Skip to main content
  • Submit
  • About
    • Editorial Board
    • PNAS Staff
    • FAQ
    • Accessibility Statement
    • Rights and Permissions
    • Site Map
  • Contact
  • Journal Club
  • Subscribe
    • Subscription Rates
    • Subscriptions FAQ
    • Open Access
    • Recommend PNAS to Your Librarian
  • Log in
  • My Cart

Main menu

  • Home
  • Articles
    • Current
    • Special Feature Articles - Most Recent
    • Special Features
    • Colloquia
    • Collected Articles
    • PNAS Classics
    • List of Issues
  • Front Matter
  • News
    • For the Press
    • This Week In PNAS
    • PNAS in the News
  • Podcasts
  • Authors
    • Information for Authors
    • Editorial and Journal Policies
    • Submission Procedures
    • Fees and Licenses
  • Submit
  • About
    • Editorial Board
    • PNAS Staff
    • FAQ
    • Accessibility Statement
    • Rights and Permissions
    • Site Map
  • Contact
  • Journal Club
  • Subscribe
    • Subscription Rates
    • Subscriptions FAQ
    • Open Access
    • Recommend PNAS to Your Librarian

User menu

  • Log in
  • My Cart

Search

  • Advanced search
Home
Home

Advanced Search

  • Home
  • Articles
    • Current
    • Special Feature Articles - Most Recent
    • Special Features
    • Colloquia
    • Collected Articles
    • PNAS Classics
    • List of Issues
  • Front Matter
  • News
    • For the Press
    • This Week In PNAS
    • PNAS in the News
  • Podcasts
  • Authors
    • Information for Authors
    • Editorial and Journal Policies
    • Submission Procedures
    • Fees and Licenses

New Research In

Physical Sciences

Featured Portals

  • Physics
  • Chemistry
  • Sustainability Science

Articles by Topic

  • Applied Mathematics
  • Applied Physical Sciences
  • Astronomy
  • Computer Sciences
  • Earth, Atmospheric, and Planetary Sciences
  • Engineering
  • Environmental Sciences
  • Mathematics
  • Statistics

Social Sciences

Featured Portals

  • Anthropology
  • Sustainability Science

Articles by Topic

  • Economic Sciences
  • Environmental Sciences
  • Political Sciences
  • Psychological and Cognitive Sciences
  • Social Sciences

Biological Sciences

Featured Portals

  • Sustainability Science

Articles by Topic

  • Agricultural Sciences
  • Anthropology
  • Applied Biological Sciences
  • Biochemistry
  • Biophysics and Computational Biology
  • Cell Biology
  • Developmental Biology
  • Ecology
  • Environmental Sciences
  • Evolution
  • Genetics
  • Immunology and Inflammation
  • Medical Sciences
  • Microbiology
  • Neuroscience
  • Pharmacology
  • Physiology
  • Plant Biology
  • Population Biology
  • Psychological and Cognitive Sciences
  • Sustainability Science
  • Systems Biology
Research Article

Rapid behavioral maturation accelerates failure of stressed honey bee colonies

Clint J. Perry, View ORCID ProfileEirik Søvik, Mary R. Myerscough, and Andrew B. Barron
PNAS March 17, 2015 112 (11) 3427-3432; first published February 9, 2015; https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1422089112
Clint J. Perry
aDepartment of Biological Sciences, Macquarie University, Sydney, NSW 2109, Australia;
bSchool of Biological and Chemical Sciences, Queen Mary University of London, London E1 4NS, United Kingdom;
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Eirik Søvik
aDepartment of Biological Sciences, Macquarie University, Sydney, NSW 2109, Australia;
cDepartment of Biology, Washington University in St. Louis, St. Louis, MO 63130; and
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
  • ORCID record for Eirik Søvik
Mary R. Myerscough
dSchool of Mathematics and Statistics, The University of Sydney, Sydney, NSW 2006, Australia
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Andrew B. Barron
aDepartment of Biological Sciences, Macquarie University, Sydney, NSW 2109, Australia;
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
  • For correspondence: andrew.barron@mq.edu.au
  1. Edited by Gene E. Robinson, University of Illinois at Urbana–Champaign, Urbana, IL, and approved January 21, 2015 (received for review November 18, 2014)

This article has a Correction. Please see:

  • Correction for Perry et al., Rapid behavioral maturation accelerates failure of stressed honey bee colonies - July 26, 2016
  • Article
  • Figures & SI
  • Info & Metrics
  • PDF
Loading

Significance

Honey bee colony death rates are unsustainably high. While many stressors have been identified that contribute to this problem, we do not know why colonies transition so rapidly from a state of apparent health to failure. It is well known that individual bees react to nutritional and pathogen stresses by foraging precociously: our study explains how colony failure arises from the social responses of individual bees to stress. We used radio tracking to monitor performance of bees and found that workers who begin foraging prematurely perform very poorly. This compounds the stresses on the colony and accelerates failure. We suggest how colonies at risk can be identified early, and the most effective interventions to prevent failure.

Abstract

Many complex factors have been linked to the recent marked increase in honey bee colony failure, including pests and pathogens, agrochemicals, and nutritional stressors. It remains unclear, however, why colonies frequently react to stressors by losing almost their entire adult bee population in a short time, resulting in a colony population collapse. Here we examine the social dynamics underlying such dramatic colony failure. Bees respond to many stressors by foraging earlier in life. We manipulated the demography of experimental colonies to induce precocious foraging in bees and used radio tag tracking to examine the consequences of precocious foraging for their performance. Precocious foragers completed far fewer foraging trips in their life, and had a higher risk of death in their first flights. We constructed a demographic model to explore how this individual reaction of bees to stress might impact colony performance. In the model, when forager death rates were chronically elevated, an increasingly younger forager force caused a positive feedback that dramatically accelerated terminal population decline in the colony. This resulted in a breakdown in division of labor and loss of the adult population, leaving only brood, food, and few adults in the hive. This study explains the social processes that drive rapid depopulation of a colony, and we explore possible strategies to prevent colony failure. Understanding the process of colony failure helps identify the most effective strategies to improve colony resilience.

