q J Econ 107(3):797–817.
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  7. ↵
    1. Bikhchandani S,
    2. Hirshleifer D,
    3. Welch I
    (1992) A theory of fads, fashion, custom, and cultural change as informational cascades. J Polit Econ 100(5):992–1026.
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  8. ↵
    1. Valone TJ,
    2. Templeton JJ
    (2002) Public information for the assessment of quality: A widespread social phenomenon. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci 357(1427):1549–1557.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  9. ↵
    1. Giraldeau LA,
    2. Valone TJ,
    3. Templeton JJ
    (2002) Potential disadvantages of using socially acquired information. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci 357(1427):1559–1566.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  10. ↵
    1. Wagner RH,
    2. Danchin E
    (2010) A taxonomy of biological information. Oikos 119(2):203–209.
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  11. ↵
    1. Clark C,
    2. Mangel M
    (1986) The evolutionary advantages of group foraging. Theor Popul Biol 30(1):45–75.
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  12. ↵
    1. Coolen I,
    2. van Bergen Y,
    3. Day RL,
    4. Laland KN
    (2003) Species difference in adaptive use of public information in sticklebacks. Proc Biol Sci 270(1531):2413–2419.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  13. ↵
    1. van Bergen Y,
    2. Coolen I,
    3. Laland KN
    (2004) Nine-spined sticklebacks exploit the most reliable source when public and private information conflict. Proc Biol Sci 271(1542):957–962.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  14. ↵
    1. Rieucau G,
    2. Giraldeau LA
    (2009) Persuasive companions can be wrong: The use of misleading social information in nutmeg mannikins. Behav Ecol 20(6):1217–1222.
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  15. ↵
    1. Lima SL
    (1995) Collective detection of predatory attack by social foragers: Fraught with ambiguity? Anim Behav 50(4):1097–1108.
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  16. ↵
    1. Nordell SE,
    2. Valone TJ
    (1998) Mate choice copying as public information. Ecol Lett 1(2):74–76.
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  17. ↵
    1. Magurran AE,
    2. Higham A
    (2010) Information transfer across fish shoals under predation threat. Ethology 78(2):153–158.
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  18. ↵
    1. Danchin E,
    2. Giraldeau LA,
    3. Valone TJ,
    4. Wagner RH
    (2004) Public information: From nosy neighbors to cultural evolution. Science 305(5683):487–491.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  19. ↵
    1. Dall SRX,
    2. Giraldeau LA,
    3. Olsson O,
    4. McNamara JM,
    5. Stephens DW
    (2005) Information and its use by animals in evolutionary ecology. Trends Ecol Evol 20(4):187–193.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  20. ↵
    1. Dugatkin LA
    (1992) Sexual selection and imitation: Females copy the mate choice of others. Am Nat 139(6):1384–1389.
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  21. ↵
    1. Ward AJW,
    2. Herbert-Read JE,
    3. Sumpter DJT,
    4. Krause J
    (2011) Fast and accurate decisions through collective vigilance in fish shoals. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 108(6):2312–2315.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  22. ↵
    1. Pérez-Escudero A,
    2. de Polavieja GG
    (2011) Collective animal behavior from Bayesian estimation and probability matching. PLOS Comput Biol 7(11):e1002282.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  23. ↵
    1. Gómez-Laplaza LM,
    2. Gerlai R
    (2011) Can angelfish (Pterophyllum scalare) count? Discrimination between different shoal sizes follows Weber’s law. Anim Cogn 14(1):1–9.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  24. ↵
    1. Perna A,
    2. et al.
    (2012) Individual rules for trail pattern formation in Argentine ants (Linepithema humile) PLOS Comput Biol 8(7):e1002592.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  25. ↵
    1. Ward AJW,
    2. Sumpter DJT,
    3. Couzin ID,
    4. Hart PJB,
    5. Krause J
    (2008) Quorum decision-making facilitates information transfer in fish shoals. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 105(19):6948–6953.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  26. ↵
    1. Sumpter DJT,
    2. Krause J,
    3. James R,
    4. Couzin ID,
    5. Ward AJW
    (2008) Consensus decision making by fish. Curr Biol 18(22):1773–1777.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  27. ↵
    1. Goss S,
    2. Aron S,
