Skip to main content

Main menu

  • Home
  • Articles
    • Current
    • Special Feature Articles - Most Recent
    • Special Features
    • Colloquia
    • Collected Articles
    • PNAS Classics
    • List of Issues
  • Front Matter
    • Front Matter Portal
    • Journal Club
  • News
    • For the Press
    • This Week In PNAS
    • PNAS in the News
  • Podcasts
  • Authors
    • Information for Authors
    • Editorial and Journal Policies
    • Submission Procedures
    • Fees and Licenses
  • Submit
  • Submit
  • About
    • Editorial Board
    • PNAS Staff
    • FAQ
    • Accessibility Statement
    • Rights and Permissions
    • Site Map
  • Contact
  • Journal Club
  • Subscribe
    • Subscription Rates
    • Subscriptions FAQ
    • Open Access
    • Recommend PNAS to Your Librarian

User menu

  • Log in
  • My Cart

Search

  • Advanced search
Home
Home
  • Log in
  • My Cart

Advanced Search

  • Home
  • Articles
    • Current
    • Special Feature Articles - Most Recent
    • Special Features
    • Colloquia
    • Collected Articles
    • PNAS Classics
    • List of Issues
  • Front Matter
    • Front Matter Portal
    • Journal Club
  • News
    • For the Press
    • This Week In PNAS
    • PNAS in the News
  • Podcasts
  • Authors
    • Information for Authors
    • Editorial and Journal Policies
    • Submission Procedures
    • Fees and Licenses
  • Submit
Research Article

Individual olfactory perception reveals meaningful nonolfactory genetic information

Lavi Secundo, Kobi Snitz, Kineret Weissler, Liron Pinchover, Yehuda Shoenfeld, Ron Loewenthal, Nancy Agmon-Levin, View ORCID ProfileIdan Frumin, Dana Bar-Zvi, Sagit Shushan, and Noam Sobel
  1. aDepartment of Neurobiology, Weizmann Institute of Science, Rehovot 7610001, Israel;
  2. bZabludowicz Center for Autoimmune Diseases, Sheba Medical Center, Tel Hashomer 5265601, Israel;
  3. cSackler Faculty of Medicine, Tel-Aviv University, Tel Aviv 6997801, Israel;
  4. dTissue Typing Laboratory, Sheba Medical Center, Tel Hashomer 5265601, Israel

See allHide authors and affiliations

PNAS July 14, 2015 112 (28) 8750-8755; first published June 22, 2015; https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1424826112
Lavi Secundo
aDepartment of Neurobiology, Weizmann Institute of Science, Rehovot 7610001, Israel;
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
  • For correspondence: noam.sobel@weizmann.ac.il lavisec@gmail.com
Kobi Snitz
aDepartment of Neurobiology, Weizmann Institute of Science, Rehovot 7610001, Israel;
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Kineret Weissler
aDepartment of Neurobiology, Weizmann Institute of Science, Rehovot 7610001, Israel;
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Liron Pinchover
aDepartment of Neurobiology, Weizmann Institute of Science, Rehovot 7610001, Israel;
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Yehuda Shoenfeld
bZabludowicz Center for Autoimmune Diseases, Sheba Medical Center, Tel Hashomer 5265601, Israel;
cSackler Faculty of Medicine, Tel-Aviv University, Tel Aviv 6997801, Israel;
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Ron Loewenthal
dTissue Typing Laboratory, Sheba Medical Center, Tel Hashomer 5265601, Israel
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Nancy Agmon-Levin
bZabludowicz Center for Autoimmune Diseases, Sheba Medical Center, Tel Hashomer 5265601, Israel;
cSackler Faculty of Medicine, Tel-Aviv University, Tel Aviv 6997801, Israel;
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Idan Frumin
aDepartment of Neurobiology, Weizmann Institute of Science, Rehovot 7610001, Israel;
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
  • ORCID record for Idan Frumin
Dana Bar-Zvi
aDepartment of Neurobiology, Weizmann Institute of Science, Rehovot 7610001, Israel;
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Sagit Shushan
aDepartment of Neurobiology, Weizmann Institute of Science, Rehovot 7610001, Israel;
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Noam Sobel
aDepartment of Neurobiology, Weizmann Institute of Science, Rehovot 7610001, Israel;
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
  • For correspondence: noam.sobel@weizmann.ac.il lavisec@gmail.com
  1. Edited by John G. Hildebrand, University of Arizona, Tucson, AZ, and approved May 13, 2015 (received for review December 28, 2014)

  • Article
  • Figures & SI
  • Info & Metrics
  • PDF
Loading

Significance

Cyrano de Bergerac observed that “a large nose is the mark of a witty, courteous, affable, generous and liberal man.” Here we report that individual noses, not how they look but rather how they function, indeed say a lot about a person. Each person expresses a nearly unique set of different olfactory receptor genes, and therefore may have unique olfactory perception. We developed a highly sensitive perceptual test we call the “olfactory fingerprint” that captures this variability. Individual olfactory fingerprints are therefore mirrors of individual olfactory genomes. We demonstrate that such fingerprints predict genetic features linked to the olfactory system, such as aspects of immune regulation. Thus, a precise measure of olfactory perception reveals meaningful nonolfactory genetic information.