  • behavioral development
  • temporal polyethism
  • colony collapse disorder
  • compartment model
  • RFID

High honey bee colony failure rates are a continuing concern. In North America, annual colony losses averaged 29.6% between 2006 and 2013 (1). In some cases, colony failure has been so rapid that outwardly healthy colonies lost most of their adult bees in a matter of weeks (2⇓–4). This has been described as colony collapse disorder (3).

Many stressors are contributing to bee colony failure, most with a strong anthropogenic component. These include parasites and pathogens, pesticides, and nutritional stresses (4). The parasitic mite Varroa destructor and its associated viruses (4) and the gut parasite Nosema have both been linked to high colony failure rates (5). There is increasing concern about the role of pesticides and agrochemicals in honey bee colony losses (6), and nutritional deficits arising from limited floral diversity and abundance have also been linked to elevated rates of colony failure (7).

It is likely that all of these factors are contributing to the elevated rates of colony losses currently experienced across most of Europe and North America (1, 8). None of these issues can be easily resolved within the economic paradigm of intensive agriculture and apiculture. The problem of honey bee colony failure is proving hard to manage because colonies often transition from apparent health to almost complete population loss within a few weeks or less (3, 9), which is often too fast for beekeepers to intervene and attempt to save their colonies.

The failure of a honey bee colony is a breakdown of a society. Bees within a colony interact to maintain the colony as a homeostatic system that has been described as a superorganism (10). Understanding why and how colonies fail therefore requires more than analyzing how individual bees react to stressors. Here we explored via empirical study and modeling how individual stress responses of honey bees interact with social dynamics to cause colony failure.

Honey bees, like many social insects, delay leaving the protected environment of the nest to forage until later in adult life (11, 12). Bees display a predictable pattern of behavioral development. They begin adult life performing in-hive tasks, and, when 2–3 wk old as adults, they become foragers gathering floral resources (13⇓–15). Varied stressors, such as a loss of foragers, starvation (16, 17), or disease (5, 18, 19), can cause bees to accelerate their behavioral development and forage precociously. The transition to foraging is socially regulated such that the presence of an established foraging force in the colony delays the onset of foraging in young bees (20⇓–22). As a consequence of this system, if the colony loses foragers, young bees accelerate their behavioral development to replace them (23, 24). Several diverse stressors cause a similar response in individual bees. These include individual or colony starvation (16, 17), pollen deprivation (25, 26), disease (5, 18), and even wax deprivation (27).

Precocious foragers may not be optimally adapted for the tasks outside the hive (28). There is evidence that precocious foragers are heavier and less efficient fliers than normal-aged foragers (28), and the flight muscle biochemistry of precocious foragers differs from that of typical foragers (29, 30). If young bees reduce the effectiveness of the foraging force, this may have serious consequences for the colony.

Here we used radio frequency tags (RFID) (31⇓–33) to continuously monitor flight activity of focal bees in experimental colonies to determine how foraging performance varied with the age at which bees commenced foraging. Our data revealed that several key aspects of forager performance were strongly age dependent. We then constructed a mathematical model of colony demography inspired by Khoury et al. (34, 35) to consider how this new information might change trajectories of colony growth or decline in chronically stressed colonies. We present an explanation for why colonies appear to change so rapidly from conditions of apparent health to colony failure, propose how to identify colonies at risk for failure, and investigate possible strategies to prevent colony failure.

Results

The RFID data showed when a bee was inside the hive and when outside, and therefore the time spent on orientation and foraging activities (Fig. 1). Foraging is typically preceded by nonforaging orientation flights (36, 37). The average cumulative duration of orientation flights has been estimated at 30.89 min (36); therefore, for the purpose of this study, we classed bees that had spent >30 min outside the hive as foragers.

Fig. 1.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Fig. 1.

Cumulative trip number (A and B) and cumulative time outside the hive (C and D) and distribution of ages at which each bee commenced foraging (E and F) for SCC and NDC. In all plots, each bee is color-coded according to the age (in days) at which it exceeded 30 mins cumulatively outside the hive and was classed as a forager [the legend: age > 30 mins (days)] to illustrate how, in both SCC and NDC, performance varied with age of foraging onset. More bees from the SCC (E) became foragers when less than 14 d than for the NDC (F) (χ2 = 20.47, df = 1, P = 0.0001).

To experimentally induce precocious foraging, we created three “single-cohort colonies” (SCC) in which all bees were 1-d-old adults at the time of establishment (24, 38, 39), and three colonies that had a normal-worker demography (NDC). SCC (24, 40⇓–42) is an established method for creating a colony with a younger age distribution of foragers. It has been argued that, for many social insects, early foraging by individuals might be a response to individual stress resulting in a shift in the demography of the foraging force (18): SCC provides a social manipulation to reduce the modal age of foragers by reducing social inhibition independent of any pathogen pressure. As expected, in SCC, more bees began foraging when less than 14 d old than in NDC (Fig. 1). A proportion of bees in SCC also start much later than normal. This may be due to the shift in colony needs within the hive once it commences brood production and bees are needed for nursing roles until the first new generation of bees has emerged (Fig. 1).

In both SCC and NDC, foraging performance varied with the age at which bees began foraging. Bees’ survival per hour of time spent outside the hive (Fig. 2 A and B) and number of completed trips per bee (Fig. 2 C and D) were greatest for bees that began foraging at about 14 d old. Polynomial survival regression (Fig. 2 A and B) accounting for colony type and replicate (colony type was treated as a fixed factor, whereas replicate was included as a random factor) showed a significant relationship between age at onset of foraging and total time spent outside the hive (χ2 = 34.04, P < 0.001, n = 749).

Fig. 2.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Fig. 2.

