Skip to main content

Main menu

  • Home
  • Articles
    • Current
    • Special Feature Articles - Most Recent
    • Special Features
    • Colloquia
    • Collected Articles
    • PNAS Classics
    • List of Issues
  • Front Matter
    • Front Matter Portal
    • Journal Club
  • News
    • For the Press
    • This Week In PNAS
    • PNAS in the News
  • Podcasts
  • Authors
    • Information for Authors
    • Editorial and Journal Policies
    • Submission Procedures
    • Fees and Licenses
  • Submit
  • Submit
  • About
    • Editorial Board
    • PNAS Staff
    • FAQ
    • Accessibility Statement
    • Rights and Permissions
    • Site Map
  • Contact
  • Journal Club
  • Subscribe
    • Subscription Rates
    • Subscriptions FAQ
    • Open Access
    • Recommend PNAS to Your Librarian

User menu

  • Log in
  • My Cart

Search

  • Advanced search
Home
Home
  • Log in
  • My Cart

Advanced Search

  • Home
  • Articles
    • Current
    • Special Feature Articles - Most Recent
    • Special Features
    • Colloquia
    • Collected Articles
    • PNAS Classics
    • List of Issues
  • Front Matter
    • Front Matter Portal
    • Journal Club
  • News
    • For the Press
    • This Week In PNAS
    • PNAS in the News
  • Podcasts
  • Authors
    • Information for Authors
    • Editorial and Journal Policies
    • Submission Procedures
    • Fees and Licenses
  • Submit
Commentary

Napping helps preschoolers learn

Sara C. Mednick

See allHide authors and affiliations

PNAS October 22, 2013 110 (43) 17171-17172; https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1316489110
Sara C. Mednick
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
  • For correspondence: smednick@ucr.edu
  • Article
  • Figures & SI
  • Info & Metrics
  • PDF
Loading

A great deal of research supports the claim that sleep benefits cognitive function in adults. However, evidence-based research on how much sleep children need for healthy cognitive development is scarce. Despite this lack of clarity, certain facts appear consistent across studies. First, recommendations for sleep need have fluctuated throughout history, with children always sleeping less than the experts recommend. Second, children are sleeping less now than ever before (1). Without targeted research on the impact of sleep on cognitive development, experts are unable to make informed recommendations for school start times, bedtimes, and nap times.

Napping is the core of infant sleep and remains an essential part of many children’s sleep diet until they begin kindergarten. PNAS now presents a unique study by Kurdziel et al. to illuminate what may be one of the primary functions of daytime napping in preschool-aged children: memory consolidation (2). Preschoolers were trained in the morning on a visuospatial memory task similar to the game “Memory,” and then either napped or played quietly without napping. Recall was tested 30 min after the nap and the following morning, 24 h posttraining. The authors found that when children napped they showed greater conservation of knowledge both in the postnap and 24-h recall conditions, whereas there was significant long-term forgetting when they did not nap. These data are unique in clearly indicating napping as a valuable resource for memory consolidation in preschool-aged children.

Numerous studies demonstrate positive effects of sleep on memory consolidation in adults (3). Although seldom researched, similar results have been shown across younger age groups. Infants who nap within 4 h of training on an artificial language show generalization of language rules compared with infants who stayed awake (4). On a visuospatial task similar to the one used by Kurdziel et al. (2), performance improvement following sleep in 6- to 8-y-olds was comparable to adults (5). In older children (9- to 12-y-old), a period of sleep following training on a list of word-pairs improved recall more so than an equivalent period of wakefulness (6). In fact, in these older children memory was improved after sleep regardless of whether the sleep period immediately followed training or came after 12 h of wakefulness. Taken together with studies of younger children, these findings indicate that as children age, they can afford increasingly longer wake windows between initial learning and sleep-dependent memory consolidation. This hypothesis is consistent with a recent study showing a negative correlation between vocabulary and attention span and nap frequency in toddlers (7).

In both children and adults, the quality of sleep (i.e., minutes of sleep stages and quantity of sleep features) is correlated with the magnitude of memory improvement. In adults, nonrapid eye movement (non-REM) sleep yields less forgetting than a comparable period of REM sleep or waking activity (8). One electrophysiological feature of non-REM sleep, the sleep spindle, has recently gained attention for its role in hippocampal-cortical communication (9) in rodents and declarative memory consolidation during sleep in humans (10). Furthermore, experimentally increasing sleep spindles in a daytime nap improved verbal memory performance compared with control conditions (11). In young adults, retention of word-pairs across a sleep interval was correlated with the minutes in non-REM sleep (6), and improvement in visuospatial memory in the present study was correlated with the number of stage 2 sleep spindles in toddlers’ naps (2). These results are consistent with the notion that sleep spindles orchestrate the consolidation of new declarative memories similarly across different ages.