Abstract

Each person expresses a potentially unique subset of ∼400 different olfactory receptor subtypes. Given that the receptors we express partially determine the odors we smell, it follows that each person may have a unique nose; to capture this, we devised a sensitive test of olfactory perception we termed the “olfactory fingerprint.” Olfactory fingerprints relied on matrices of perceived odorant similarity derived from descriptors applied to the odorants. We initially fingerprinted 89 individuals using 28 odors and 54 descriptors. We found that each person had a unique olfactory fingerprint (P < 10−10), which was odor specific but descriptor independent. We could identify individuals from this pool using randomly selected sets of 7 odors and 11 descriptors alone. Extrapolating from this data, we determined that using 34 odors and 35 descriptors we could individually identify each of the 7 billion people on earth. Olfactory perception, however, fluctuates over time, calling into question our proposed perceptual readout of presumably stable genetic makeup. To test whether fingerprints remain informative despite this temporal fluctuation, building on the linkage between olfactory receptors and HLA, we hypothesized that olfactory perception may relate to HLA. We obtained olfactory fingerprints and HLA typing for 130 individuals, and found that olfactory fingerprint matching using only four odorants was significantly related to HLA matching (P < 10−4), such that olfactory fingerprints can save 32% of HLA tests in a population screen (P < 10−6). In conclusion, a precise measure of olfactory perception reveals meaningful nonolfactory genetic information.

  • olfactory perception
  • HLA
  • MHC
  • olfactory genetics
  • autoimmunity

Humans have superb olfactory discrimination (1, 2), likely reflecting the combined output of ∼400 different subtypes of olfactory receptors (3). Specific olfactory receptors are likely responsible for specific aspects of olfactory perception (4⇓⇓–7). Because any two individuals differ by ∼30% of their olfactory receptor subtype genome (8), this renders a potentially unique nose for each person (4, 9). If we could capture this uniqueness with a perceptual test, a sort of perceptual olfactory fingerprint, this should then be informative on the underlying individual olfactory receptor subtype genome. The notion of a psychophysical test informing on underlying genes is of course well known from vision, where color blindness charts inform us about genes coding for different opsins in the retina (10). In olfaction, specific anosmias can also point to alterations in a specific gene (4, 11, 12). Unlike in color vision or specific anosmias, the olfactory fingerprints we propose will likely not inform on a specific gene, but they may link overall perception to overall genetic makeup.

Moreover, because the olfactory receptor genome is linked to various other genetic loci, this perceptual test may inform on genetic makeup beyond olfaction alone. For example, the olfactory receptor subtype genome is linked to MHC genetic makeup (13⇓–15). The human counterpart of MHC is known as HLA. HLA genes encode cell-surface glycoproteins that bind short peptides and make them available to T lymphocytes. Through this mechanism, HLA genes influence immunological self/non–self-discrimination and subsequently, tissue rejection and immune recognition of infectious disease. Therefore, HLA tests are routinely conducted to match up donors and recipients of organ and bone marrow transplants. Given an olfactory receptor genome to HLA link, we hypothesized that if olfactory fingerprints mirror olfactory receptor repertoire, they should be indicative of HLA. It has long been known that information on HLA is available in body odor (16, 17). Linking HLA to the sense of smell alone (18, 19) would close a functional loop potentially subserving behavioral mechanisms of selection based on the sense of smell (20). With all this in mind, we set out to test if we could generate individual olfactory fingerprints, and whether these inform on HLA.

Results

Olfactory Fingerprints Were Based on Matrices of Perceived Odorant Similarity.

We derived fingerprints using a matrix of perceived odor similarities (21). We used a palette of 28 odors (list of experiment 1A odors in Table S1) that provided for 378 pairwise similarities (28 × 27/2 = 378). Such a 378-dimensional olfactory fingerprint allows for potential characterization of a practically infinite number of individuals. Rather than directly obtaining pairwise similarity estimates, we derived pairwise similarity from 54 different descriptors applied to each odorant alone (list of experiment 1A descriptors in Table S2). We opted for derived over direct similarity ratings because though the two are highly correlated (22), derived similarity is potentially much easier and faster to obtain. For example, direct similarity ratings of the 378 possible odorant pairs in this study would entail 756 individual odorant presentations (A vs. B × 378) each followed by one question: rate similarity. Such a large number of odorant presentations (756) are impractical in an experiment with a human subject. In contrast, 378 derived similarity ratings entail 28 odorant presentations each followed by several questions (e.g., rate how coconut, rate how lemony), and derivation of similarity from the answers; this remains a feasible experiment, and moreover, the number of questions can later be reduced based on the current analysis. Importantly, we acknowledge that the use of odorant descriptors likely entails personal (23) and cultural differences (24), yet this method does not assume or rely on any agreement across individuals in the application of a given descriptor (Fig. 1 contains a schematic of fingerprint acquisition that explains this issue). Thus, we calculated an individual olfactory fingerprint by computing all of the pairwise distances (i.e., derived similarity) between all odorants rated. For a measure of distance between odorant k and odorant m we used the following equation:distancek,m=∑i=1n(Pik−Pim)2n,[1]where Pik is the perceptual rating of odorant k using descriptor i, and Pim is the perceptual rating of odorant m using descriptor i. In words, the distance between odorant k and odorant m is the square root of the mean of the squared difference between all perceptual ratings for those two odorants. Once all of the pairwise distances are computed, we can notate an olfactory fingerprint in the following form:FPk+m−1=distancek,m,[2]where k = 1…N − 1 and m = k + 1…N. In words, each element of the olfactory fingerprint is the distance between pairs of odorants, where each pairwise distance is calculated only once (because distancek,m = distancem,k), and the distance between an odorant to itself is omitted (because distancek,k = 0). Consequently, if we use N odors to construct an olfactory fingerprint, the resulting olfactory fingerprint will have N×(N−1)2 elements. To negate the impact of variance in the number of descriptors used by subjects (e.g., one subject may have used only 50 of the 54 descriptors, and another only 45, leaving all other descriptors unrated, see Figs. S1 and S2 for descriptor use), we normalized each distance between odor pairs by the number of mutual descriptors used for this pair. For example, if odors A and B were rated along 50 descriptors by subject 1, and 45 descriptors by subject 2, dividing the distance between odor pairs by 50 for subject 1 and by 45 for subject 2 enables direct comparison of the perceived distances between odors A and B for subjects 1 and 2.