(A and B) Cumulative time outside the hive during the lifespan of all bees overlaid on predicted survival probabilities (from polynomial survival regression) dependent upon the age at which bees began foraging in SCC (A) and NDC (B). (C and D) Number of completed foraging trips in lifetime plotted against age at which bees commenced foraging in both SCC (C) and NDC (D). (E−G) Comparisons of total cumulative time outside the hive (E), total lifetime trip number (F), and mean trip duration (G) for bees that commenced foraging when <14 d old and ≥14 d (mean ± SE) in both SCC and NDC colonies. Numbers in bars are sample sizes. Generalized linear modeling analyses of effects of age of foraging onset and colony type on each of these parameters in Table S1.

The consequences of precocious foraging were severe. Bees that began foraging when <14 d old spent less time outside the hive, completed fewer flights, and performed longer foraging trips than bees that began foraging at ≥14 d (Fig. 2 E−G and Table S1). In both SCC and NDC, the likelihood of surviving past 30 min of flight activity (and becoming a forager) increased as age of first flight increased (Fig. 3 and Table S2).

Fig. 3.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Fig. 3.

Proportion of bees that successfully accumulate more than 30 min of time outside the hive plotted against age of first flight for SCC (A) and NDC (B). Summary of logistic regression analysis of these data in Table S2. Overlaid line is fit from logistic regression analysis of mortality during the first 30 min outside colony and age of first flight, green area is SE of fit (Table S2).

Modeling Consequences of Precocious Foraging for Colony Function.

Precocious foraging is a reaction to various acute stressors (5, 16⇓–18, 23, 24), which may be adaptive, as it rapidly replaces any losses to the foraging force to enable more food gathering. There is a risk, however, that chronic stress could lead to a progressively younger and less effective foraging force. To examine the consequences of chronic stress on the colony, we developed a compartment model of honey bee population dynamics, implemented as a set of delay differential equations, where survival and food collection per forager varied with age (Fig. 4).

Fig. 4.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Fig. 4.

Schematic representation of the model. Workers develop as brood, emerge as adult hive bees, and mature to foragers. Existing foragers inhibit hive bees from becoming foragers by social inhibition (20, 49). Purple lines represent consumption of food. Food also influences brood numbers indirectly because, in periods of low food influx, workers will cannibalize larvae (purple dotted line). Death rates during the transition from hive bee to forager, forager death rate, and the rate of food collection by foragers (indicated by red arrows) were functions of the age at onset of foraging. “Capped brood” appears in a box with a dashed border because it is not modeled explicitly, but the period that brood spends as capped pupae is modeled by a delay in the model between the time a larva enters pupation and emergence as an adult.

Modeling forager performance and survival as age dependent caused a dramatic change in trajectories of colony growth and failure (Figs. 5 A−D and 6) compared with earlier models in which performance was age independent (34). Model colonies buffered small changes in forager mortality to maintain a stable population (Fig. 5 A−C), but if forager mortality was chronically elevated above a threshold, then colony population was unsustainable. In conditions of chronic high mortality, the population stabilized for a period and brood rearing continued (Fig. 5D) but the foraging force became progressively younger and less effective. Ultimately, this triggered a very rapid terminal decline once the forager force became unable to sustain food levels. Food limitation prompted accelerated recruitment to the foraging force: Most bees became foragers and died soon after, leaving abandoned brood in the colony.

Fig. 5.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Fig. 5.

The effects of stress on the model colony shown as plots of populations of uncapped brood (dotted line), hive bees (dash-dot black line), and foragers (dashed line); food (solid line) and age at onset of foraging (dashed red line) against time for different values of the underlying death rate. Death rate is expressed as the ratio between death rate of the simulated hive and a healthy hive (mr). All hives start with 1 kg of food, 16,000 hive bees, 8,000 foragers, and no brood. (A) Plot for a healthy hive (mr = 1); (B) mr = 1.6; (C) mr = 1.9. At this value, the hive will collapse eventually but not when t < 150 days; (D) mr = 2.0. For this death rate, the forager population reached zero at about t = 99 d. At about 3 wk before collapse, the colony in D had 8,250 uncapped brood items, 6,780 hive bees, and 1,780 foragers. The mathematics are such that modeling could not continue beyond the point of zero foragers. Note that A and B are on a different vertical scale to C and D.

Fig. 6.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Fig. 6.

(A) Plot of the age at onset of foraging as a function of the death rate of foragers as a function of time for different values of mr (that is, the ratio between death rate of the simulated hive and a healthy hive). Each hive started with 1 kg of food, 16,000 hive bees, 8,000 foragers, and no brood, and the simulation was run until t = 300 d or the hive collapsed, whichever occurred sooner. The same values of mr were used as in Fig. 5 A−C: solid line, mr = 1; dotted line, mr = 1.6; dashed line, mr = 1.9. Each red cross is a time point at an integer value of t. The black cross is the initial value, and the black dot is the median value over all integer values of t. (B) Plot of the median age at onset of foraging (illustrated for three different values in A) as a function of the ratio of death rate of the simulated colony to the death rate of the healthy hive (mr). The median age of onset of foraging drops rapidly for values of mr that produce colonies that are close to collapse. When the colony collapses, then the median age of onset of foraging is 2 d, which is the minimum allowed in the model.

In model colonies, the time to failure was sensitive to the initial amounts of brood and food (Fig. 7). Increasing stored food at the start of the simulation extended the time to collapse (Fig. 7B). Adding some brood had a similar effect to a point, but, above a threshold, adding more brood accelerated colony collapse as the additional brood depleted food stores more quickly (Fig. 7A). Progressive supplemental feeding also extends colony life (Fig. 8B). Inhibiting precocious foraging, however, did not prevent colony collapse (Fig. 8 C and D). Although inhibiting the transition of hive bees to foragers kept bees safely in the hive, it also reduced the size of the foraging force and inhibited the collection of food. This accelerated the point at which food limited brood production and accelerated colony population decline.

Fig. 7.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Fig. 7.

