Skip to main content

Main menu

  • Home
  • Articles
    • Current
    • Special Feature Articles - Most Recent
    • Special Features
    • Colloquia
    • Collected Articles
    • PNAS Classics
    • List of Issues
  • Front Matter
    • Front Matter Portal
    • Journal Club
  • News
    • For the Press
    • This Week In PNAS
    • PNAS in the News
  • Podcasts
  • Authors
    • Information for Authors
    • Editorial and Journal Policies
    • Submission Procedures
    • Fees and Licenses
  • Submit
  • Submit
  • About
    • Editorial Board
    • PNAS Staff
    • FAQ
    • Accessibility Statement
    • Rights and Permissions
    • Site Map
  • Contact
  • Journal Club
  • Subscribe
    • Subscription Rates
    • Subscriptions FAQ
    • Open Access
    • Recommend PNAS to Your Librarian

User menu

  • Log in
  • My Cart

Search

  • Advanced search
Home
Home
  • Log in
  • My Cart

Advanced Search

  • Home
  • Articles
    • Current
    • Special Feature Articles - Most Recent
    • Special Features
    • Colloquia
    • Collected Articles
    • PNAS Classics
    • List of Issues
  • Front Matter
    • Front Matter Portal
    • Journal Club
  • News
    • For the Press
    • This Week In PNAS
    • PNAS in the News
  • Podcasts
  • Authors
    • Information for Authors
    • Editorial and Journal Policies
    • Submission Procedures
    • Fees and Licenses
  • Submit
Letter

Still no convincing evidence for cognitive map use by honeybees

Allen Cheung, Matthew Collett, Thomas S. Collett, Alex Dewar, Fred Dyer, Paul Graham, Michael Mangan, Ajay Narendra, Andrew Philippides, Wolfgang Stürzl, Barbara Webb, Antoine Wystrach, and Jochen Zeil
  1. aQueensland Brain Institute, The University of Queensland, Brisbane, QLD 4072, Australia;
  2. bCentre for Research in Animal Behaviour, University of Exeter, Exeter EX4 4QG, United Kingdom;
  3. cCentre for Computational Neuroscience and Robotics, University of Sussex, Brighton BN1 9QG, United Kingdom;
  4. dDepartment of Zoology, Michigan State University, East Lansing, MI 48824;
  5. eInstitute of Perception, Action and Behaviour, School of Informatics, University of Edinburgh, Edinburgh EH8 9AB, United Kingdom;
  6. fResearch School of Biology, The Australian National University, Canberra, ACT 0200, Australia; and
  7. gGerman Aerospace Center, Institute of Robotics and Mechatronics, D82234 Wessling, Germany

See allHide authors and affiliations

PNAS October 21, 2014 111 (42) E4396-E4397; first published October 2, 2014; https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1413581111
Allen Cheung
aQueensland Brain Institute, The University of Queensland, Brisbane, QLD 4072, Australia;
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Matthew Collett
bCentre for Research in Animal Behaviour, University of Exeter, Exeter EX4 4QG, United Kingdom;
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Thomas S. Collett
cCentre for Computational Neuroscience and Robotics, University of Sussex, Brighton BN1 9QG, United Kingdom;
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Alex Dewar
cCentre for Computational Neuroscience and Robotics, University of Sussex, Brighton BN1 9QG, United Kingdom;
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Fred Dyer
dDepartment of Zoology, Michigan State University, East Lansing, MI 48824;
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Paul Graham
cCentre for Computational Neuroscience and Robotics, University of Sussex, Brighton BN1 9QG, United Kingdom;
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Michael Mangan
eInstitute of Perception, Action and Behaviour, School of Informatics, University of Edinburgh, Edinburgh EH8 9AB, United Kingdom;
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Ajay Narendra
fResearch School of Biology, The Australian National University, Canberra, ACT 0200, Australia; and
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Andrew Philippides
cCentre for Computational Neuroscience and Robotics, University of Sussex, Brighton BN1 9QG, United Kingdom;
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Wolfgang Stürzl
gGerman Aerospace Center, Institute of Robotics and Mechatronics, D82234 Wessling, Germany
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Barbara Webb
eInstitute of Perception, Action and Behaviour, School of Informatics, University of Edinburgh, Edinburgh EH8 9AB, United Kingdom;
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Antoine Wystrach
eInstitute of Perception, Action and Behaviour, School of Informatics, University of Edinburgh, Edinburgh EH8 9AB, United Kingdom;
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Jochen Zeil
fResearch School of Biology, The Australian National University, Canberra, ACT 0200, Australia; and
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
  • For correspondence: jochen.zeil@anu.edu.au
  • Article
  • Figures & SI
  • Info & Metrics
  • PDF
Loading

Cheeseman et al. (1) claim that an ability of honey bees to travel home through a landscape with conflicting information from a celestial compass proves the bees' use of a cognitive map. Their claim involves a curious assumption about the visual information that can be extracted from the terrain: that there is sufficient information for a bee to identify where it is, but insufficient to guide its path without resorting to a cognitive map. We contend that the authors’ claims are unfounded.