Fig. 1.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Fig. 1.

A schematic for obtaining olfactory fingerprints. Odorant ratings along visual analog scales (VAS) are converted into numbers reflecting location on the VAS line. Pairwise odorant similarity is calculated as the distance across all descriptors used, and pairwise person similarity is calculated as the correlation across individuals. All this ensures that fingerprints are odorant specific but descriptor independent. For example, imagine John who was raised on an island smelling real coconuts, and Jane who knows coconut only from Bounty chocolate bars. Coconut is very different for these two individuals. John may rate odor A as 47% like coconut, odor B as 49% like coconut, and odor C as 4% like coconut. Thus, odors A and B are highly similar, and both are dissimilar from odor C. Jane may rate same odor A as 21% like coconut, odor B as 19% like coconut, and odor C as 100% like coconut. Once again, odors A and B are highly similar, and both are dissimilar from odor C. Thus, John and Jane will have very similar olfactory fingerprints derived from these three odorants and one descriptor, even though they are in total disagreement as to what coconut smell is like.

As noted, such derivations of similarity do not assume that people agree with each other along any given descriptor (e.g., how coconut?), they only assume that a person agrees with him/herself (Fig. 1). In other words, such similarity matrices are odor dependent but descriptor independent, which is here verified in experiments where fingerprints for the same individual obtained with the same odorants but different descriptors remained highly correlated. Finally, for fingerprint visualization purposes alone, we first generated a rectangular image by interpolating the 378 fingerprint values to 500 values. We then projected these values onto a 25 × 20 matrix, and conducted a 2D interpolation providing a 2 × 2 K matrix, which we then projected onto a semicircle (Fig. 2 A–D). We applied these methods in 89 subjects (40 women, mean age = 25.7 ± 3.1 y).

Fig. 2.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Fig. 2.

Olfactory fingerprints were consistent within individuals and different across individuals. To visualize fingerprints, we interpolated the 378 pairwise similarities (Methods). (A) An example olfactory fingerprint of one individual. (B) The olfactory fingerprint of the same individual from A, but here derived using a different set of nonoverlapping descriptors. (C) The olfactory fingerprint of the same individual from A and B, but here obtained separately 16 d later. (D) The olfactory fingerprint of a different individual. Correlations: A ⇔ B, r = 0.89; A ⇔ C, r = 0.61; A ⇔ D, r = 0.25. (E) The best-correlated descriptors across all odors. (F) Heat-map matrix of distances between all subject pairs. (G) Histogram of correlation coefficients of all non-self–self pairs.

Olfactory Fingerprints Were Individually Specific.

Previous studies implied that gross olfactory perception is similar across individuals (25), and to test whether this was true here as well, we calculated the mean rating along each descriptor for each odorant, and then calculated the correlation of each individual with the mean. Note that here we are not comparing fingerprints, but rather ratings along specific descriptors; this revealed that individuals are indeed similar to each other in their gross perception. For example, in all top 10 correlated descriptors the mean perception was a very good predictor of individual perception (all r > 0.68; Fig. 2E). Moreover, the primary perceptual dimension of odor, namely pleasantness (2, 26) was particularly highly correlated across individuals and odors, at r = 0.73 ± 0.1 (Fig. 2E). In other words, the average description of an odorant using common descriptors is a pretty good estimation of what any given individual will say about that odor.

We next set out to determine whether despite this gross agreement on odor perception, our sensitive perceptual test will uncover a unique olfactory perceptual fingerprint. Consistent with our hypothesis, we found that across the 89 subjects we tested, no two subjects had the same fingerprint (e.g., Fig. 2A vs. Fig. 2 D, F, and G). We calculated all pairwise distances between olfactory fingerprints (using Pearson’s correlation) and found that the average correlation across individuals (omitting self–self) was r = 0.3 ± 0.13 (Fig. 2G). In other words, whereas gross perception was similar across individuals, a fine measure of perception revealed individual perception for each of the 89 subjects we tested.

Olfactory Fingerprints Were Independent of Descriptor Identity.