Contour plots that show time to collapse as a function of mr (the ratio of the death rate to the death rate of a healthy hive) and (A) initial number of uncapped brood and (B) initial mass of stored food. The white regions in the plots represent colonies that did not collapse within 200 d. In A, the hive had initially 1 kg of stored food, and in B, the hive had no uncapped brood at t = 0. In A, the modeling also assumed that there was an equivalent number of capped brood items so that adults emerged continuously from the start of the simulation. The time to collapse increased as the brood numbers increased until it reached a maximum at about 25,000–30,000, depending on mr. If the initial brood numbers were increased by about 10,000 beyond the value where this maximum occurred, then the hive collapsed very rapidly, which suggests that there is a maximum number of brood which can be comfortably fed without putting a colony under extreme food stress. The time to collapse was not particularly sensitive to the mass of stored food (Fig. 7B); indeed, increasing the mass of stored food present at t = 0 beyond 2 kg made a negligible difference to the results. Increasing the initial levels of stored food from zero to 2 kg increased the time to collapse by about 50 d and, for mr between 1.8 and 1.95, could prevent collapse altogether.

Fig. 8.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Fig. 8.

The effect of in-hive additional feeding and treatment to delay onset of foraging in young bees. All these plots are for mr = 2. (A) Colony with no treatment. (This is the same plot as Fig. 5D.) (B) Colony with in-hive feeding only with CF = 60 grams per day. (C) Colony with pheromonal treatment only with Q = 500. (D) Colony with both in-hive feeding (CF = 60) and pheromonal treatment (Q = 500). The line styles are the same as Fig. 5 A−D. The modeling suggests that in-hive feeding is generally helpful in preventing vulnerable colonies from collapse. Inhibiting precocious foraging, however, hastens colony collapse.

Discussion

Individual honey bees react to a range of social, pathogen, and nutritional stressors by accelerating their behavioral development and foraging precociously (5, 16⇓–18, 23, 24), but this is not without significant cost to the individual bee, or potentially to its colony. Precocious foragers had a greatly reduced effective foraging life compared to normal-aged foragers (Figs. 2 and 3). We used modeling to explore the possible impacts of age-dependent foraging performance on a colony experiencing chronic stress. Initially, precocious foraging enabled colonies to buffer the impacts of stress. Colony populations stabilized for a period, and brood rearing continued (Fig. 5D), but, in effect, this concealed the true extent of the chronic stresses on the colony until it entered a final population decline as its buffering capacity was exhausted. The foraging force became progressively younger and less effective until it could no longer sustain food levels in the colony. This triggered a very rapid terminal decline as food limitation prompted accelerated recruitment of hive bees to the foraging force. Most bees that became foragers died soon after, leaving abandoned brood in the colony.

These model trajectories of population decline are strikingly similar to field reports of honey bee colony collapse (3) where colonies that appeared healthy were found later to be almost devoid of adult bees and with abandoned brood (2, 3). The results from this model shed light on why honey bee colonies often appear to undergo a very rapid state change from a condition of apparent health and growth to a crash in the adult population.

Several earlier models of honey bee colony population dynamics have been developed (34, 35, 43⇓⇓–46). Of these, the most relevant studies are those of Schmickl and Crailsheim (46), Makela et al. (45), and Khoury et al. (35), which have all modeled how changes in food and mortality can influence colony growth. However, to our knowledge, our approach is the first to recognize (based on our new data) how bee foraging performance varies with age. The decline in foraging performance as bees aged meant that stressed colonies with a younger foraging force became less efficient at gathering food. This interaction dramatically altered the trajectories of colony population collapse compared with earlier similar models that predicted a slow decay in adult bee populations rather than a sudden decline (35). Our model is the first, to our knowledge, to display dynamics of colony population collapse that are similar to field reports of colony collapse disorder.

In model colonies, a dramatic population collapse (similar to some reports of CCD) was seen only when colonies began with quite a low base of food stores and experienced chronically sustained forager death rates of more than twice that experienced by a healthy hive (Fig. 5D). This would be in the order of 30% of foragers dying each day. These are rather extreme conditions; hence, modeling suggests dramatic population collapses can be expected to be rare events. Such high rates of forager mortality could be caused by chronic diseases such as Varroensis or Nosema (4, 5, 19), or perhaps agrochemicals (6), which may explain why incidences of colony collapse have seemed to occur in sporadic clusters (3, 4). In model colonies, the time to failure was sensitive to the initial amounts of brood and low food stores (Fig. 7). This suggests that colonies may be at their most vulnerable when internal food stores and brood production are low. It may explain why failure is most common in spring as colonies attempt growth with food stores that have been depleted over winter (1). Our model proposes that food limitation imposed by inefficiency of a young foraging force would accelerate colony terminal decline. We note however that there are reports of CCD in which colonies died leaving some honey stores (3). In our model, we combined pollen (protein) and nectar/honey (carbohydrate) into a single category of food because too few field data exist on the fluxes of these in hives to consider them separately. A colony with abundant residual honey stores could still be nutrient-limited and fail to produce brood if it had no pollen. Better field data on the consequences of protein and carbohydrate limitation on colonies would certainly further improve our understanding of how nutritional factors contribute to colony growth and failure.

Effective interventions to stop colony failure hinge on early identification of vulnerable colonies. Beekeepers usually rely on snapshot assessments of brood production or colony honey stores to assess colony health, but our modeling suggests these parameters would be slow to react to a change in forager mortality (Fig. 5 A−D). Rather, our modeling suggests that forager mortality rate and age at onset of foraging may provide the best information on colony condition (Fig. 5 A−D).

