Skip to main content
  • Submit
  • About
    • Editorial Board
    • PNAS Staff
    • FAQ
    • Accessibility Statement
    • Rights and Permissions
    • Site Map
  • Contact
  • Journal Club
  • Subscribe
    • Subscription Rates
    • Subscriptions FAQ
    • Open Access
    • Recommend PNAS to Your Librarian
  • Log in
  • My Cart

Main menu

  • Home
  • Articles
    • Current
    • Special Feature Articles - Most Recent
    • Special Features
    • Colloquia
    • Collected Articles
    • PNAS Classics
    • List of Issues
  • Front Matter
  • News
    • For the Press
    • This Week In PNAS
    • PNAS in the News
  • Podcasts
  • Authors
    • Information for Authors
    • Editorial and Journal Policies
    • Submission Procedures
    • Fees and Licenses
  • Submit
  • About
    • Editorial Board
    • PNAS Staff
    • FAQ
    • Accessibility Statement
    • Rights and Permissions
    • Site Map
  • Contact
  • Journal Club
  • Subscribe
    • Subscription Rates
    • Subscriptions FAQ
    • Open Access
    • Recommend PNAS to Your Librarian

User menu

  • Log in
  • My Cart

Search

  • Advanced search
Home
Home

Advanced Search

  • Home
  • Articles
    • Current
    • Special Feature Articles - Most Recent
    • Special Features
    • Colloquia
    • Collected Articles
    • PNAS Classics
    • List of Issues
  • Front Matter
  • News
    • For the Press
    • This Week In PNAS
    • PNAS in the News
  • Podcasts
  • Authors
    • Information for Authors
    • Editorial and Journal Policies
    • Submission Procedures
    • Fees and Licenses

New Research In

Physical Sciences

Featured Portals

  • Physics
  • Chemistry
  • Sustainability Science

Articles by Topic

  • Applied Mathematics
  • Applied Physical Sciences
  • Astronomy
  • Computer Sciences
  • Earth, Atmospheric, and Planetary Sciences
  • Engineering
  • Environmental Sciences
  • Mathematics
  • Statistics

Social Sciences

Featured Portals

  • Anthropology
  • Sustainability Science

Articles by Topic

  • Economic Sciences
  • Environmental Sciences
  • Political Sciences
  • Psychological and Cognitive Sciences
  • Social Sciences

Biological Sciences

Featured Portals

  • Sustainability Science

Articles by Topic

  • Agricultural Sciences
  • Anthropology
  • Applied Biological Sciences
  • Biochemistry
  • Biophysics and Computational Biology
  • Cell Biology
  • Developmental Biology
  • Ecology
  • Environmental Sciences
  • Evolution
  • Genetics
  • Immunology and Inflammation
  • Medical Sciences
  • Microbiology
  • Neuroscience
  • Pharmacology
  • Physiology
  • Plant Biology
  • Population Biology
  • Psychological and Cognitive Sciences
  • Sustainability Science
  • Systems Biology
Research Article

Chaos–order transition in foraging behavior of ants

Lixiang Li, Haipeng Peng, Jürgen Kurths, Yixian Yang, and Hans Joachim Schellnhuber
PNAS June 10, 2014 111 (23) 8392-8397; first published May 27, 2014; https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1407083111
Lixiang Li
aInformation Security Center, State Key Laboratory of Networking and Switching Technology, and
bPotsdam Institute for Climate Impact Research, D14473 Potsdam, Germany; and
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Haipeng Peng
aInformation Security Center, State Key Laboratory of Networking and Switching Technology, and
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
  • For correspondence: kurths@pik-potsdam.de penghaipeng@bupt.edu.cn director@pik-potsdam.de
Jürgen Kurths
bPotsdam Institute for Climate Impact Research, D14473 Potsdam, Germany; and
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
  • For correspondence: kurths@pik-potsdam.de penghaipeng@bupt.edu.cn director@pik-potsdam.de
Yixian Yang
aInformation Security Center, State Key Laboratory of Networking and Switching Technology, and
cNational Engineering Laboratory for Disaster Backup and Recovery, Beijing University of Posts and Telecommunications, Beijing 100876, China;
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Hans Joachim Schellnhuber
bPotsdam Institute for Climate Impact Research, D14473 Potsdam, Germany; and
dSanta Fe Institute, Santa Fe, NM 87501
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
  • For correspondence: kurths@pik-potsdam.de penghaipeng@bupt.edu.cn director@pik-potsdam.de
  1. Contributed by Hans Joachim Schellnhuber, April 24, 2014 (sent for review February 5, 2014)

  • Article
  • Figures & SI
  • Info & Metrics
  • PDF
Loading

Significance

We have studied the foraging behavior of group animals that live in fixed colonies (especially ants) as an important problem in ecology. Building on former findings on deterministic chaotic activities of single ants, we uncovered that the transition from chaotic to periodic regimes results from an optimization scheme of the self-organization of such an animal colony. We found that an effective foraging of ants mainly depends on their nest as well as their physical abilities and knowledge due to experience. As an important outcome, the foraging behavior of ants is not represented by random, but rather by deterministic walks, in a random environment: Ants use their intelligence and experience to navigate.