Importantly, napping is not universal across preschoolers. Between the ages of 2- and 6-y-old, most children give up napping because of biological and social pressures (12). Until now, no studies have examined differences in sleep-dependent memory consolidation between habitual and nonhabitual napping children. Intriguingly, Kurdziel et al. (2) found that the deficits in the no-nap condition were solely because of the habitual nappers. That is, nonnappers who remained awake maintained memory performance equivalent to that of habitual and nonnappers who napped. In contrast, massive forgetting was seen when habitual nappers were prevented from napping. The authors posited that one possible reason for this difference is brain maturation; that is, more neurologically mature children are able to withstand longer wake windows without forgetting newly learned information, compared with less mature children who require sleep within a shorter window following learning. Consistent with this hypothesis, studies show that children maintain a constant total sleep time across their transitions from napping to not napping, indicating a stability in sleep requirements along with tolerance of longer wake periods (7). This relationship between brain maturity, wake windows, and sleep-dependent learning may be an important factor contributing to the transition from polyphasic napping habits in infants, to the consolidation of sleep into a biphasic pattern in preschool children, and to the eventual monophasic pattern in older children (Fig. 1).

Fig. 1.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Fig. 1.

A map of nocturnal sleep and naps from infancy to older adulthood. Each circle represents an age group, with colored areas showing nocturnal and daytime sleep periods. (1) Infant polyphasic sleep is shown in the innermost circle in purple. (2) After the first year of life, children (light blue) begin to sleep through the night and daytime sleep consolidates into two naps. (3) Toddlers (green) have later bedtimes and only one (or no) afternoon nap. (4) Children between 6 and 12 y of age (orange) maintain a steady sleep duration between 8 and 10 h at night, with decreased night wakings and infrequent naps (20). (5) A marked delay in sleep patterns is a normal feature of adolescent development (brown), with later bedtimes in postpubertal adolescents. Because of social pressures, including early school start times, sleep can be severely curtailed and many teenagers nap after (and during) school. (6) Adults (red) usually get 6–8 h of sleep, with 30% taking short naps during the afternoon. (7) Older adults experience sleep-phase advancement with earlier bedtimes and wake times and frequent short naps during the day.

These results may also be unique evidence that napping serves a different purpose in children and adults. Thirty-percent of healthy adults endorse appetitive napping, or napping for pleasure. Several studies have reported that habitual appetitive nappers remain in lighter sleep (stages 1 and 2), have more fluctuations between light sleep stages, and feel more refreshed upon waking from a nap compared with nonnappers (13). Furthermore, increased slow-wave sleep in nonnappers likely contributes to postnap grogginess (i.e., sleep inertia) reported in this group. Habitual nappers also gain more performance benefits from napping than nonnappers. Milner et al. reported that habitual nappers performed better in a motor learning task, and performance gains were correlated with stage 2 sleep spindles, compared with nonnappers (14). Thus, adults may choose to nap because of the extra boost in alertness and performance enhancements, whereas napping in children may be related to developmental limitations in cognitive abilities. Along these lines, it would be informative to know, do nap habits from childhood transfer to adulthood? Are there biological/genetic differences between habitual and nonnappers? Or is appetitive napping a learned, trainable skill? Further research into the biological basis of napping will shed light on this topic.

Along with cognitive development, sleep, and perhaps napping, in the early years have surprisingly long-term consequences for adolescent and adult health and well-being. Short sleep duration in children has been associated with higher body mass index (BMI) 5 y after baseline (15). In addition, children with short sleep durations had significantly higher BMIs at 32 y of age, compared with the adult BMIs of children with moderate and long sleep durations (16). Furthermore, young teenagers with poor preschool sleep habits were more than twice as likely to use drugs, tobacco, or alcohol 10 y later (17). Inadequate sleep has also been raised as a possible contributor to childhood attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD). In one review, children with ADHD were sleepier during the day, snored more, had higher apnea-hypopnea index, and more restless sleep (18). In fact, a comparison between treatment of ADHD with a commonly prescribed ADHD drug and adenotonsillectomy found decreased ADHD scores in both treatment groups, compared with no treatment. However, along with improved sleep, the surgical group had better impulse control, response time, and total ADHD score, as well as lower hyperactivity and inattention subscales than the drug group (19). Unfortunately, none of these studies considered the effect of napping on present and future health outcomes; however, the current findings of the benefits of preschool napping on memory urge such a comparison.