Understanding the social dynamics that drive colony population decline is essential if effective preventative measures are to be developed. Our model framework allows exploration of the impacts of possible treatments. Here we focused on three (Figs. 7 and 8): supplemental feeding (either in the initial condition or progressive food supplementation throughout the simulation), adding brood, and blocking the precocious foraging response to stress. The latter might be achieved by supplementing colonies with pheromones that inhibit foraging in young bees (20). Of these possible treatments, progressive supplemental feeding was clearly the most effective in delaying or preventing colony population collapse (Fig. 8B). Adding brood also had a positive effect when a hive was marginal, but too much brood accelerated colony failure, because demands of the brood increased nutritional stress on the colony (Fig. 7A). Slowing worker behavioral development to prevent precocious foraging (20) was not at all effective, because this strategy also compromised food collection (Fig. 8 C and D).

Our modeling framework clarifies why honey bee colonies might collapse and refines hypotheses for how it might be prevented, but experimental studies are urgently needed to test the validity of these hypotheses. We recognize that there are many different stressors impacting bee colonies, and each has specific modes of action and symptoms. We do not try to capture this detail in our simple modeling approach, but our objective is to provide an overview of how colonies’ internal demographic processes might generally react to stressors.

Given the difficulties beekeepers have in identifying stressed colonies while there is still time to intervene, better strategies to identify vulnerable colonies and reduce colony losses are urgently needed. Our findings propose an explanation for the enigmatic phenomenon of rapid colony population collapse. In situations of chronically elevated forager loss, the social mechanisms that normally stabilize colony function instead accelerate colony failure by causing a destructive positive feedback response that increases forager mortality and decreases forager age, resulting in colony population collapse. Monitoring rate of forager mortality may give a more immediate assessment of colony health than assessments of brood and stored food.

Materials and Methods

Experiments were carried out between February 2012 and May 2013. Honey bees (Apis mellifera) were obtained from research apiaries maintained at Macquarie University (Sydney, Australia). Bees were housed in four-frame nucleus hives each containing ∼3,000 bees located inside a laboratory building but connected to the outside via a custom-designed entrance that separated entering and exiting bees into different channels, each with their own entrance. Single SCC and NDC colonies were constructed simultaneously and observed for 40 d. The experiment was replicated three times. Bees and brood to establish SCC and NDC colonies were sourced from the same seven research colonies. Each paired SCC and NDC was provided with new young sister queens. For NDC, ∼3,000 bees (estimated by weight) were shaken off brood and honey combs from donor colonies into the nucleus hive box, which was then sealed with mesh and placed in a dark room at 24 °C for 2 d before connecting to the entrance tunnel and adding a new queen. SCCs were constructed from ∼3,000 bees that were within 24 h of emergence from brood frames. To obtain newly emerged bees, brood combs were removed from research colonies and placed in an incubator maintained at 32 °C and 37% humidity overnight. Newly emerged bees were brushed from frames the following morning.

RFID System.

Between 500 and 1,000 Radio Frequency Identification (RFID; Invengo Technology) tagged bees were introduced to each colony. RFID were glued to the dorsal thoraxes of newly emerged bees within 12 h of emergence. Each nucleus colony was equipped with two RFID antennae placed within a modified entrance to monitor each of the entering and exiting channels.

RFID tags contained a unique 12-byte hexadecimal identifier that allowed us to individually track the life history of each bee. Data were collected on a PC (Windows) into a .csv file containing data for each successful trip for each bee, including the date and time the bee left the hive, the date and time the bee returned to the hive, and the RFID number for that bee. The RFID entrance tunnel was customized to detect >99% of all tagged bee entries and exits from the colony from adult emergence to the time of last detection. Trips that were filtered from the data included trips that lasted over 8 h (some bees spent one or more nights outside the hive, and these data skewed the comparisons) and those that lasted less than 10 s (trips less than 10 s long were considered misreads by the hardware and occurred infrequently and usually in addition to successfully logged trips). MATLAB (version 8.0) was used to analyze data and create figures.

Model Formulation.

The population of uncapped brood is denoted by B, hive bees by H, foragers by F, and the weight of stored food by f.

The rate of change of stored food is given by the rate that food is collected by foragers minus the rate that it is consumed by adult bees and brood,dfdt=cTN(a)F−γA(F+H)−γBB,

where cT is the quantity of food collected on a single trip (expressed in grams of food) and N(a) is the number of trips that a forager makes each day as a function of age of onset of foraging, a; γA and γB are the average weight of food consumed per day by an individual adult bee or brood item, respectively.

The rate of change of the population of uncapped brood is given bydBdt=Lf2f2+b2HH+v−ϕB.

The first term on the right-hand side models the production and survival of uncapped brood, which depends on the queen’s laying rate L and the amount of stored food and the number of hive bees. Here b and ν are constants. Brood leaves the uncapped brood class at a rate of ϕ when it pupates.

The rate of change of the hive bee population is given bydHdt=ϕB(t−τ)−R(H,F,f)H.

The first term models the emergence of adult bees that entered pupation τ days earlier. The second term models the recruitment of hive bees to foraging whereR(H,F,f)=αmin+αmax(b2b2+f2)−σ(FF+H).

The first two terms in the function R(H,F,f) represent the intrinsic rate of transition from hive bee to forager and the enhanced transition rate when stored food is scarce. The last term represents social inhibition.

The rate of change of the forager population is modeled asdFdt=T(a)R(H,F,f)H−mrM(a)F

where T(a) is the transition survival, represented as the proportion of bees that leave the hive bee class that successfully become foragers. M(a) is the death rate of foragers in a healthy hive as a function of age at onset of foraging a, and mr is the ratio of the death rate in a hive under stress to the death rate of a healthy hive. The age of onset of foraging is given by a = 1/R(H,F,f), and the foraging span is equal to 1/(mr M(a)).

The expressions for the functions N(a), T(a) and M(a) that depend on the age at onset of foraging areN(a)={−0.02(a−10)2+3.5for a≤100.015(a−10)2+3.5for a>10T(a)={−0.06(a−5)+0.5for a≤13.31for a>13.3M(a)=(a−a0)4+3(4.94+0.8a)(a−a0)4

where a0 = 2 is the minimum age at which hive bees can become foragers in this model.