Abstract

The study of the foraging behavior of group animals (especially ants) is of practical ecological importance, but it also contributes to the development of widely applicable optimization problem-solving techniques. Biologists have discovered that single ants exhibit low-dimensional deterministic-chaotic activities. However, the influences of the nest, ants’ physical abilities, and ants’ knowledge (or experience) on foraging behavior have received relatively little attention in studies of the collective behavior of ants. This paper provides new insights into basic mechanisms of effective foraging for social insects or group animals that have a home. We propose that the whole foraging process of ants is controlled by three successive strategies: hunting, homing, and path building. A mathematical model is developed to study this complex scheme. We show that the transition from chaotic to periodic regimes observed in our model results from an optimization scheme for group animals with a home. According to our investigation, the behavior of such insects is not represented by random but rather deterministic walks (as generated by deterministic dynamical systems, e.g., by maps) in a random environment: the animals use their intelligence and experience to guide them. The more knowledge an ant has, the higher its foraging efficiency is. When young insects join the collective to forage with old and middle-aged ants, it benefits the whole colony in the long run. The resulting strategy can even be optimal.

  • foraging dynamics
  • learning process
  • low-dimensional chaos
  • mathematical modeling
  • synchronization

Both experimental data analysis and mathematical modeling on the foraging behavior of group animals (especially ant colonies) have recently captured much attention due to the high level of self-organizing structures that emerge at the collective level (1⇓⇓⇓–5). Random walking is a widely discussed strategy in the research literature on the foraging behavior of group animals (2, 6⇓–8). Some ecologists maintain that especially Lévy flight schemes can appropriately be used to describe the foraging behavior (6, 7). However, some recent studies have raised doubts whether this is a valid conjecture (2, 8, 9). It is even argued that the rules of locomotion for a walker are always consistent with a purely deterministic model, rather than with a stochastic scheme (9, 10).

On the other hand, in the studies on the foraging behavior of animals, the existence of homes has so far received relatively little attention. Here we argue that the existence of a home or nest influences the foraging process to a large extent. Animals are due to return to their homes because of increasing exhaustion of energy. Moreover each foraging process of an animal is also a learning process. With foraging repetition, long-term memory continues to accumulate, an animal’s knowledge about the environment of its nest gets richer, and the region that the animal is familiar with continues to enlarge. Moreover, animals’ physical ability and knowledge as determined by their age directly influence their foraging strategy. All these factors deserve close attention.

There is already a rich history of research on the foraging behavior of ant colonies (see, e.g., ref. 11). In particular foraging strategies of ants were discussed in the context of solving distributed control and optimization problems. Already 30 y ago, it was proposed that Lévy flights might characterize the behavior of foraging ants (12). In 1990, Deneubourg et al. designed a well-known wide binary bridge experiment which showed that ants could mark the path followed by a trail of pheromone and find an optimal path between the nest and the food source (13). Based on similar experiments, Dorigo and coworkers (3) developed ant colony optimization algorithms which have been used for solving various difficult problems, including combinatorial optimization, object clustering, and routing selection in communication networks. A limited binary bridge experiment was presented to show that ants could even form two lanes to solve traffic flow problems on crowded branches (4).

However, all these experiments were conducted in special man-designed environments, which were not identical to natural ones, so the ants’ free crawling was restricted. It was argued that unrestricted foraging ants might not perform Lévy flights. Moreover, through an experimental study on the dynamical behaviors of an isolated ant and a whole ant colony, Cole (14) discovered that the activity of an ant colony exhibited periodic behavior, whereas the behavior of a single ant showed a low-dimensional deterministic chaotic pattern. In 1993, Solé et al. (15) constructed a 1D chaotic map following Cole to describe the foraging process of an isolated ant. Nemes and Roska (16) designed a cellular neural network model to describe the synchronized oscillating pattern of activity as a result of an array of chaotic dynamic elements placed in a regular 2D grid. In 2006, Li and coworkers (17, 18) developed a chaotic ant swarm model building on Cole’s research to describe the phenomenon that the chaotic behavior of a single ant contributes to the self-organization behavior of a whole ant colony. These models have explained some relationships between the chaotic (or random) strategy, individual dynamics, and group dynamics. However, these studies ignored the possibility that the ants also use their own experience and intelligence to guide their foraging. Hence, further studies on the influences of physical ability, age, and knowledge on foraging behavior are needed to explain the biological behavior of ants in nature.

The aim of this paper is to provide a novel perspective on mechanisms of effective foraging of ant colonies. We assume that the foraging process of these animals is entirely controlled by three successive strategies: hunting, homing, and path building. A mathematical model is developed to understand the whole foraging process. We discuss the influences of the special region around the nest, the size of the food source, the search range, the limitation of ants’ physical ability, and ants’ learning process with respect to foraging behavior. Our analysis suggests that group animals that have a home do not perform random walks, but rather deterministic walks in a random environment. They use their knowledge to guide them and their behavior is also influenced by their physical abilities, their age, and the existence of homes.

Foraging Strategies of Ants

To survive, ants need to leave their nest and forage for food. The survival-of-the-fittest mechanism entails that ants do not only find food, but also an optimal path between their nest and the food source (19, 20). This reflects the collective intelligence of the insects. Nest and food source indeed play important roles in ants’ foraging behavior. The whole foraging process of ants may be described by three strategies: hunting, homing, and path building.

Hunting Strategy.

During the first phase in foraging, some ants of the colony leave the nest to search for food; we call these “scout ants” (21). We assume that there is a food source located in some random environment. We consider the search for food as the search for an unknown point (or region). Thus, initially, no ant knows where the food is or in which direction it should go. In particular, there is no pheromone on the path before the food source is found. From Cole’s observational results on the behavior of individual ants, we know that ants search chaotically around their nest and initially have no impacts on their neighbors. For the details of chaotic and self-organizing ant behavior, see SI Text.