Proof that an animal uses a cognitive map requires, at the very least, results that cannot be explained by other known mechanisms. Cheeseman et al. consider only one alternative mechanism to the use of a cognitive map: the association of compass directions with visual scenes (as detailed in ref. 2). They thus neglect the extensive experimental and theoretical evidence that insects can also be guided purely by disparities between their memories of visual scenes and their current view of the world (3). It has long been known that this visual guidance in insects can operate independently of information from path integration and more generally with no celestial compass or with a conflicting celestial compass (4).

Image analysis and modeling show that the information contained in panoramic views of natural scenes can provide guidance across large areas, without the need of celestial compass information or a map-like representation (4, 5). For a flying bee, such a view would include both the skyline and the ground below. The authors have failed to do any image analysis of the visual information available to bees at their study site. However, based on what they supply, there is reason to expect that view-based guidance can also account for the bees' behavior at this site (Fig. 1).

Fig. 1.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Fig. 1.

Visual scenes from the experimental site are likely to supply visual guidance to navigating bees. Using the coordinates provided by Cheeseman et al., we used Google maps to estimate the distance of treelines and forest edges from the experimental area. Combining this landscape model with the landscape images (taken facing north and south) provided in their SI Materials and Methods, we estimated the typical height of trees in this area and created a 3D virtual reconstruction of the experimental site from which we could compute panoramic skylines from any of the experimental locations. (A) Recreated high-resolution skyline from release point 2, with the two photos provided in ref. 1 each showing ∼45° of the skyline inserted above. The skyline and photos are magnified vertically for clarity. (B–D, Upper) Panoramic views of the skyline at a bee's 4° resolution from the feeder, release point 2, and release point 3, respectively. All views are centered on west. The gray level of each pixel represents how much of that pixel is covered by trees. Therefore, gray level is a proxy for skyline height. Pixels are magnified vertically for clarity. (Lower) Rotational image difference functions (rIDFs). An rIDF at a location compares the reconstructed panorama as perceived from that location with the reconstructed panorama as perceived from the feeder location. It is computed from the root-mean-square pixel differences obtained by horizontally sliding one panorama over the other in steps of 2° (for details, see ref. 4). Image differences are shown as a proportion of the maximum image difference. B shows an auto-rIDF of the reconstructed panorama from the feeder location compared with itself. The rIDF is zero when the panorama is aligned with itself and gradually rises with increasing shift of the panoramas relative to each other. In this case the reference panorama was centered on the hive direction (dashed line). C and D show rIDFs in which the view from each release point is compared with the view from the feeder toward the hive (i.e., B). In both cases, the minimum is near the hive direction (dashed lines). These comparisons show that a remembered direction can be obtained by matching a view stored at a location to the currently perceived view at that location (B) and, further, that homing directions can be obtained by comparing the stored view with the current view from other locations in the neighborhood (C and D). The general point that the terrain supplies visual information for view-based guidance is independent of the accuracy of this particular reconstruction. Indeed, bees will have a more complex set of stored views gathered during their exploration and orientation flights, including information below the horizon. These extra views would enhance the robustness of their view-based navigation.

With an understanding of how insects can use view-based guidance, we can go further and suggest an alternative explanation for the effect of anesthesia. Rather than acting to clock-shift the celestial compass, anesthesia may well have simply knocked out the path integration home vector. The authors claim that in one of the two conditions, the anesthetized bees follow a clock-shifted path integration home vector. This predicted direction, however, coincides with the direction of the trained feeder to which at least 8 of 24 bees in experiment 1 and 7 of 12 bees in experiment 2 indeed first fly. The initial flight directions of all of the anesthetized bees can thus be explained in terms of view-based guidance toward the trained feeders, toward the trained feeder nest routes, or toward dominant landscape features. Consequently, the authors' dismissal of a proposed association of compass directions with visual scenes (2) is also questionable.

Taking all these points, we believe that Cheeseman et al.’s claims are not substantiated and that their results do not add anything new to the debate surrounding cognitive maps in insects.

Footnotes

  • ↵1To whom correspondence should be addressed. Email: jochen.zeil{at}anu.edu.au.
  • Author contributions: A.C., M.C., T.S.C., A.D., F.D., P.G., M.M., A.N., A.P., W.S., B.W., A.W., and J.Z. analyzed data and wrote the paper.

  • The authors declare no conflict of interest.

References

  1. ↵
    1. Cheeseman JF, et al.
    (2014) Way-finding in displaced clock-shifted bees proves bees use a cognitive map. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 111(24):8949–8954
    .
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  2. ↵
    1. Cruse H,
    2. Wehner R
    (2011) No need for a cognitive map: Decentralized memory for insect navigation. PLOS Comput Biol 7(3):e1002009
    .
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  3. ↵
    1. Zeil J
    (2012) Visual homing: An insect perspective. Curr Opin Neurobiol 22(2):285–293
    .
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  4. ↵
    1. Narendra A,
    2. Gourmaud S,
    3. Zeil J
    (2013) Mapping the navigational knowledge of individually foraging ants, Myrmecia croslandi. Proc R Soc B 280(1765):20130683
    .
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  5. ↵
    1. Baddeley B,
    2. Graham P,
    3. Husbands P,
    4. Philippides A
    (2012) A model of ant route navigation driven by scene familiarity. PLOS Comput Biol 8(1):e1002336
    .
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
PreviousNext
Back to top
Article Alerts
Email Article

Thank you for your interest in spreading the word on PNAS.