The emergence of 89 individual fingerprints alone does not necessarily imply that we captured individual olfactory perception because one can potentially obtain such a result (89 different fingerprints) with random odor similarity ratings. To verify that olfactory fingerprints captured individual olfactory perception, for each of the 89 participants we next generated two alternative fingerprints, A and B, each using a random independent half of the descriptors used to derive similarity. We computed a matrix of all of the pairwise distances between olfactory fingerprints A and B (89 × 89) and tested whether the distance of a subject from him/herself (using different descriptors) was smaller than the distance of a subject to anyone else. In other words, whether, despite the use of different descriptors each time, a subject remained more similar to him/herself than to anyone else (distance between fingerprints was estimated by correlation, see SI Text and Figs. S3–S5 for impact of alternative methods); we repeated this 1,000 times, each time selecting a different set of nonoverlapping descriptors for fingerprints A and B, and assessed the distances between fingerprints. We again plotted the heat-map correlation matrix of each individual with all other individuals, this time, however, each pairwise distance is computed as the correlation between olfactory fingerprints A and B (Fig. 3A). We found that the distance between an individual’s two fingerprints based on the same odors but different descriptors (the diagonal in Fig. 3A, and Fig. 2A vs. Fig. 2B) was overwhelmingly smaller than the average distance between two different individuals [nondiagonal values in Fig. 3A and Fig. 2A vs. Fig. 2D; mean difference between an individual’s two fingerprints r = 0.75 ± 0.025, mean difference between two different individuals’ fingerprints r = 0.25 ± 0.008, paired t test, t(999) = 885.7, P < 10−10; Fig. 3B]. We also found that the maximum of the heat-map correlation matrix lies on the diagonal (i.e., self–self correlation). In other words, olfactory fingerprints A and B of the same individual were always more similar than olfactory fingerprints of different individuals. We repeated the analysis of this data using a permutational multivariate ANOVA (PERMANOVA) to compare the distance between an individual’s two fingerprints based on the same odors but different descriptors (e.g., Fig. 2A vs. Fig. 2B) to the distance between two different individuals (e.g., Fig. 2A vs. Fig. 2D). PERMANOVA implements a flexible nonparametric distance-based analog of analysis of variance for multivariate data that provides a distribution-free means of testing differences between treatments in their multivariate profile (27). Again, the mean difference between an individual’s two fingerprints was r = 0.75 ± 0.025, whereas the mean difference between two different individuals’ fingerprints was r = 0.25 ± 0.008 (PERMNOV test, pseudo F = 8.16, P < 10−6; Fig. S3C). Thus, the fingerprint genuinely captured personal identity, and a subject’s odorant-specific olfactory fingerprint remains unique even when different descriptors are used to construct it. Once we established the main effect using PERMANOVA, we set out to extrapolate the ability of the olfactory fingerprint to identify an individual beyond our sample; for this, one needs to calculate whether a subject’s correlation to him/herself (calculated between fingerprints A and B) is within the distribution of correlations of a subject to all other subjects (between fingerprint A of a subject to fingerprint A of all other subjects). In other words, to conclude that a subject has a unique fingerprint, the intersubject correlation should not belong (low probability) to the distribution of intrasubject correlations. We fitted a Gaussian to the intrasubject distances distribution, then calculated how many SDs a intersubject’s score lies from the mean of the distribution of intrasubject scores (i.e., z value); from this, we determined the probability of a subject’s correlations to him/herself to be within the distribution of correlations of a subject to all other subjects (i.e., P value). Finally, we averaged all of the individual z-value scores and calculated the overall P value. The distribution of correlations of a subject to all other subjects is not Gaussian, and the subject’s correlation to him/herself is limited by 1 (or −1), hence other metrics for distance between subjects may yield modestly different results (see SI Text for caveats of the Z value and for impact of alternative methods). We repeated this procedure 1,000 times, each time randomly halving the descriptors used to derive similarity (with one half used to generate fingerprint A and the other half used to generate fingerprint B), and averaged across all iterations and all subjects. We obtained an average Z value of 4.9 that corresponds to an ability to use the 28-odor olfactory fingerprint to identify one person of ∼2 million individuals.

Fig. 3.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Fig. 3.

Olfactory fingerprints are independent of descriptor identity. (A) Heat-map matrix of distances between fingerprints A and B for 89 subjects, where A and B were derived using the same odorants but different descriptors. The diagonal represents the correlation of a subject to him/herself. (B) Violin plots comparing correlation coefficients of all self–self pairs (using different descriptors) to all self–other pairs, the distribution of correlation coefficients of self–self and self–others are shown in orange; the mean and median of the distribution are depicted in black and red, respectively.

Fingerprint Specificity Depended on Number of Descriptors and Odors Used.

In our initial experiment we used as many as 54 descriptors and 28 odors because we wanted to explore the impact of these parameters. To estimate the dependence of fingerprint discriminability on the number of descriptors and odorants used, we again generated two alternative fingerprints for each subject, A and B, each using a random independent half of the descriptors used to derive similarity. Here, however, we successively reduced the number of odorants and descriptors used. We repeated each analysis 1,000 times, each time shuffling the particular odorants and descriptors omitted. We plotted the averaged fingerprint specificity (averaging the Z score across subjects and iterations) as a function of the number of descriptors and odorants used to generate it (Fig. 4 A and B). We observed a monotonic decrease in the specificity of the fingerprint, yet even with only 7 odors and 11 descriptors, the correlation between an individual’s two fingerprints based on the same odors but different descriptors was significantly above the correlation between two different individuals: z = 1.65, P < 0.05. Thus, potentially meaningful olfactory fingerprints can be obtained in under 10 min. In turn, we extrapolated to estimate how many odors and descriptors we would need to use to obtain an individual fingerprint for each of the ∼7 billion people on earth, and concluded at 34 odors and 35 descriptors. We estimate that obtaining such a detailed fingerprint would take ∼5 h.

Fig. 4.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Fig. 4.

Fingerprints depend on the number of odors and descriptors and the passing of time. (A) Heat map of fingerprint ability to distinguish self–self from self–other pairs (represented in Z-score values) as a function of number of odors and descriptors used. Dashed line represents Z-score value of 1.65 (P = 0.05). (B) 3D plot of Z-score values as a function of number of odors and descriptors used to generate a fingerprint. (C) First test–retest. Violin plot comparing correlation coefficients of 23 subjects refingerprinted across time. (Left) Correlation coefficients distribution of a subject to him/herself over time. (Right) Correlation coefficients distribution of a subject to other subjects over time. The mean and median of the distribution are depicted in black and red, respectively. (D) Second test–retest with five repetitions. Right y axis is correlation across retests (r) shown in yellow. Left y axis is the ability of the fingerprint to discriminate self from others in Z-score values when comparing the first to ensuing retests (black bars) or each two consecutive retests (red line).

Olfactory Fingerprints Remained Specific Despite Fluctuation Over Time.