Suppose that initially n ants go out to look for food. These ants forage in a continuous search space S, often denoted Rl, the l-dimensional continuous space of real numbers, i.e., S = Rl. Each ant with a position vector s→i=Z→i=(Zi1,…,Zil) attempts to search for the location P→food=(Pfood1,…,Pfoodl) of a food source by minimizing an object function (cost function) f:S→R, where S = Rl and f ≥ 0, is a map from l dimensions real number to real number, and when Z→i=P→food, f(Z→i)=f(P→food)=0. In the search space S = Rl, the food possesses a small region (i.e., the neighborhood of the point P→food). The bigger the food is, the larger the region is. The values of the function f corresponding to different points in the neighborhood of the food source have an upper bound △, where △ is a relatively small positive number. Here, larger △ means larger region around the point P→food. So the value of △ represents the size of the food source. f(Z→i) < △ means the point Z→i locates in a small neighborhood of the food source, i.e., the ant finds the food.

Fig. 1A provides a schematic diagram of ants crawling in the search space where the black solid point represents the nest and the green triangle the nearest food source. When food is scarce, ants need to go out to forage many times because they must return to the nest to access the stored energy supply. We have developed a map of the nest–food source–nest motion against time. This is sketched in Fig. 1B where the solid dot again represents the nest and the green triangle the food source. The length of the optimal path from the nest to the food source is denoted by L. In this map, ants forage chaotically, setting out at the origin. If they do not find food, then the curve does not pass through the green triangle. Because n ants conduct concurrent searches, there is likely some ant i that quickly finds the food source (Fig. 1B). This hunting strategy, whereby ants leave the nest and return to it, is used throughout the foraging process.

Fig. 1.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Fig. 1.

Stylized presentation of the ant foraging process. The optimized path between the nest (black dot) and the food source (green triangle) has length L. (A) Characteristic search behavior of scout ants distinguished by different path colors. S is the overall foraging space and M the nest’s neighborhood, which the ants are quite familiar with. (B) The foraging motion in S can be mapped onto a simplified trajectory, i.e., a relationship of position vs. time. As detailed in SI Text, time t is normalized in a way that highlights periodicity and is represented by the abscissa in B–D. In the same charts, the ordinate d represents the nearest distance of the respective ant position from the set L. Thus, each foraging excursion appears as a curve segment between two consecutive black dots (home-to-home tour). B describes the situation where n scouts search chaotically in parallel. The ith ant happens to find the food source during its kth tour (as indicated by the passing of the trajectory through the green triangle). (C and D) m-recruited insects are guided by the pheromone traces left by the successful ant i. (C) Initially, the pheromone intensity is rather low, thus its guiding influence on the chaotic ant motion is very small. To find the optimal path, the recruited insects pursue various chaotically selected paths around the route marked by the pheromone from the scout ant i. However, all those paths hit the green triangle because the followers “know” the position of the food source from the pioneer. (D) With increasing pheromone concentration around the optimal path, the chaotic foraging regime is gradually overcome. Eventually the new regime emerges where all ants pursue L to convey food from source to nest. Thus, all trajectories become straight lines.

Homing Strategy.

The existence of a nest has an important influence on the behavior of the ants during the whole search for food. The homing strategy is triggered when a foraging ant makes a decision to return to the nest. The aim of that strategy is to find the home most efficiently. The nest of the ants is denoted by P→nest=(Pnest1,…,Pnestl). Different ants have different knowledge about the environment of the nest, i.e., each ant considers different domains Mi as nest neighborhoods (22). If ant i moves into its nest neighborhood Mi={Z→i:‖Z→i−P→nest‖<ci}, then we assume that it has found the nest (Fig. 1A), where the neighborhood range is determined by the positive constant ci, and a larger ci indicates that the ant has more knowledge about the nest environment. Obviously, older ants have larger ci because they have more knowledge about their nest. Foraging can be considered as searching for an unknown point (or small region), whereas searching for the nest is considered as searching for a special region with which an ant is very familiar. In the homing process, the ants use chaos and pheromone in combination with their own knowledge to search for the nest.

Different ants have different search ranges, different continuing search times, and different search paths. The starting time of the homing strategy is very important. If it is triggered very early, then the probability of ants finding food will be minute; if it happens very late, however, then the ants may not be able to return to the nest because their energy is exhausted. That is, each ant has its own searching time threshold, i.e., the tiring time ttired. When ttired is reached, the ant should start its homing strategy, independent of having found food or not.

Path-Building Strategy.

When a scout ant finds a food source, it begins returning to the nest and leaving pheromone on its return path as a means of communicating to fellow ants the location of the food and appropriate paths (3, 4). Once the scout ant has returned to the nest, it will gather recruited ants to follow its path marked with pheromone. We assume there are m recruits. In addition, there are still some freely walking scout ants looking for food in the search space.

Both the chaotic walking and the pheromone have important impacts on the foraging process of recruited ants. Pheromone influences their choice of path making. Ants encountering a path previously laid with heavy amounts of pheromone are more likely to decide to follow it. Pheromone is an evaporative substance, though. An autocatalytic collective behavior of recruited ants emerges, where the shorter the path, the higher its pheromone density becomes (23). During the initial construction phase of the optimal path, the orchestration between ants is weak, and chaotic movement dominates their behavior because of the very low density of pheromone. Fig. 1C depicts the initial foraging behavior of m recruited ants. This figure illustrates how the ants initially foraged chaotically around the route marked by the pheromone of the scout ant, while leaving more pheromone on their wandering paths. This means that chaotic walking and pheromone interact in the optimization process.