Most parameter values are taken from ref. 34, where they replicate the field data of Harbo (47); that is, L = 2,000, f = 1/9, ν = 5,000, σ = 1.3, αmin = 0.25, αmax = 0.25, b = 500, γA = 0.007, and γB = 0.018. We also use τ = 12 as worker bees pupate for 12 d, and cT = 0.033. Estimating cT is not easy from available data. Harbo (47) does not measure this directly, but from his data, it can be inferred that foragers returned 0.1 g food each day to the colony (47, 48). From our flight data, the mean number of trips per day for all bees (including those that did not complete any foraging trips) was approximately three. Therefore, for our model, we set cT = 0.033.

In all of the simulations shown in Fig. 5, the age of onset of foraging changed over time and was a function of the actual death rate of foragers, mr M(a). We tracked these changes for mr = 1, 1.6, and 1.9 and recorded the age of onset of foraging for each day, that is, for each integer value of t. Although the age of onset of foraging did change with time, there were long periods when it changed very little and so we took the median value of age of onset of foraging over the simulation as a measure of the typical age of onset of foraging for each value of mr, which is the ratio of the death rate in the simulated hive to the death rate in a healthy hive. Fig. 6 shows three simulations, each corresponding to a different value of mr, and a plot of median age of onset of foraging vs. mr. The age of onset of foraging drops steadily as mr increases and, as mr approaches the critical value where collapse occurs before t = 300, the median age of onset of foraging plummets. Once collapse occurs within 300 d, the median age of onset is reduced to 2 d, which is the youngest age that bees are able to commence foraging in this model. The small kink in the curve of Fig. 6B at median age 13.3 is caused by the slope discontinuity of the function T(a) when a = 13.3.

We included the effect of extra, in-hive feeding in the model by adding an extra constant term CF to the equation for the rate of change of food so that the equation becamedfdt=cTN(a)F−γA(F+H)−γBB+CF.

The effect of pheromone treatment to mimic the presence of extra foragers and thus inhibit hive bee to forager transition was modeled by adding a constant Q in the numerator of the ratio of foragers to total adult bee in the transition function so thatR(H,F,f)=αmin+αmax(b2b2+f2)−σ(Q+FF+H).

Here Q effectively represents that number of foragers that the hive bees perceive to have been added due to the increased concentration of the social inhibition pheromone (Fig. 8 C and D).

Acknowledgments

We thank Allan Taylor for RFID software development. We thank Lars Chittka, Gene Robinson, and Mark Brown for critical feedback on the manuscript. A.B.B. received funding from Macquarie University, US Department of Agriculture (Grant 58-5342-3-004F), and The Hermon Slade Foundation (Grant HSF 11/7). M.R.M. acknowledges a Thompson Fellowship from the University of Sydney.

Footnotes

  • ↵1C.J.P. and E.S. contributed equally to this work.

  • ↵2To whom correspondence should be addressed. Email: andrew.barron{at}mq.edu.au.
  • Author contributions: C.J.P., E.S., and A.B.B. designed research; C.J.P., E.S., M.R.M., and A.B.B. performed research; C.J.P., E.S., and M.R.M. analyzed data; and C.J.P., E.S., M.R.M., and A.B.B. wrote the paper.

  • The authors declare no conflict of interest.

  • This article is a PNAS Direct Submission.

  • This article contains supporting information online at www.pnas.org/lookup/suppl/doi:10.1073/pnas.1422089112/-/DCSupplemental.