NOTE: We only request your email address so that the person you are recommending the page to knows that you wanted them to see it, and that it is not junk mail. We do not capture any email address.

Enter multiple addresses on separate lines or separate them with commas.
Still no convincing evidence for cognitive map use by honeybees
(Your Name) has sent you a message from PNAS
(Your Name) thought you would like to see the PNAS web site.
CAPTCHA
This question is for testing whether or not you are a human visitor and to prevent automated spam submissions.
Citation Tools
No evidence for cognitive map use by honeybees
Allen Cheung, Matthew Collett, Thomas S. Collett, Alex Dewar, Fred Dyer, Paul Graham, Michael Mangan, Ajay Narendra, Andrew Philippides, Wolfgang Stürzl, Barbara Webb, Antoine Wystrach, Jochen Zeil
Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences Oct 2014, 111 (42) E4396-E4397; DOI: 10.1073/pnas.1413581111

Citation Manager Formats

  • BibTeX
  • Bookends
  • EasyBib
  • EndNote (tagged)
  • EndNote 8 (xml)
  • Medlars
  • Mendeley
  • Papers
  • RefWorks Tagged
  • Ref Manager
  • RIS
  • Zotero
Request Permissions
Share
No evidence for cognitive map use by honeybees
Allen Cheung, Matthew Collett, Thomas S. Collett, Alex Dewar, Fred Dyer, Paul Graham, Michael Mangan, Ajay Narendra, Andrew Philippides, Wolfgang Stürzl, Barbara Webb, Antoine Wystrach, Jochen Zeil
Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences Oct 2014, 111 (42) E4396-E4397; DOI: 10.1073/pnas.1413581111
del.icio.us logo Digg logo Reddit logo Twitter logo CiteULike logo Facebook logo Google logo Mendeley logo
  • Tweet Widget
  • Facebook Like
  • Mendeley logo Mendeley

Article Classifications

  • Biological Sciences
  • Neuroscience
  • Social Sciences
  • Psychological and Cognitive Sciences

This Letter has a Reply and related content. Please see:

  • Relationship between Letter and Reply - October 02, 2014
  • Way-finding in displaced clock-shifted bees proves bees use a cognitive map - June 02, 2014
Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences: 111 (42)
Table of Contents

Submit

Sign up for Article Alerts

Jump to section

  • Article
    • Footnotes
    • References
  • Figures & SI
  • Info & Metrics
  • PDF

You May Also be Interested in

Setting sun over a sun-baked dirt landscape
Core Concept: Popular integrated assessment climate policy models have key caveats
Better explicating the strengths and shortcomings of these models will help refine projections and improve transparency in the years ahead.
Image credit: Witsawat.S.
Model of the Amazon forest
News Feature: A sea in the Amazon
Did the Caribbean sweep into the western Amazon millions of years ago, shaping the region’s rich biodiversity?
Image credit: Tacio Cordeiro Bicudo (University of São Paulo, São Paulo, Brazil), Victor Sacek (University of São Paulo, São Paulo, Brazil), and Lucy Reading-Ikkanda (artist).
Syrian archaeological site
Journal Club: In Mesopotamia, early cities may have faltered before climate-driven collapse
Settlements 4,200 years ago may have suffered from overpopulation before drought and lower temperatures ultimately made them unsustainable.
Image credit: Andrea Ricci.
Steamboat Geyser eruption.
Eruption of Steamboat Geyser
Mara Reed and Michael Manga explore why Yellowstone's Steamboat Geyser resumed erupting in 2018.
Listen
Past PodcastsSubscribe
Birds nestling on tree branches
Parent–offspring conflict in songbird fledging
Some songbird parents might improve their own fitness by manipulating their offspring into leaving the nest early, at the cost of fledgling survival, a study finds.
Image credit: Gil Eckrich (photographer).

Similar Articles

Site Logo
Powered by HighWire
  • Submit Manuscript
  • Twitter
  • Facebook
  • RSS Feeds
  • Email Alerts

Articles

  • Current Issue
  • Special Feature Articles – Most Recent
  • List of Issues

PNAS Portals

  • Anthropology
  • Chemistry
  • Classics
  • Front Matter
  • Physics
  • Sustainability Science
  • Teaching Resources

Information

  • Authors
  • Editorial Board
  • Reviewers
  • Subscribers
  • Librarians
  • Press
  • Site Map
  • PNAS Updates
  • FAQs
  • Accessibility Statement
  • Rights & Permissions
  • About
  • Contact

Feedback    Privacy/Legal

Copyright © 2021 National Academy of Sciences. Online ISSN 1091-6490