We use the pheromone field concept to help us understand the behavior of the recruited ants. The pheromone field is the cause of self-organization among ants and is used to direct their movements. As time passes, the pheromone left by the recruited ants on their paths increases gradually, and a distinct pheromone field is constructed between the food source and the nest. The reinforcement of pheromone continues to weaken the chaotic behavior of the ants. The evaporation of the pheromone causes the density of pheromone on the shorter path to increase more quickly than on the longer one, which in turn causes more ants to choose the shorter path. The increment of pheromone density on the paths is equivalent to a decrement in the chaotic crawling of insects (SI Text). During the formation of the pheromone field and the finding of the optimal route, the ants still possess some chaotic crawling, which is eventually superseded by the pheromone signals.

Fig. 1D shows the final foraging phase of the m recruited ants. The ants gradually enter a state of ordered periodic motion through self-organization in the colony. Obviously, the chaotic behavior causes the ants to conduct a global search, whereas the pheromone field causes the ants to conduct a local search until they finally converge to periodic movements. In this process, the transformation of dynamical behavior of an ant colony causes the emergence of something that may be called “swarm intelligence.”

Thus, we consider the process by which ants begin their foraging and form their optimal path to be an intelligent process, whereby the status of ants is transformed from an asynchronous chaotic regime into a collective synchronous periodic act. Ants use their intelligence and experience to guide their foraging process. We regard a foraging cycle of ants as the process in which ants leave their nest to find a food source until an optimal path is found and then convey all of the food to their nest. After one foraging cycle is completed, the ants initiate another one to survive, searching for a new food source.

Results

Now we show how the basic principles sketched above are applied. For simplicity, we assume that there is only one food source in the search space and that there is only one optimal route between the food source and the nest. The chaotic model Z′(t+1)=Z′(t)eμ(1−Z′(t)), constructed in ref. 15, is introduced to mimic the random activity of a single ant at the beginning of the foraging process, where Z′∈R+ is a continuous variable and μ a positive constant. When μ=3, the system is in a chaotic state (15, 18).

Next, we take into account the influence of the nest and the food source on the ants’ motion. Here, the organization of ants sets in under the competing influences of the pheromone and the ants’ chaotic crawling. The characteristic variable of chaotic crawling represented by yi(t) is introduced, where 0≤yi(t)<1, and the value indicates the degree of chaotic crawling. A larger y means a higher degree of chaotic crawling. Based on the mechanism of chaotic annealing (24), the continual decrement dynamics of yi(t) is represented by yi(t)=yi(t−1)(1+ri), which depends on the self-organization factor ri (see Methods for details on ri). The movement adjustment of each ant is executed as follows:Zik(t+1)=(Zik(t)+Vk)e(1−e−ayi(t))(3−ψk(Zik(t)+Vk))−Vk+e−2ayi(t)+b(|sin(ωt)|(Pfoodk−Pnestk)−(Zik(t)−Pnestk)),[1]where ψk can adjust the search range, V determines the search region of ant i and accounts for the option that ants can roam diverse realms, ω is used to adjust the frequency of ants’ periodic oscillation between the nest and the food source, a is a sufficiently large positive constant such that the variable yi(t) could have a large enough impact on the position vector Zik, and b is the local search factor which controls the local optimal path strategy. When yi approaches 0, b begins to work, where 0 ≤ b < ln(2). If b ≥ ln(2), the system is unstable. When b = 0, the system is in a periodic oscillatory regime between the nest and the food source without undergoing the process of a local search. When 0 < b < ln(2), the system starts from a transient chaos state and finally converges to a periodic behavior.

Now we analyze and examine the nonlinear dynamics of the proposed chaotic ant foraging model. We use it to solve a concrete optimization problem whose objective function is defined by f(x1,x2)=(x−0.7)2×(0.1+(0.6+x2)2)+(x2−0.5)2×(0.15+(0.4+x1)2), where (0.7,0.5) is the global minimum of the energy function f. To simulate the foraging cycle, we assume that the point (0.7,0.5) is the position of the food source, and (0.4,0.4) is the position of the nest. All of the figures in this section are the results of numerical simulations for Eq. 1.

Food Hunting and Homing Processes.

Ants of different ages have different physical abilities and different knowledge about their nest. The age of the ants thus has a significant impact on their foraging behavior. Fig. S1 displays the foraging probability curves of ants with different ages as △ changes, where △ is the size of food source. We find that when △ is fixed, old and middle-aged ants hit the food source much more easily than the young ones. From Fig. S1, we also see that on the whole, the probability of finding food for an ant increases as △ increases, which is not very surprising.

Obviously, the tiring time ttired and the nest neighborhood range constant c play crucial roles in the search process, which shows that the physical ability and the knowledge the ants have have an important impact on their foraging behavior. Fig. 2 A and B shows the influence of ttired on the probability an ant finding food for different △. We see first that for a given group size, the foraging success increases as ttired grows. The greater the physical ability of an ant, the larger its ttired, i.e., the greater the physical ability, the easier the foraging. Second, the higher the number of foraging ants, the larger the probability of finding food. This agrees with traditional views reported in existing studies on optimization (3, 17). Third, when the value of ttired is fixed, the larger the size of the food source, the greater the probability of finding food.

Fig. 2.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Fig. 2.

(A and B) Influence of the tiring time ttired on different numbers of middle-aged ants finding food, where c=0.1 and ψ=7 and (A) Δ=10−4 and (B) Δ=10−5. (C and D) Influence of ψ on the probability of finding food for middle-aged ants of different numbers, where (C) Δ=10−4 and (D) Δ=10−5. Different color curves denote foraging ants with different numbers.