View Abstract

References

  1. ↵
    1. Steinhauer NA, et al.
    (2014) A national survey of managed honey bee 2012-2013 annual colony losses in the USA: Results from the Bee Informed Partnership. J Apic Res 53(1):1–18
    .
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  2. ↵
    1. Oldroyd BP
    (2007) What’s killing American honey bees? PLoS Biol 5(6):e168
    .
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  3. ↵
    1. Vanengelsdorp D, et al.
    (2009) Colony collapse disorder: A descriptive study. PLoS ONE 4(8):e6481
    .
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  4. ↵
    1. Ratnieks FLW,
    2. Carreck NL
    (2010) Ecology. Clarity on honey bee collapse? Science 327(5962):152–153
    .
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  5. ↵
    1. Higes M, et al.
    (2008) How natural infection by Nosema ceranae causes honeybee colony collapse. Environ Microbiol 10(10):2659–2669
    .
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  6. ↵
    1. Henry M, et al.
    (2012) A common pesticide decreases foraging success and survival in honey bees. Science 336(6079):348–350
    .
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  7. ↵
    1. Huang Z
    (2012) Pollen nutrition affects honey bee stress resistance. Terrest Arth Rev 5:175–189
    .
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  8. ↵
    1. Neumann P,
    2. Carreck NL
    (2010) Honey bee colony losses. J Apic Res 49(1):1–6
    .
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  9. ↵
    1. van Engelsdorp D,
    2. Hayes J Jr,
    3. Underwood RM,
    4. Pettis J
    (2008) A survey of honey bee colony losses in the U.S., fall 2007 to spring 2008. PLoS ONE 3(12):e4071
    .
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  10. ↵
    1. Seeley TD
    (1989) The honey bee colony as a superorganism. Am Sci 77(6):546–553
    .
    OpenUrl
  11. ↵
    1. Seeley TD
    (1995) The Wisdom of the Hive (Harvard University Press, Cambridge, MA)
    .
  12. ↵
    1. Winston ML
    (1987) The Biology of the Honey Bee (Harvard University Press, Cambridge, MA)
    .
  13. ↵
    1. Seeley TD
    (1982) Adaptive significance of the age polyethism schedule in honeybee colonies. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 11(4):287–293
    .
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  14. ↵
    1. Robinson GE
    (1987) Regulation of honey bee age polyethism by juvenile hormone. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 20(5):329–338
    .
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  15. ↵
    1. Sekiguchi K,
    2. Sakagami SF
    (1966) Structure of foraging population and related problems in the honey bee, with considerations on the division of labour in bee colonies. Hokkaido Natl Agric Exper Stn Rep 69:1–65
    .
    OpenUrl
  16. ↵
    1. Toth AL,
    2. Robinson GE
    (2005) Worker nutrition and division of labour in honeybees. Anim Behav 69:427–435
    .
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  17. ↵
    1. Schulz DJ,
    2. Huang Z-Y,
    3. Robinson GE
    (1998) Effects of colony food shortage on behavioral development in honey bees. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 42(5):295–303
    .
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  18. ↵
    1. Woyciechowski M,
    2. Moron D
    (2009) Life expectancy and onset of foraging in the honeybee (Apis mellifera). Ins. Soc 56(2):193–201
    .
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  19. ↵
    1. Goblirsch M,
    2. Huang ZY,
    3. Spivak M
    (2013) Physiological and behavioral changes in honey bees (Apis mellifera) induced by Nosema ceranae infection. PLoS ONE 8(3):e58165
    .
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  20. ↵
    1. Leoncini I, et al.
    (2004) Regulation of behavioral maturation by a primer pheromone produced by adult worker honey bees. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 101(50):17559–17564
    .
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  21. ↵
    1. Beshers SN,
    2. Fewell JH
    (2001) Models of division of labor in social insects. Annu Rev Entomol 46:413–440
    .
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  22. ↵
    1. Leoncini I,
    2. Crauser D,
    3. Robinson GE,
    4. Le Conte Y
    (2004) Worker-worker inhibition of honey bee behavioural development independent of queen and brood. Insectes Sociaux 51(4):392–394
    .
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  23. ↵
    1. Robinson GE,
    2. Page RE,
    3. Huang Z-Y
    (1994) Temporal polyethism in social insects is a developmental process. Anim Behav 48(2):467–469
    .
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  24. ↵
    1. Huang Z-Y,
    2. Robinson GE
    (1996) Regulation of honey bee division of labor by colony age demography. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 39:147–158
    .
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  25. ↵
    1. Free JB
    (1961) Hypopharyngeal gland development and division of labor in honey-bee (Apis mellifera L.) colonies. Proc R Entomol Soc London Ser A 36(1-3):5–8
    .
    OpenUrl
  26. ↵
    1. Janmaat AF,
    2. Winston ML
    (2000) The influence of pollen storage area and Varroa jacobsoni Oudemans parasitism on temporal caste structure in honey bees (Apis mellifera L.). Insectes Sociaux 47(2):177–182
    .
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  27. ↵
    1. Fergusson LA,
    2. Winston ML
    (1988) The influence of wax deprivation on temporal polyethism in honey bee (Apis mellifera L.) colonies. Can J Zool 66(9):1997–2001
    .
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  28. ↵
    1. Vance JT,
    2. Williams JB,
    3. Elekonich MM,
    4. Roberts SP
    (2009) The effects of age and behavioral development on honey bee (Apis mellifera) flight performance. J Exp Biol 212(Pt 16):2604–2611
    .
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  29. ↵
    1. Schippers M-P, et al.
    (2006) Lifetime performance in foraging honeybees: Behaviour and physiology. J Exp Biol 209(Pt 19):3828–3836
    .
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  30. ↵
    1. Schippers M-P,
    2. Dukas R,
    3. McClelland GB
    (2010) Lifetime- and caste-specific changes in flight metabolic rate and muscle biochemistry of honeybees, Apis mellifera. J Comp Physiol B 180(1):45–55
    .
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  31. ↵
    1. Streit S,
    2. Bock F,
    3. Pirk CW,
    4. Tautz J
    (2003) Automatic life-long monitoring of individual insect behaviour now possible. Zoology (Jena) 106(3):169–171
    .
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  32. ↵
    1. He X, et al.
    (2013) Assessment of flight activity and homing ability in Asian and European honey bee species, Apis cerana and Apis mellifera, measured with radio frequency tags. Apidologie 44(1):38–51
    .
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  33. ↵
    1. Molet M,
    2. Chittka L,
    3. Stelzer RJ,
    4. Streit S,
    5. Raine NE
    (2008) Colony nutritional status modulates worker responses to foraging recruitment pheromone in the bumblebee Bombus terrestris. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 62(12):1919–1926
    .
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  34. ↵
    1. Khoury DS,
    2. Barron AB,
    3. Myerscough MR
    (2013) Modelling food and population dynamics in honey bee colonies. PLoS ONE 8(5):e59084
    .
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  35. ↵
    1. Khoury DS,
    2. Myerscough MR,
    3. Barron AB
    (2011) A quantitative model of honey bee colony population dynamics. PLoS ONE 6(4):e18491
    .
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  36. ↵
    1. Capaldi EA, et al.
    (2000) Ontogeny of orientation flight in the honeybee revealed by harmonic radar. Nature 403(6769):537–540
    .
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  37. ↵
    1. Capaldi EA,
    2. Dyer FC
    (1999) The role of orientation flights on homing performance in honeybees. J Exp Biol 202(Pt 12):1655–1666
    .
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  38. ↵
    1. Barron AB,
    2. Schulz DJ,
    3. Robinson GE
    (2002) Octopamine modulates responsiveness to foraging-related stimuli in honey bees (Apis mellifera). J Comp Physiol A Neuroethol Sens Neural Behav Physiol 188(8):603–610
    .
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  39. ↵
    1. Schulz DJ,
    2. Robinson GE
    (2001) Octopamine influences division of labor in honey bee colonies. J Comp Physiol A Neuroethol Sens Neural Behav Physiol 187(1):53–61
    .
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  40. ↵
    1. Detrain C,
    2. Deneubourg JL,
    3. Pasteels JM
    1. Huang Z-Y,
    2. Robinson GE
    (1999) Social control of division of labor in honey bee colonies. Information Processing in Social Insects, eds Detrain C, Deneubourg JL, Pasteels JM (Birkhauser, Basel), pp 165–187
    .
  41. ↵
    1. Huang Z-Y,
    2. Robinson GE,
    3. Borst DW
    (1994) Physiological correlates of division of labor among similarly aged honey bees. J Comp Physiol A Neuroethol Sens Neural Behav Physiol 174(6):731–739
    .
    OpenUrlPubMed
  42. ↵
    1. Fahrbach SE,
    2. Robinson GE
    (1996) Juvenile hormone, behavioral maturation, and brain structure in the honey bee. Dev Neurosci 18(1-2):102–114
    .
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  43. ↵
    1. DeGrandi-Hoffman G,
    2. Curry R
    (2004) A mathematical model of varroa mite (Varroa destructor Anderson and Trueman) and honeybee (Apis mellifera L.) population dynamics. Int J Acarol 30(3):259–274
    .
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  44. ↵
    1. DeGrandi-Hoffman G,
    2. Curry R
    (2005) The population dynamics of Varroa mites in honey bee colonies: Part I—The VARROAPOP program. Am Bee J 145(7):592–595
    .
    OpenUrl
  45. ↵
    1. Makela ME,
    2. Rowell GA,
    3. Sames WJ IV,
    4. Wilson LT
    (1993) An object-oriented intracolonial and population level model of honey bees based on behaviors of European and Africanized subspecies. Ecol Modell 67(2-4):259–284
    .
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  46. ↵
    1. Schmickl T,
    2. Crailsheim K
    (2007) HoPoMo: A model of honeybee intracolonial population dynamics and resource management. Ecol Modell 204(1-2):219–245
    .
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  47. ↵
    1. Harbo JR
    (1986) Effect of population size on brood production, worker survival and honey gain in colonies of honeybees. J Apic Res 25(1):22–29
    .
    OpenUrl
  48. ↵
    1. Russell S,
    2. Barron AB,
    3. Harris D
    (2013) Dynamic modelling of honey bee (Apis mellifera) colony growth and failure. Ecol Modell 265:158–169
    .
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  49. ↵
    1. Beshers SN,
    2. Huang ZY,
    3. Oono Y,
    4. Robinson GE
    (2001) Social inhibition and the regulation of temporal polyethism in honey bees. J Theor Biol 213(3):461–479
    .
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
PreviousNext
Back to top
Article Alerts
Email Article