    3. Deneubourg JL,
    4. Pastels JM
    (1989) Self-organized shortcuts in the Argentine ant. Naturwissenschaften 76(12):579–581.
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  28. ↵
    1. Deneubourg J,
    2. Aron S,
    3. Goss S,
    4. Pasteels J
    (1990) The self-organizing exploratory pattern of the argentine ant. J Insect Behav 3(2):159–168.
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  29. ↵
    1. Meunier H,
    2. Leca JB,
    3. Deneubourg JL,
    4. Petit O
    (2006) Group movement decisions in capuchin monkeys: The utility of an experimental study and a mathematical model to explore the relationship between individual and collective behaviours. Behaviour 143(12):1511–1527.
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  30. ↵
    1. Tero A,
    2. Kobayashi R,
    3. Nakagaki T
    (2007) A mathematical model for adaptive transport network in path finding by true slime mold. J Theor Biol 244(4):553–564.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  31. ↵
    1. Gallup AC,
    2. et al.
    (2012) Visual attention and the acquisition of information in human crowds. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 109(19):7245–7250.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  32. ↵
    1. Sumpter DJT,
    2. Pratt SC
    (2009) Quorum responses and consensus decision making. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci 364(1518):743–753.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  33. ↵
    1. Jeanson R,
    2. et al.
    (2005) Self-organized aggregation in cockroaches. Anim Behav 69(1):169–180.
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  34. ↵
    1. Bousquet CAH,
    2. Sumpter DJT,
    3. Manser MB
    (2011) Moving calls: A vocal mechanism underlying quorum decisions in cohesive groups. Proc R Soc B Biol Sci 278(1711):1482–1488.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  35. ↵
    1. Behrend ER,
    2. Bitterman ME
    (1961) Probability-matching in the fish. Am J Psychol 74(4):542–551.
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  36. ↵
    1. Greggers U,
    2. Menzel R
    (1993) Memory dynamics and foraging strategies of honeybees. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 32(1):17–29.
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  37. ↵
    1. Kirk KL,
    2. Bitterman ME
    (1965) Probability-learning by the turtle. Science 148(3676):1484–1485.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  38. ↵
    1. Wozny DR,
    2. Beierholm UR,
    3. Shams L
    (2010) Probability matching as a computational strategy used in perception. PLOS Comput Biol 6(8):7.
    OpenUrl
  39. ↵
    1. Fretwell S,
    2. Lucas H
    (1969) On territorial behavior and other factors influencing habitat distribution in birds. Acta Biotheor 19(1):16–36.
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  40. ↵
    1. Houston A,
    2. McNamara J
    (1987) Switching between resources and the ideal free distribution. Anim Behav 35(1):301–302.
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  41. ↵
    1. Vulkan N
    (2000) An economist’s perspective on probability matching. J Econ Surv 14(1):101–118.
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  42. ↵
    1. Staddon J
    (1983) Adaptive Behavior and Learning (Cambridge Univ Press, Cambridge, UK).
  43. ↵
    1. Gaissmaier W,
    2. Schooler LJ
    (2008) The smart potential behind probability matching. Cognition 109(3):416–422.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  44. ↵
    1. Green CS,
    2. Benson C,
    3. Kersten D,
    4. Schrater P
    (2010) Alterations in choice behavior by manipulations of world model. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 107(37):16401–16406.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  45. ↵
    1. Engeszer RE,
    2. Ryan MJ,
    3. Parichy DM
    (2004) Learned social preference in zebrafish. Curr Biol 14(10):881–884.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  46. ↵
    1. Wright D,
    2. Krause J
    (2006) Repeated measures of shoaling tendency in zebrafish (Danio rerio) and other small teleost fishes. Nat Protoc 1(4):1828–1831.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  47. ↵
    1. Feigenson L,
    2. Dehaene S,
    3. Spelke E
    (2004) Core systems of number. Trends Cogn Sci 8(7):307–314.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  48. ↵
    1. Dehaene S,
    2. Izard V,
    3. Spelke E,
    4. Pica P
    (2008) Log or linear? Distinct intuitions of the number scale in Western and Amazonian indigene cultures. Science 320(5880):1217–1220.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
View Abstract
PreviousNext
Back to top
Article Alerts
Email Article