Olfactory perception is not only variable across individuals, it is also highly variable within individuals over time (28). This variability may reflect in part that odor perception is the combination of a given receptor activation pattern with the fluctuating homeostatic state in which it is perceived (hunger/satiety, mood, arousal, etc.) (2). To test the persistence of the olfactory fingerprint at retest, we refingerprinted 23 participants at a time ranging between 10 and 30 d following their initial fingerprinting (e.g., Fig. 2A vs. Fig. 2C). We found that the average distance of a person from him/herself remained significantly lower than the average distance between different individuals [mean difference between an individual’s two fingerprints over time: r = 0.58 ± 0.15; mean difference between two different individuals’ fingerprints, r = 0.31 ± 0.076, paired t test, t(44) = 7.69, P < 10−8; Fig. 4C]. In other words, despite the passage of time, a person remained significantly more correlated with him/herself than with others.

Despite the above result, the slight reduction in self–self correlation over retests raises the concern that given additional retests, the self–self correlation advantage may disappear altogether. To address this concern, we refingerprinted an additional group of 18 subjects across five fingerprinting sessions that spanned 14–30 d (list of odorants and descriptors for experiment 1B in Tables S3 and S4). A repeated-measures ANOVA revealed that at each repetition (II, III, IV, and V) the average distance of a person from his/her first fingerprint remained unchanged [F(17, 3) = 2.24, P = 0.09, mean difference between an individual’s two fingerprints across retests r first–second = 0.58 ± 0.21, r first–third = 0.54 ± 0.18, r first–fourth = 0.54 ± 0.2, r first–fifth = 0.49 ± 0.19; Fig. 4D, yellow]. Moreover, the fingerprint stability in fact improved after the first retest [F(17, 3) = 6.08, P < 0.001] such that the second to third (r = 0.66 ± 0.19), third to fourth (r = 0.68 ± 0.20), and fourth to fifth (r = 0.69 ± 0.16) repetitions were all significantly better than the first to second [r = 0.58 ± 0.21, all t(17) > 2.67, all P < 0.02; Fig. 4D]. Taken together, we conclude that despite the passage of time and repeated testing, a person remained significantly more correlated with him/herself than with others. We recalculated the ability of the olfactory fingerprint to identify an individual beyond our sample, this time comparing the initial and the later (a few weeks later) fingerprints with all of the other subjects, and observed a decreased yet significant discriminability (z1–2 = 2.67, P < 0.01, z1–3 = 2.67, P < 0.01, z1–4 = 2.67, P < 0.01, z1–5 = 2.67, P < 0.01; Fig. 4D, black and red); this amounts to an ability to use the current olfactory fingerprint to identify 1 subject of ∼300 individuals. Moreover, given this variability over time, to effectively obtain long-lasting olfactory fingerprints for the entire world population we find by extrapolation that rather than 34 odors with 35 descriptors we would now need 160 odors with 35 descriptors. Note that this reduced discriminability is not only because of the extent of shift in fingerprint over time; self-correlation over time decreased from r = 0.75 to r = 0.58, which remains significantly higher than the correlation across individuals. However, because on average all subjects shifted in this way, the ability to identify one person out of a crowd is significantly reduced.

Similar Olfactory Fingerprints Imply High HLA Matching.

The hypothesis underlying our effort was that fingerprints would provide a unique perceptual counterpart of an individual’s unique olfactory receptor subtype genome. Potentially consistent with this notion, 28-odor-based fingerprints were special to the tune of 1 in 2 million. However, whereas olfactory receptor genomes are likely stable over 10–30 d (but see refs. 29⇓⇓–32), fingerprints were less so. Given this temporal fluctuation, we set out to test whether olfactory fingerprints can nevertheless remain informative of genetic traits linked to olfaction, in this case HLA; to test this, we studied an additional 130 subjects (65 women, mean age = 29.93 ± 8.44 y) who provided blood samples for HLA typing (Methods), and olfactory fingerprints using 11 odors (Table S5). Combinatorially, 130 subjects provide for 16,770 possible donor–recipient pairs, because HLA match is not symmetric, i.e., in a given pair, a subject can have a high HLA match as a donor but poor HLA match as a recipient (note that we use the terms “donor” and “recipient” to describe the directionality of HLA matching). Therefore, 130 subjects resulted in 16,770 possible pairs (130 × 129) and not in 8,385 (130 × 129/2). For each pair we calculated an olfactory fingerprint match using Euclidean distance (see SI Text for impact of alternative methods) and an HLA match along a 7-point scale (0–6, 0 = no match) previously described (16). Only 65 of 16,770 possible pairs of individuals had a high HLA match of 5 or 6 (Fig. 5A). We found that the olfactory fingerprint match of these individuals was significantly better than the olfactory fingerprint match for poorly HLA-matched individuals [HLA 5–6: mean olfactory fingerprint match in arbitrary units (AU) of Euclidean distance = 12.7 ± 4.1 AU, HLA 1–4: mean olfactory fingerprint match = 14.6 ± 5.3 AU, Wilcoxon rank-sum test: Z = 3.2, P < 0.0015]. In other words, the olfactory perceptual fingerprint similarity was significantly informative on HLA matching, implying that it captured meaningful genetic information.

Fig. 5.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Fig. 5.

Similar olfactory fingerprints imply high HLA matching. (A) A total of 16,770 pairwise comparisons of olfactory fingerprint distance vs. HLA match. The dotted red line reflects the cutoff for saved tests. (B) ROC curves of HLA comparisons saved vs. HLA matches missed. The diagonal identity line reflects no gain or loss. ROCs: red, using all 11 odors; gray, 200 testing curves using 4 odorants; black and blue, median and mean of the 4 best odorants, respectively.