The search range has an important influence on the probability of finding food. We show that the increment of foraging probability for the ants is related to the increment of ψ in a nonlinear way. Fig. 2 C and D depicts the influence of ψ on the probability of an ant finding food. From Fig. 2 C and D, we conclude first that the larger the value of ψ, the smaller the foraging range—when ψ is very large (e.g., ψ≥13), the search range is so small that the food source is outside, and the probability of finding food is negligible. Second, when ψ is very small (for example, ψ=5), the probability of finding food is small because the foraging range is rather large. Third, when ψ is within an optimal range (for example, 5≤ψ≤12), the probability of finding food initially increases, then decreases and finally increases gain with varying ψ; that is, an optimal choice of the search space is needed for the ants to forage effectively.

Fig. 3 shows the influence of c on the average time of finding food for middle-aged ants with different ψ, where ttired = 1,000 and △ = 10−4. The results of Fig. 3 are averaged over 100 cycles. As seen from Fig. 3, we conclude first that the larger the value of c, the smaller the average foraging time and the smaller the average homing time. A larger value of the neighborhood range constant c indicates that the ant has more knowledge about the nest environment, which helps the whole ant colony find the food faster. Second, the larger the value of c, the smaller the ratio between average homing time and average foraging time; i.e., the average homing time decreases with increasing knowledge, which helps to raise the foraging efficiency of the ants. This is indeed an appropriate optimization strategy for animals with a home, whereas for other animals other optimization strategies are more appropriate.

Fig. 3.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Fig. 3.

The influence of c on the foraging behavior of middle-aged ants with different ψ. (A) Green and magenta curves show the average times tf that ants need in one foraging process when ψ=5 and ψ=7, respectively. The yellow and blue curves show the average times th at which ants start to use the homing strategy to return to the nest when ψ=5 and ψ=7, respectively. (B) The percentages of th and tf . The green curve represents the percentage th/tf when ψ=5 and the magenta line represents the ratio th/tf when ψ=7.

To summarize, we find that the physical ability and the knowledge about the environment of the nest strongly influence the foraging behavior of ants. We define an integrated search ability Γ(age)=(1−λ)ttired(age)+λc(age) to represent the synthesized capability of ants searching for food and nest, where “age” evidently denotes the age and λ is a scale factor which determines the proportion of ttired and c in Γ, where 0≤λ≤1. Thus, we derive a critical value Γ′: When Γ>Γ′, an ant is suitable to forage over long distances. Specifically, we obtain an age range in which ants are suitable to search over long distances. Middle-aged ants have a greater physical ability than young and old ones. However, with increasing age, ants have more knowledge of the nest, and the familiar neighborhood range c of a nest increases, i.e., old ants are very familiar with the environment around the nest. Thus, middle-aged and old ants have greater Γ than young ants, so they are suitable to forage over long distances. As for young ants, their foraging success is relatively small. Their main objective is not to find food but to learn, and the learning process is important for the long-term foraging ability of the whole colony.

Finding the Optimal Path.

When a scout ant finds a food source, it returns to the nest and gathers the recruited ants to find an optimal path between the food source and the nest. Fig. 4 A–D describes the discovery of the optimal path as time passes. Fig. 4 E and F shows the evolution of the variables y(t) and ‖Z→(t)‖. We see that the ant colony passes from an initially unsynchronized transient chaotic state into a synchronized periodic state under the influence of the variable y(t). We also see from Fig. 4E that y(t) determines the length of the chaotic search. The impact of the pheromone on the foraging behavior of ants is represented by a decrement in the variable y(t). With a continuous decrease of the variable y(t) with time, the influence of the collective on the behavior of an individual ant becomes stronger. When the effect of the social organization is sufficiently large, the chaotic behavior of the individual ant disappears. From Fig. 4F we find that the construction of the optimal path for the whole ant colony is similar to that of chaotic annealing, which is an effective optimization mechanism (24, 25). Fig. S2 shows the evolution of ‖Z→(t)‖ for different ω. From Fig. S2, we infer that the value of ω has a strong impact on the periodic behavior of the whole ant colony, i.e., the larger the value of ω, the faster the periodic oscillation (or the speed at which ants convey food).

Fig. 4.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Fig. 4.

The evolution of the different variables. (A–D) Finding the optimal path as time passes, where the point (0.4,0.4) is the position of the nest, point (0.7,0.5) is the position of the food source, and different symbols with different colors represent different ants. (E and F) The evolution of the variables y(t) and ‖Z→(t)‖=Z12(t)+Z22(t), respectively, where different colors refer to different ants.

Now we consider the influence of different local search factors b. Here all of the ants have the parameters ri=0.2 and ω=0.1. Fig. S3 shows the convergence of the angle ϕ, where ϕ=|((Z→i−P→nest)T(P→food−P→nest))/(‖(Z→i−P→nest)‖‖(P→food−P→nest)‖)| represents the angle between the system’s evolution vector Z→i−P→nest and the vector P→food−P→nest. It shows first that when b = ln(2), the angle ϕ cannot converge, implying that the foraging process is unstable. Second, when b < ln(2), the angle ϕ converges to 1 which implies that the ants start from a transient chaotic state and finally converge to a periodic behavior, and they walk directly between the nest and the food source. Third, the closer to ln(2) the value of b, the longer the transient process. And the closer b is to 0, the faster the convergence speed of a local search.

In the above, we make the important assumption that there is only one food source in the search space. Additionally, when two food sources exist with a symmetrical setup, symmetry breaking is more likely to occur. In this condition, the positive feedback drives the transition from the chaotic to the periodic foraging (SI Text).