Practically speaking, how does this new information on napping figure into expert recommendations for sleep schedules in schools? Currently, such data do not appear to be considered, as many schools are making the decision to eliminate naps from preschool in favor of increased active learning periods. These no-nap policies began at the same time as—and perhaps in response to—public initiatives to reward schools that have higher standardized test scores and an increased emphasis on preschool readiness. Without scientific research addressing the importance of napping for cognitive development, many school administrators across America have been forced to make the choice between giving children a period for sleep, versus for study. In light of these changes in preschool curriculum, Kurdziel et al.’s (2) findings of the essential nature of the nap for children, especially habitual-nappers, are timely and have the potential to affect educational standards. Given the individual differences in nap need, an updated framework might move away from the one-size-fits-all approach to sleep schedules, and consider each child’s sleep need individually. Given that depriving a child of a nap can have dire consequences for his or her long-term memory and future test performance, addressing these issues appears to be scientifically justified.

Acknowledgments

I would like to thank William A. Alaynick for figure design, and Elizabeth McDevitt and Lauren Whitehurst for edits.

Footnotes

  • ↵1E-mail: smednick{at}ucr.edu.
  • Author contributions: S.C.M. wrote the paper.

  • The author declares no conflict of interest.

  • See companion article on page 17267.

References

  1. ↵
    1. Matricciani LA,
    2. Olds TS,
    3. Blunden S,
    4. Rigney G,
    5. Williams MT
    (2012) Never enough sleep: A brief history of sleep recommendations for children. Pediatrics 129(3):548–556.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  2. ↵
    1. Kurdziel L,
    2. Duclos K,
    3. Spencer RMC
    (2013) Sleep spindles in midday naps enhance learning in preschool children. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 110:17267–17272.
    OpenUrl
  3. ↵
    1. Mednick SC,
    2. Cai DJ,
    3. Shuman T,
    4. Anagnostaras S,
    5. Wixted JT
    (2011) An opportunistic theory of cellular and systems consolidation. Trends Neurosci 34(10):504–514.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  4. ↵
    1. Gómez RL,
    2. Bootzin RR,
    3. Nadel L
    (2006) Naps promote abstraction in language-learning infants. Psychol Sci 17(8):670–674.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  5. ↵
    1. Wilhelm I,
    2. Diekelmann S,
    3. Born J
    (2008) Sleep in children improves memory performance on declarative but not procedural tasks. Learn Mem 15(5):373–377.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  6. ↵
    1. Backhaus J,
    2. Hoeckesfeld R,
    3. Born J,
    4. Hohagen F,
    5. Junghanns K
    (2008) Immediate as well as delayed post learning sleep but not wakefulness enhances declarative memory consolidation in children. Neurobiol Learn Mem 89(1):76–80.
    OpenUrlPubMed
  7. ↵
    1. Lam JC,
    2. Mahone EM,
    3. Mason T,
    4. Scharf SM
    (2011) The effects of napping on cognitive function in preschoolers. J Dev Behav Pediatr 32(2):90–97.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  8. ↵
    1. Plihal W,
    2. Born J
    (1997) Effects of early and late nocturnal sleep on declarative and procedural memory. J Cogn Neurosci 9(4):534–547.
    OpenUrl
  9. ↵
    1. Eschenko O,
    2. Mölle M,
    3. Born J,
    4. Sara SJ
    (2006) Elevated sleep spindle density after learning or after retrieval in rats. J Neurosci 26(50):12914–12920.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  10. ↵
    1. Clemens Z,
    2. Fabó D,
    3. Halász P
    (2005) Overnight verbal memory retention correlates with the number of sleep spindles. Neuroscience 132(2):529–535.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  11. ↵
    1. Mednick SC,
    2. et al.
    (2013) The critical role of sleep spindles in hippocampal-dependent memory: A pharmacology study. J Neurosci 33(10):4494–4504.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  12. ↵
    1. Weissbluth M
    (1995) Naps in children: 6 months-7 years. Sleep 18(2):82–87.
    OpenUrlPubMed
  13. ↵
    1. McDevitt EA,
    2. Alaynick WA,
    3. Mednick SC
    (2012) The effect of nap frequency on daytime sleep architecture. Physiol Behav 107(1):40–44.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  14. ↵
    1. Milner CE,
    2. Fogel SM,
    3. Cote KA
    (2006) Habitual napping moderates motor performance improvements following a short daytime nap. Biol Psychol 73(2):141–156.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  15. ↵
    1. Snell EK,
    2. Adam EK,
    3. Duncan GJ
    (2007) Sleep and the body mass index and overweight status of children and adolescents. Child Dev 78(1):309–323.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  16. ↵
    1. Landhuis CE,
    2. Poulton R,
    3. Welch D,
    4. Hancox RJ
    (2008) Childhood sleep time and long-term risk for obesity: A 32-year prospective birth cohort study. Pediatrics 122(5):955–960.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  17. ↵
    1. Wong MM,
    2. Brower KJ,
    3. Zucker RA
    (2009) Childhood sleep problems, early onset of substance use and behavioral problems in adolescence. Sleep Med 10(7):787–796.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  18. ↵
    1. Cortese S,
    2. Konofal E,
    3. Yateman N,
    4. Mouren MC,
    5. Lecendreux M
    (2006) Sleep and alertness in children with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: A systematic review of the literature. Sleep 29(4):504–511.
    OpenUrlPubMed
  19. ↵
    1. Huang YS,
    2. et al.
    (2007) Attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder with obstructive sleep apnea: A treatment outcome study. Sleep Med 8(1):18–30.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  20. ↵
    1. Galland BC,
    2. Taylor BJ,
    3. Elder DE,
    4. Herbison P
    (2012) Normal sleep patterns in infants and children: A systematic review of observational studies. Sleep Med Rev 16(3):213–222.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
PreviousNext
Back to top
Article Alerts
Email Article