To further assess the strength of the link between olfactory fingerprint match and HLA match, we asked what would happen if one used olfactory fingerprints to screen for potentially high HLA matches in the population. We calculated the percentage of high HLA matches one would potentially miss (incurred cost) vs. the percentage of matches one could identify (gain), and present this using a receiver operating characteristic (ROC) curve (Fig. 5B, red line). We found that all points in the ROC curve fall above the identity line; hence, olfactory fingerprints can potentially identify pairs of individuals likely to have a high HLA match. Given the extended time needed to test 11 odorants as we did in data collection, we next asked if we could optimize this test. We halved the data into training (8,387 subject pairs) and testing sets (8,385 subject pairs), each maintaining the original fractions of each level of HLA match. In the training set, we calculated olfactory fingerprints using all possible combinations of 3–11 odorants, and selected the four best-performing odorants, which we then tested in the testing set. We repeated this 200 times (Fig. 5B, grey lines). Taking the median score (Fig. 5B, black line), we found that a selection of four odors (isoamyl acetate, isovaleric acid, 2-ethyl pyrazine, and 1-hexanol) decreased the average olfactory fingerprint distances of high-HLA matched individuals to 10.8 ± 3.7 AU compared with an olfactory fingerprint distance for poorly HLA matched individuals of 13.8 ± 5.8 AU (Z = 4.35, P < 0.000015). Finally, we calculated the actual savings implicated by this: the 65 high-HLA matches in our data comprised of 45 individuals (some individuals were matched with more than one). We iteratively selected one individual from these 45 as recipient, and randomly drew donors until we encountered a high HLA match. We repeated this 10,000 times. Consistent with the expectation from chance, we had to test an average of 65.35 ± 37.5 donors to identify a match. We then repeated these exact procedures again, but rather than randomly drawing donors, we drew them in rank order in accordance with their rapidly obtainable optimized olfactory fingerprint distance, starting with the closest. We found that the average number of individuals we had to test to identify a match was 44 ± 29, implying a 32% savings [t(64) = 5.5, P < 10−6]. In other words, using this brief perceptual test, one could rank order the population to save more than 30% of HLA tests.

Discussion

A recurring question in both popular and scientific settings is whether people are similar or different in their olfactory perception. This question, however, must be answered with caution, even with the current data in hand. If we look at gross olfactory perception, people remain quite similar to one another. As evidenced in Fig. 2E, if we take the average perception of an odor, it serves as a reasonable estimation of what any given individual will say about that odor. However, once we applied the method we developed for maximizing differences, we find that olfactory perception was highly individually specific and variable across individuals.

Notably, variability across individuals was somewhat smoothed by variability within individuals over time, which we think reflects an interaction between a given olfactory receptor subtype genome that determines the borders of individual perception, and fluctuating homeostatic state that determines position within these borders. Nevertheless, these borders remained confined enough such that despite fluctuation, individual olfactory perception meaningfully informed on nonolfactory genetic makeup, in this instance, HLA matching. As noted in the introduction, information on HLA is available through body odor (16, 17). It has been suggested that several vertebrate species use this information to select mates with dissimilar HLA (33⇓⇓–36), and for this to occur, the olfactory system should have access to the information of self HLA makeup. In here finding that HLA is also linked to olfactory perception alone, our results combine with a previous effort (19) to provide a path for such self-recognition, and in this close a loop likely subserving behavioral selection mechanisms that rely on the sense of smell (20).

The current effort has some clear limitations that we would like to acknowledge: the tool we developed provides a precise measure of olfactory perception. Although we speculate that individual olfactory perception reflects individual olfactory receptor subtype repertoire, we did not directly measure genetic makeup. Therefore, an obvious albeit expensive follow-up study should collect olfactory fingerprints and full olfactory receptor genomes from the same individuals. Similarly, we speculated that the olfactory fingerprint to HLA link is related to the genetic link between HLA and olfactory receptor loci. However, the fingerprint to HLA link may in fact follow some other indirect path. Specifically, the HLA mechanism both enables and depends on identification of self. In turn, the olfactory fingerprint also captures self. Given that HLA captures self and olfactory fingerprints capture self, then there will be a link between HLA and olfactory fingerprints even if they are not the result of linked genes. There is in fact a future way to test these alternatives—namely, whether the fingerprint-to-HLA link reflects linkage through the genome or not: HLA is on chromosome 6. If the fingerprint-to-HLA link reflects linkage through the genome, then olfactory fingerprints based on odors that activate olfactory receptors on chromosome 6 should be better predictors of HLA than olfactory fingerprints based on odors that activate receptors on other chromosomes. The current data, however, do not have the power to conduct such an analysis, because 6 of the 11 odorants we used do not have a known receptor, and only two of the odorants have known receptors on chromosome 6 (androstadienone and cis-3-hexen-1-ol) (4, 37). Notably, these two odorants did not end up in the optimized set of four odorants.

Despite these limitations, our results indicate a powerful tool whose potential usefulness goes beyond characterization of olfaction alone. In some instances, usefulness may follow linkage to the olfactory genome. In turn, various olfactory receptors, in addition to responding to odors in the nose, likely also play assorted roles in various nonolfactory organs and functions (38⇓–40). Olfactory fingerprints may provide a measure for these receptors, and therefore serve as a predictive tool for their respective nonolfactory functions as well. Finally, olfaction is highly predictive of health status (41, 42), strongly linked to neurodegenerative diseases (43), autoimmune diseases (44), and more. With this in mind, we conclude in predicting that olfactory fingerprints can become a widely applicable tool, informative on a host of conditions in both health and disease.