Thank you for your interest in spreading the word on PNAS.

NOTE: We only request your email address so that the person you are recommending the page to knows that you wanted them to see it, and that it is not junk mail. We do not capture any email address.

Enter multiple addresses on separate lines or separate them with commas.
A common rule for decision making in animal collectives across species
(Your Name) has sent you a message from PNAS
(Your Name) thought you would like to see the PNAS web site.
Citation Tools
General rule for collective decisions
Sara Arganda, Alfonso Pérez-Escudero, Gonzalo G. de Polavieja
Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences Dec 2012, 109 (50) 20508-20513; DOI: 10.1073/pnas.1210664109

Citation Manager Formats

  • BibTeX
  • Bookends
  • EasyBib
  • EndNote (tagged)
  • EndNote 8 (xml)
  • Medlars
  • Mendeley
  • Papers
  • RefWorks Tagged
  • Ref Manager
  • RIS
  • Zotero
Request Permissions
Share
General rule for collective decisions
Sara Arganda, Alfonso Pérez-Escudero, Gonzalo G. de Polavieja
Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences Dec 2012, 109 (50) 20508-20513; DOI: 10.1073/pnas.1210664109
del.icio.us logo Digg logo Reddit logo Twitter logo CiteULike logo Facebook logo Google logo Mendeley logo
  • Tweet Widget
  • Facebook Like
  • Mendeley logo Mendeley
Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences: 116 (49)
Current Issue

Submit

Sign up for Article Alerts

Article Classifications

  • Biological Sciences
  • Ecology

Jump to section

  • Article
    • Abstract
    • Results
    • Discussion
    • Materials and Methods
    • Acknowledgments
    • Footnotes
    • References
  • Figures & SI
  • Info & Metrics
  • PDF

You May Also be Interested in

Modulating the body's networks could become mainstream therapy for many health issues. Image credit: The Feinstein Institutes for Medicine Research.
Core Concept: The rise of bioelectric medicine sparks interest among researchers, patients, and industry
Modulating the body's networks could become mainstream therapy for many health issues.
Image credit: The Feinstein Institutes for Medicine Research.
Adaptations in heart structure and function likely enabled endurance and survival in preindustrial humans. Image courtesy of Pixabay/Skeeze.
Human heart evolved for endurance
Adaptations in heart structure and function likely enabled endurance and survival in preindustrial humans.
Image courtesy of Pixabay/Skeeze.
Viscoelastic carrier fluids enhance retention of fire retardants on wildfire-prone vegetation. Image courtesy of Jesse D. Acosta.
Viscoelastic fluids and wildfire prevention
Viscoelastic carrier fluids enhance retention of fire retardants on wildfire-prone vegetation.
Image courtesy of Jesse D. Acosta.
Water requirements may make desert bird declines more likely in a warming climate. Image courtesy of Sean Peterson (photographer).
Climate change and desert bird collapse
Water requirements may make desert bird declines more likely in a warming climate.
Image courtesy of Sean Peterson (photographer).
QnAs with NAS member and plant biologist Sheng Yang He. Image courtesy of Sheng Yang He.
Featured QnAs
QnAs with NAS member and plant biologist Sheng Yang He
Image courtesy of Sheng Yang He.

Similar Articles

Site Logo
Powered by HighWire
  • Submit Manuscript
  • Twitter
  • Facebook
  • RSS Feeds
  • Email Alerts

Articles

  • Current Issue
  • Latest Articles
  • Archive

PNAS Portals

  • Classics
  • Front Matter
  • Teaching Resources
  • Anthropology
  • Chemistry
  • Physics
  • Sustainability Science

Information

  • Authors
  • Editorial Board
  • Reviewers
  • Press
  • Site Map
  • PNAS Updates

Feedback    Privacy/Legal

Copyright © 2019 National Academy of Sciences. Online ISSN 1091-6490