Thank you for your interest in spreading the word on PNAS.

NOTE: We only request your email address so that the person you are recommending the page to knows that you wanted them to see it, and that it is not junk mail. We do not capture any email address.

Enter multiple addresses on separate lines or separate them with commas.
Napping helps preschoolers learn
(Your Name) has sent you a message from PNAS
(Your Name) thought you would like to see the PNAS web site.
CAPTCHA
This question is for testing whether or not you are a human visitor and to prevent automated spam submissions.
Citation Tools
Napping helps preschoolers learn
Sara C. Mednick
Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences Oct 2013, 110 (43) 17171-17172; DOI: 10.1073/pnas.1316489110

Citation Manager Formats

  • BibTeX
  • Bookends
  • EasyBib
  • EndNote (tagged)
  • EndNote 8 (xml)
  • Medlars
  • Mendeley
  • Papers
  • RefWorks Tagged
  • Ref Manager
  • RIS
  • Zotero
Request Permissions
Share
Napping helps preschoolers learn
Sara C. Mednick
Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences Oct 2013, 110 (43) 17171-17172; DOI: 10.1073/pnas.1316489110
del.icio.us logo Digg logo Reddit logo Twitter logo CiteULike logo Facebook logo Google logo Mendeley logo
  • Tweet Widget
  • Facebook Like
  • Mendeley logo Mendeley

Article Classifications

  • Social Sciences
  • Psychological and Cognitive Sciences

See related content:

  • Nap-dependent learning in preschool children
    - Sep 23, 2013
Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences: 110 (43)
Table of Contents

Submit

Sign up for Article Alerts

Jump to section

  • Article
    • Acknowledgments
    • Footnotes
    • References
  • Figures & SI
  • Info & Metrics
  • PDF

You May Also be Interested in

Water from a faucet fills a glass.
News Feature: How “forever chemicals” might impair the immune system
Researchers are exploring whether these ubiquitous fluorinated molecules might worsen infections or hamper vaccine effectiveness.
Image credit: Shutterstock/Dmitry Naumov.
Reflection of clouds in the still waters of Mono Lake in California.
Inner Workings: Making headway with the mysteries of life’s origins
Recent experiments and simulations are starting to answer some fundamental questions about how life came to be.
Image credit: Shutterstock/Radoslaw Lecyk.
Cave in coastal Kenya with tree growing in the middle.
Journal Club: Small, sharp blades mark shift from Middle to Later Stone Age in coastal Kenya
Archaeologists have long tried to define the transition between the two time periods.
Image credit: Ceri Shipton.
Illustration of groups of people chatting
Exploring the length of human conversations
Adam Mastroianni and Daniel Gilbert explore why conversations almost never end when people want them to.
Listen
Past PodcastsSubscribe
Panda bear hanging in a tree
How horse manure helps giant pandas tolerate cold
A study finds that giant pandas roll in horse manure to increase their cold tolerance.
Image credit: Fuwen Wei.

Similar Articles

Site Logo
Powered by HighWire
  • Submit Manuscript
  • Twitter
  • Facebook
  • RSS Feeds
  • Email Alerts

Articles

  • Current Issue
  • Special Feature Articles – Most Recent
  • List of Issues

PNAS Portals

  • Anthropology
  • Chemistry
  • Classics
  • Front Matter
  • Physics
  • Sustainability Science
  • Teaching Resources

Information

  • Authors
  • Editorial Board
  • Reviewers
  • Subscribers
  • Librarians
  • Press
  • Cozzarelli Prize
  • Site Map
  • PNAS Updates
  • FAQs
  • Accessibility Statement
  • Rights & Permissions
  • About
  • Contact

Feedback    Privacy/Legal

Copyright © 2021 National Academy of Sciences. Online ISSN 1091-6490