Methods

Subjects.

A total of 238 generally healthy subjects participated in three experiments (experiment 1A: 89 subjects, 40 women, mean age = 25.7 ± 3.1 y; experiment 1B: 18 subjects, 11 women, mean age 26.8 ± 3.4; experiment 2: 130 subjects, 65 women, mean age = 29.93 ± 8.44 y) after providing written informed consent to procedures approved by the Sheba Medical Center Helsinki Committee.

Odorants.

We used two forms of odorant presentation in this study. Experiment 1A contained 24 odorants in scratch-and-sniff form provided by The PrintBox, Inc., and four odorants presented in sniff jars. Experiment 1B contained 22 odorants presented in sniff jars. Experiment 2 contained 11 odorants all in jars; the four jar odorants from experiment 1 (isoamyl acetate, 1,8-cineole, cis‐3‐hexen‐1‐ol, and isovaleric acid) and seven additional odorants. All odorants and the experiments they were used in are detailed in Tables S1–S6. Because the initial test–retest experiment used mostly scratch-and-sniff odorants and the second test–retest experiment used jars, we could directly assess any difference between these methods of presentation. We found no significant difference in test–retest when comparing scratch and sniff (r = 0.59 ± 0.14) to jars [r first–second = 0.58 ± 0.21, t(39) = 0.24, P = 0.81].

Ratings.

Each subject rated 28 odorants along 54 verbal descriptors in experiment 1A, 22 odorants along 23 verbal descriptors in experiment 1B, and 11 odorants along 57 verbal descriptors in experiment 2 (see list of odorants in Tables S1, S3, and S5 and list of descriptors in Tables S2, S4, and S6) using visual analog scales. For example, the question “to what extent does this odor smell like coconut?” above a 14-cm line ranging from “not at all” at one end to “very much so” at the other. After sniffing the odorant presented in scratch and sniff or jar, participants crossed the line at a point reflecting their perception, and the line was later parsed to 100 for analysis. Odor order was random across participants, and interodor interval was >40 s. To account for individual differences in use of scales, each subject’s data were normalized by first subtracting the minimal value applied by the subject, then dividing by the maximal remaining value, and finally multiplying by 100; this generated a normalized range between 0 and 100.

HLA Typing.

Blood samples were collected at the Sheba Medical Center Autoimmune and Chemical Laboratories. HLA analyses were conducted at the Sheba Medical Center Tissue Typing Unit. In brief, 5–10 mL of blood was drawn from each volunteer and kept at 4 °C until DNA was extracted. Genomic DNA Extraction was carried out from 400 μL of whole blood using the MagNA Pure Compact Nucleic Acid Isolation Kit I (Roche Diagnostics). DNA samples were stored at −20 °C. HLA typing was performed using Luminex technology and Immucor Transplant Diagnostic kits to obtain HLA A*, B*, and DRB1* loci typings at low/intermediate resolution (a list of genotypes at the three HLA loci of each of the 130 subjects is in Tables S7).

HLA Matching.

We calculated HLA match using methods previously reported in ref. 16. There are three general groups of HLA: HLA-A*, HLA-B*, and HLA-DRB1*, and within each group there are different specific HLA proteins (there are 59 different HLA-A* proteins, 118 different HLA-B*, and 124 different HLA-DRB1*). Each of these HLA groups (A*, B*, and DRB1*) is notated by a two-digit numerical designation (e.g., HLA-A* 01:07, HLA-B* 15:15, HLA-DRB1* 15:33). A match is calculated by counting the number of HLA proteins (in each group separately) present in one subject that are also present in another subject. Because there are two digits for each HLA group, the count can be 0, 1, or 2. Once the count for each HLA group is obtained, a match is calculated by summing the values of all of the groups. In other words, for each donor/recipient pair we counted the number of antigens present in the donor that matched an antigen in the recipient. Homozygous antigens in the recipient that matched a donor antigen were counted as two matches. Because we measured three HLA loci, there was a potential for a maximum of seven matches. For example, if donor A has the following HLA genotype A*24,68 B*14,35 DRB1*01,11 and recipient B has the following HLA genotype A*03,23 B*41,47 DRB1*10,11, this pair (A→B) will have an HLA match score of 0 + 0 + 1 = 1. However, if donor C has the following HLA genotype A*02, 30 B* 13, 50 DRB1* 07, 07 and recipient D has the following HLA genotype A*02, 02 B*27, 41 DRB1*07, 11 then the pair (C→D) will have an HLA match score of 2 + 0 + 1 = 3 and the pair (D→C) will have an HLA match score of 1 + 0 + 2 = 3 (note that even though the total HLA match score is the same, it is not symmetric between C↔D).

Acknowledgments

This work was supported by European Research Council Ideas Advanced Grant 670798.

Footnotes

  • ↵1L.S. and K.S. contributed equally to this work.

  • ↵2To whom correspondence may be addressed. Email: noam.sobel{at}weizmann.ac.il or lavisec{at}gmail.com.
  • Author contributions: L.S., K.S., K.W., and N.S. designed research; L.S., K.S., K.W., L.P., Y.S., R.L., N.A.-L., I.F., D.B.-Z., S.S., and N.S. performed research; L.S., K.S., K.W., L.P., and N.S. analyzed data; and L.S., K.S., R.L., and N.S. wrote the paper.

  • Conflict of interest statement: The Weizmann Institute is filing for a patent on the method of olfactory fingerprinting.

  • This article is a PNAS Direct Submission.

  • This article contains supporting information online at www.pnas.org/lookup/suppl/doi:10.1073/pnas.1424826112/-/DCSupplemental.