Discussion

We emphasize the generic character of our analysis because a homing strategy in foraging is used by other animals with fixed basis, such as bumblebees, albatrosses, etc. Our model is readily applicable to these situations. For instance, in the foraging process of albatrosses, an optimal fish-searching strategy will not be optimal for home searching. Therefore, if the entire foraging process is assessed, the birds’ knowledge about the environment of their home should also be considered. However, this important aspect has received little attention in the existing studies. A homing strategy is not limited to the foraging process of animals, but is also relevant to many aspects of human behavior such as the daily return from work (trivial) and the recurrent accessing of the Internet (less trivial). The impact of human homing behavior on Internet use could be studied, for example, by analyzing how individuals search and roam in cyberspace around their home page.

Learning processes are evidently important for the lives of humans and animals. As demonstrated above, the multitime learning process of young animals benefits the sustainability of the whole group. Continuous learning about changes to the environment is also necessary for the group’s adaptation. Storms and heavy wind, for instance, might change the environment with which an animal is familiar. For humans, knowledge that has been proven to be useful has an important influence on behavioral patterns. Young individuals carry on learning about the environment in which they live for several years or even decades. This ability is the basis on which humans have evolved and developed. The views about learning processes expressed in this paper represent a significant departure from current notions about animals foraging strategies, which use either probabilistic distribution schemes or deterministic models. Our findings provide a new perspective on the behavior patterns of certain animals, and of humans, which is of importance in areas as diverse as the spread of diseases, the formation of groups (or networks), the patterns of many social activities, and the evolution of short message (or Web) services.

Conclusion

We have developed a model which can be used to explain not only how a single ant uses chaotic behavior to find a food source and its nest in the hunting and homing processes, but also describes how an ant colony organizes itself to find the optimal path between a food source and the nest. Here the transition of ant foraging from chaotic to periodic regimes is explained as a three-stage process. (i) An uncoordinated search occurs, which is characterized by the chaotic wandering of scout ants. When a scout finds a food source, it will return to the nest and recruit ants to find the optimal path between the nest and the food source. (ii) A cooperative search occurs, which is characterized by a phase during which the recruited ants find the optimal path under the combined influences of chaotic walking and pheromone detection. Individual ants, while still moving chaotically, often deposit pheromone as a form of indirect communication to help other ants find the food source. The collective organization power of the ants increases and their chaotic crawling decreases with the accumulation of pheromone on the paths. This phase lasts until the individual behavior is superseded. (iii) Finally, a synchronized periodic motion sets in. All recruited ants are busy conveying the food back and forth along the optimal path between the nest and the food source. In our model, the transition from chaotic to synchronized regimes results from solving an optimization problem (see Table S1). Moreover, according to our analysis, physical ability, experience, and the existence of a nest have important impacts on the foraging behavior of ant colonies.

Through numerical experiments, we reach the following main conclusions. (i) The age of the ants is crucial. Old and middle-aged ants find a food source much more easily than the young ones. However, pursuing a strategy whereby young ants forage together with old and middle-aged ants can be optimal because it benefits the long-term foraging prospects of the whole colony. (ii) The physical ability of a single ant is also crucial. The greater the physical performance, the better the foraging. Therefore, it is easier for a middle-aged ant to find a food source than younger and older ants. (iii) The search range has an important influence on the probability of finding food. In order for ants to forage effectively, the range of the search space should lie within an optimal realm. (iv) The foraging efficiency of group animals with homes is clearly different from those without. For group animals, more knowledge about the neighborhood of the nest increases foraging efficiency, i.e., the more knowledge of its home an animal has, the shorter its homing time. Based on these insights, we suggest that for group animals that have a home, their foraging behavior should not be characterized by random walking but rather by deterministic walking in random environments.

Methods

In the foraging process, the movement strategy of a single ant i depends on the current position of the ant Z→i(t), the best position found by itself or any one of its neighbors, the position of the nest P→nest, the position of the food source P→food, the characteristic variable of chaotic crawling yi(t), and the self-organization factor ri. Generally, the following function is used to describe the whole foraging process of ants:Z→i(t+1)=g(Z→i(t),P→food,P→nest,yi(t),ri),[2]

where t means the current time step and g is a nonlinear function.

To mimic an initially chaotic search, we introduce the chaotic model Z′(t+1)=Z′(t)eμ(1−Z′(t)) described by Solé et al. in ref. 15. Let Z′=(1/μ)ψZ, then we get Z(t+1)=Z(t)eμ−ψ⋅Z(t), and the search center is approximately 7.5/(2ψ).

Here, the organization of ants sets in under the influences of the pheromone and the chaotic crawling of ants. As time evolves, the pheromone intensity increases and the chaotic crawling of ants is gradually reduced. Based on the annealing mechanism (24, 25), the adjustment of the chaotic behavior of individual ant i is achieved by introducing a successively decreasing dynamical equation represented by yi(t)=yi(t−1)(1+ri). The self-organization factor ri is used to control the time of the chaotic search. If ri is very large, the chaotic search is short, and vice versa. Because small changes are desired as time evolves, ri is chosen typically in the range [0,0.5].

Moreover, the term |sin(ωt)|(Pfoodk−Pnestk)−(Zik(t)−Pnestk) is introduced to achieve periodic oscillating behavior of an individual ant between the nest and the food source, where k is the kth dimension of the position vector. The length of the optimal path is L=∑k=1l(Pfoodk−Pnestk)2. Adjustment of the position of each ant obeys Eq. 1.

In the food searching process, because there is no organization initially, the position of the food source could be found by setting ri=0. The foraging strategy of ants is to search Z→i such that f(Z→i)<Δ. When ri = 0, e−ayi(t), and e−2ayi(t)+bi approximate to 0, and Eq. 1 applies, we get the following chaotic model:Zik(t+1)=(Zik(t)+Vk)e(3−ψk(Zik(t)+Vk))−Vk.[3]

That is, the ants walk chaotically throughout the foraging process. Here, Vk=7.5/(2ψk)−Pnestk. The ants center around the nest and search for food. The search diameter ϕk depends on ψk and ϕk≈7.5/ψk. Because all the n ants conduct a parallel search in the search space, the ant colony quickly finds the food source P→food.