Thank you for your interest in spreading the word on PNAS.

NOTE: We only request your email address so that the person you are recommending the page to knows that you wanted them to see it, and that it is not junk mail. We do not capture any email address.

Enter multiple addresses on separate lines or separate them with commas.
Rapid behavioral maturation accelerates failure of stressed honey bee colonies
(Your Name) has sent you a message from PNAS
(Your Name) thought you would like to see the PNAS web site.
CAPTCHA
This question is for testing whether or not you are a human visitor and to prevent automated spam submissions.
Citation Tools
Failure of stressed honey bee colonies
Clint J. Perry, Eirik Søvik, Mary R. Myerscough, Andrew B. Barron
Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences Mar 2015, 112 (11) 3427-3432; DOI: 10.1073/pnas.1422089112

Citation Manager Formats

  • BibTeX
  • Bookends
  • EasyBib
  • EndNote (tagged)
  • EndNote 8 (xml)
  • Medlars
  • Mendeley
  • Papers
  • RefWorks Tagged
  • Ref Manager
  • RIS
  • Zotero
Request Permissions
Share
Failure of stressed honey bee colonies
Clint J. Perry, Eirik Søvik, Mary R. Myerscough, Andrew B. Barron
Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences Mar 2015, 112 (11) 3427-3432; DOI: 10.1073/pnas.1422089112
Digg logo Reddit logo Twitter logo Facebook logo Google logo Mendeley logo
  • Tweet Widget
  • Facebook Like
  • Mendeley logo Mendeley
Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences: 112 (11)
Table of Contents

Submit

Sign up for Article Alerts

Article Classifications

  • Biological Sciences
  • Ecology

Jump to section

  • Article
    • Abstract
    • Results
    • Discussion
    • Materials and Methods
    • Acknowledgments
    • Footnotes
    • References
  • Figures & SI
  • Info & Metrics
  • PDF

You May Also be Interested in

Abstract depiction of a guitar and musical note
Science & Culture: At the nexus of music and medicine, some see disease treatments
Although the evidence is still limited, a growing body of research suggests music may have beneficial effects for diseases such as Parkinson’s.
Image credit: Shutterstock/agsandrew.
Large piece of gold
News Feature: Tracing gold's cosmic origins
Astronomers thought they’d finally figured out where gold and other heavy elements in the universe came from. In light of recent results, they’re not so sure.
Image credit: Science Source/Tom McHugh.
Dancers in red dresses
Journal Club: Friends appear to share patterns of brain activity
Researchers are still trying to understand what causes this strong correlation between neural and social networks.
Image credit: Shutterstock/Yeongsik Im.
Yellow emoticons
Learning the language of facial expressions
Aleix Martinez explains why facial expressions often are not accurate indicators of emotion.
Listen
Past PodcastsSubscribe
Goats standing in a pin
Transplantation of sperm-producing stem cells
CRISPR-Cas9 gene editing can improve the effectiveness of spermatogonial stem cell transplantation in mice and livestock, a study finds.
Image credit: Jon M. Oatley.

Similar Articles

Site Logo
Powered by HighWire
  • Submit Manuscript
  • Twitter
  • Facebook
  • RSS Feeds
  • Email Alerts

Articles

  • Current Issue
  • Special Feature Articles – Most Recent
  • List of Issues

PNAS Portals

  • Anthropology
  • Chemistry
  • Classics
  • Front Matter
  • Physics
  • Sustainability Science
  • Teaching Resources

Information

  • Authors
  • Editorial Board
  • Reviewers
  • Librarians
  • Press
  • Site Map
  • PNAS Updates

Feedback    Privacy/Legal

Copyright © 2021 National Academy of Sciences. Online ISSN 1091-6490