In searching for the nest, the main aim of ant i is to use its homing strategy to find the neighborhood of its nest such that Mi={Z→i:‖Z→i−P→nest‖<ci}. Here, older ants have larger ci because they have more knowledge about their nest.

During the optimal path finding process, self-organization in the ant colony gradually occurs, where ri>0. Under the influence of self-organization, the variable yi(t) of ants is gradually attenuated to 0. Eq. 1 is then a transient chaotic convergence process. The larger ri, the faster the alteration of yi(t), and the faster the self-organization process of the system is formed. When yi(t) approaches 0, both e−ayi(t) and e−2ayi(t) approach 1, and parameter b begins to work. Here, the search model becomesZik(t+1)=Zik(t)+eb(|sin(ωt)|(Pfoodk−Pnestk)−(Zik(t)−Pnestk)).

When 0 < b < ln(2), the system starts from a transient chaos state and finally converges to a periodic behavior, and the ants walk between the nest and the food source to convey food. That is, the angle ϕ converges from ϕ<1 to ϕ=1, where i = 1,…,n.

Because the chaotic search belongs to a global search and the search caused by pheromone belongs to a local one, the self-organization process is the one that transfers from the global search to the local one. In this process, the ants finally find the optimal path along which they carry the food periodically.

Acknowledgments

This work was partially supported by International Research Training Group 1740 (Deutsche Forschungsgemeinschaft and Fundação de Amparo à Pesquisa do Estado de São Paulo), the Government of the Russian Federation (Grant 14.Z50.31.0033), the Beijing Center for Mathematics and Information Interdisciplinary Sciences, and the National Natural Science Foundation of China (Grants 61100204 and 61121061).

Footnotes

  • ↵1To whom correspondence may be addressed. E-mail: kurths{at}pik-potsdam.de, penghaipeng{at}bupt.edu.cn, or director{at}pik-potsdam.de.
  • Author contributions: L.L., H.P., and J.K. designed research; L.L., H.P., and Y.Y. performed research; H.P. analyzed data; and L.L., J.K., and H.J.S. wrote the paper.

  • The authors declare no conflict of interest.

  • This article contains supporting information online at www.pnas.org/lookup/suppl/doi:10.1073/pnas.1407083111/-/DCSupplemental.

View Abstract

References

  1. ↵
    1. Couzin ID,
    2. Krause J,
    3. Franks NR,
    4. Levin SA
    (2005) Effective leadership and decision-making in animal groups on the move. Nature 433(7025):513–516.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  2. ↵
    1. Edwards AM,
    2. et al.
    (2007) Revisiting Lévy flight search patterns of wandering albatrosses, bumblebees and deer. Nature 449(7165):1044–1048.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  3. ↵
    1. Bonabeau E,
    2. Dorigo M,
    3. Theraulaz G
    (2000) Inspiration for optimization from social insect behaviour. Nature 406(6791):39–42.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  4. ↵
    1. Dussutour A,
    2. Fourcassié V,
    3. Helbing D,
    4. Deneubourg JL
    (2004) Optimal traffic organization in ants under crowded conditions. Nature 428(6978):70–73.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  5. ↵
    1. Wu Y,
    2. Zhou CS,
    3. Xiao JH,
    4. Kurths J,
    5. Schellnhuber HJ
    (2010) Evidence for a bimodal distribution in human communication. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 107(44):18803–18808.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  6. ↵
    1. Viswanathan GM,
    2. et al.
    (1996) Levy flight search patterns of wandering albatrosses. Nature 381(6581):413–415.
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  7. ↵
    1. Reynolds AM
    (2011) On the origin of bursts and heavy tails in animal dynamics. Physica A 390(2):245–249.
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  8. ↵
    1. Travis J
    (2007) Ecology. Do wandering albatrosses care about math? Science 318(5851):742–743.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  9. ↵
    1. Viswanathan GM,
    2. Raposo EP,
    3. da Luz MGE
    (2008) Lévy flights and superdiffusion in the context of biological encounters and random searches. Phys Life Rev 5(3):133–150.
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  10. ↵
    1. Bunimovich L
    (2004) Deterministic walks in random environments. Physica D 187(1-4):20–29.
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  11. ↵
    1. Mailleux AC,
    2. Detrain C,
    3. Deneubourg JL
    (2005) Triggering and persistence of trail-laying in foragers of the ant Lasius niger. J Insect Physiol 51(3):297–304.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  12. ↵
    1. Shlesinger MF
    (1986) On Growth and Form, eds Stanley HF, Ostrowsky N (Nijhoff, Dordrecht, The Netherlands), pp 279–283.
  13. ↵
    1. Deneubourg JL,
    2. Aron S,
    3. Goss S,
    4. Pasteels JM
    (1990) The selforganizing Exploratory pattern of the Argentine ant. J Insect Behav 3(2):159–168.
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  14. ↵
    1. Cole BJ
    (1991) Is animal behavior chaotic? Evidence from the activity of ants. Proc Biol Sci 244(1311):253–259.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  15. ↵
    1. Solé RV,
    2. Miramontes O,
    3. Goodwin BC
    (1993) Oscillations and chaos in ant societies. J Theoret Biol 161(3):343–357.
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  16. ↵
    1. Nemes L,
    2. Roska T
    (1995) A CNN model of oscillation and chaos in ant colonies: A case study. IEEE Trans Circuit Syst I 42(10):741–745.
    OpenUrl
  17. ↵
    1. Li LX,
    2. Yang YX,
    3. Peng HP,
    4. Wang XD
    (2006) An optimization method inspired by ‘chaotic’ ant behavior. Int J Bifurcat Chaos 16(8):2351–2364.
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  18. ↵
    1. Peng HP,
    2. Li LX,
    3. Yang YX,
    4. Liu F
    (2010) Parameter estimation of dynamical systems via a chaotic ant swarm. Phys Rev E Stat Nonlin Soft Matter Phys 81(1 Pt 2):016207.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  19. ↵
    1. Sudd JH
    (1967) An Introduction to the Behavior of Ants (Univ of Hull St Martin’s Press, New York).
  20. ↵
    1. Herbers JM
    (1983) Social organization in Leptothorax ants: Within- and between-species patterns. Psyche (Stuttg) 90(4):361–386.
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  21. ↵
    1. Liang ZS,
    2. et al.
    (2012) Molecular determinants of scouting behavior in honey bees. Science 335(6073):1225–1228.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  22. ↵
    1. Sommer S,
    2. von Beeren C,
    3. Wehner R
    (2008) Multiroute memories in desert ants. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 105(1):317–322.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  23. ↵
    1. Bonabeau E,
    2. Dorigo M,
    3. Theraulaz G
    (1999) Swarm Intelligence: From Natural to Artificial System (Oxford Univ Press, Oxford).
  24. ↵
    1. Chen L,
    2. Aihara K
    (1995) Chaotic simulated annealing by a neural network model with transient chaos. Neural Netw 8(6):915–930.
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  25. ↵
    1. Aihara K
    (2002) Chaos engineering and its application to parallel distributed processing with chaotic neural networks. Proc IEEE 90(5):919–930.
    OpenUrlCrossRef
PreviousNext
Back to top
Article Alerts
Email Article

Thank you for your interest in spreading the word on PNAS.

NOTE: We only request your email address so that the person you are recommending the page to knows that you wanted them to see it, and that it is not junk mail. We do not capture any email address.

Enter multiple addresses on separate lines or separate them with commas.
Chaos–order transition in foraging behavior of ants
(Your Name) has sent you a message from PNAS
(Your Name) thought you would like to see the PNAS web site.
CAPTCHA
This question is for testing whether or not you are a human visitor and to prevent automated spam submissions.
Citation Tools
Chaos–order transition in foraging of ants
Lixiang Li, Haipeng Peng, Jürgen Kurths, Yixian Yang, Hans Joachim Schellnhuber
Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences Jun 2014, 111 (23) 8392-8397; DOI: 10.1073/pnas.1407083111

Citation Manager Formats

  • BibTeX
  • Bookends
  • EasyBib
  • EndNote (tagged)
  • EndNote 8 (xml)
  • Medlars
  • Mendeley
  • Papers
  • RefWorks Tagged
  • Ref Manager
  • RIS
  • Zotero
Request Permissions
Share
Chaos–order transition in foraging of ants
Lixiang Li, Haipeng Peng, Jürgen Kurths, Yixian Yang, Hans Joachim Schellnhuber
Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences Jun 2014, 111 (23) 8392-8397; DOI: 10.1073/pnas.1407083111
Digg logo Reddit logo Twitter logo Facebook logo Google logo Mendeley logo
  • Tweet Widget
  • Facebook Like
  • Mendeley logo Mendeley
Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences: 111 (23)
Table of Contents

Submit

Sign up for Article Alerts

Article Classifications

  • Physical Sciences
  • Physics
  • Biological Sciences
  • Ecology

Jump to section

  • Article
    • Abstract
    • Foraging Strategies of Ants
    • Results
    • Discussion
    • Conclusion
    • Methods
    • Acknowledgments
    • Footnotes
    • References
  • Figures & SI
  • Info & Metrics
  • PDF

You May Also be Interested in

Abstract depiction of a guitar and musical note
Science & Culture: At the nexus of music and medicine, some see disease treatments
Although the evidence is still limited, a growing body of research suggests music may have beneficial effects for diseases such as Parkinson’s.
Image credit: Shutterstock/agsandrew.
Large piece of gold
News Feature: Tracing gold's cosmic origins
Astronomers thought they’d finally figured out where gold and other heavy elements in the universe came from. In light of recent results, they’re not so sure.
Image credit: Science Source/Tom McHugh.
Dancers in red dresses
Journal Club: Friends appear to share patterns of brain activity
Researchers are still trying to understand what causes this strong correlation between neural and social networks.
Image credit: Shutterstock/Yeongsik Im.
Yellow emoticons
Learning the language of facial expressions
Aleix Martinez explains why facial expressions often are not accurate indicators of emotion.
Listen
Past PodcastsSubscribe
Goats standing in a pin
Transplantation of sperm-producing stem cells
CRISPR-Cas9 gene editing can improve the effectiveness of spermatogonial stem cell transplantation in mice and livestock, a study finds.
Image credit: Jon M. Oatley.

Similar Articles

Site Logo
Powered by HighWire
  • Submit Manuscript
  • Twitter
  • Facebook
  • RSS Feeds
  • Email Alerts

Articles

  • Current Issue
  • Special Feature Articles – Most Recent
  • List of Issues

PNAS Portals

  • Anthropology
  • Chemistry
  • Classics
  • Front Matter
  • Physics
  • Sustainability Science
  • Teaching Resources

Information

  • Authors
  • Editorial Board
  • Reviewers
  • Librarians
  • Press
  • Site Map
  • PNAS Updates

Feedback    Privacy/Legal

Copyright © 2021 National Academy of Sciences. Online ISSN 1091-6490