Freely available online through the PNAS open access option.

References

  1. ↵
    1. Bushdid C,
    2. Magnasco MO,
    3. Vosshall LB,
    4. Keller A
    (2014) Humans can discriminate more than 1 trillion olfactory stimuli. Science 343(6177):1370–1372
    .
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  2. ↵
    1. Yeshurun Y,
    2. Sobel N
    (2010) An odor is not worth a thousand words: From multidimensional odors to unidimensional odor objects. Annu Rev Psychol 61:219–241, C1–C5
    .
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  3. ↵
    1. Gilad Y,
    2. Lancet D
    (2003) Population differences in the human functional olfactory repertoire. Mol Biol Evol 20(3):307–314
    .
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  4. ↵
    1. Keller A,
    2. Zhuang H,
    3. Chi Q,
    4. Vosshall LB,
    5. Matsunami H
    (2007) Genetic variation in a human odorant receptor alters odour perception. Nature 449(7161):468–472
    .
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  5. ↵
    1. Araneda RC,
    2. Kini AD,
    3. Firestein S
    (2000) The molecular receptive range of an odorant receptor. Nat Neurosci 3(12):1248–1255
    .
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  6. ↵
    1. Axel R
    (1995) The molecular logic of smell. Sci Am 273(4):154–159
    .
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  7. ↵
    1. Buck LB
    (1996) Information coding in the vertebrate olfactory system. Annu Rev Neurosci 19:517–544
    .
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  8. ↵
    1. Mainland JD, et al.
    (2014) The missense of smell: Functional variability in the human odorant receptor repertoire. Nat Neurosci 17(1):114–120
    .
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  9. ↵
    1. Menashe I,
    2. Man O,
    3. Lancet D,
    4. Gilad Y
    (2003) Different noses for different people. Nat Genet 34(2):143–144
    .
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  10. ↵
    1. Wandell BA
    (1995) Foundations of Vision (Sinauer, Sunderland, MA)
    .
  11. ↵
    1. Wysocki CJ,
    2. Beauchamp GK
    (1984) Ability to smell androstenone is genetically determined. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 81(15):4899–4902
    .
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  12. ↵
    1. Menashe I, et al.
    (2007) Genetic elucidation of human hyperosmia to isovaleric acid. PLoS Biol 5(11):e284
    .
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  13. ↵
    1. Amadou C, et al.
    (2003) Co-duplication of olfactory receptor and MHC class I genes in the mouse major histocompatibility complex. Hum Mol Genet 12(22):3025–3040
    .
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  14. ↵
    1. Ziegler A,
    2. Dohr G,
    3. Uchanska-Ziegler B
    (2002) Possible roles for products of polymorphic MHC and linked olfactory receptor genes during selection processes in reproduction. Am J Reprod Immunol 48(1):34–42
    .
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  15. ↵
    1. Younger RM, et al.
    (2001) Characterization of clustered MHC-linked olfactory receptor genes in human and mouse. Genome Res 11(4):519–530
    .
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  16. ↵
    1. Jacob S,
    2. McClintock MK,
    3. Zelano B,
    4. Ober C
    (2002) Paternally inherited HLA alleles are associated with women’s choice of male odor. Nat Genet 30(2):175–179
    .
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  17. ↵
    1. Wedekind C,
    2. Seebeck T,
    3. Bettens F,
    4. Paepke AJ
    (1995) MHC-dependent mate preferences in humans. Proc Biol Sci 260(1359):245–249
    .
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  18. ↵
    1. Wedekind C,
    2. Penn D
    (2000) MHC genes, body odours, and odour preferences. Nephrol Dial Transplant 15(9):1269–1271
    .
    OpenUrlFREE Full Text
  19. ↵
    1. Milinski M,
    2. Wedekind C
    (2001) Evidence for MHC-correlated perfume preferences in humans. Behav Ecol 12(2):140–149
    .
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  20. ↵
    1. Christakis NA,
    2. Fowler JH
    (2014) Friendship and natural selection. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 111(Suppl 3):10796–10801
    .
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  21. ↵
    1. Wise PM,
    2. Olsson MJ,
    3. Cain WS
    (2000) Quantification of odor quality. Chem Senses 25(4):429–443
    .
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  22. ↵
    1. Khan RM, et al.
    (2007) Predicting odor pleasantness from odorant structure: Pleasantness as a reflection of the physical world. J Neurosci 27(37):10015–10023
    .
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  23. ↵
    1. Keller A,
    2. Hempstead M,
    3. Gomez IA,
    4. Gilbert AN,
    5. Vosshall LB
    (2012) An olfactory demography of a diverse metropolitan population. BMC Neurosci 13:122
    .
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  24. ↵
    1. Majid A,
    2. Burenhult N
    (2014) Odors are expressible in language, as long as you speak the right language. Cognition 130(2):266–270
    .
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  25. ↵
    1. Dravnieks A
    (1982) Odor quality: Semantically generated multidimensional profiles are stable. Science 218(4574):799–801
    .
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  26. ↵
    1. Schiffman SS
    (1974) Physicochemical correlates of olfactory quality. Science 185(4146):112–117
    .
    OpenUrlFREE Full Text
  27. ↵
    1. Anderson MJ
    (2001) A new method for non‐parametric multivariate analysis of variance. Austral Ecol 26(1):32–46
    .
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  28. ↵
    1. Stevens JC,
    2. Cain WS,
    3. Burke RJ
    (1988) Variability of olfactory thresholds. Chem Senses 13(4):643–653
    .
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  29. ↵
    1. Santoro SW,
    2. Dulac C
    (2012) The activity-dependent histone variant H2BE modulates the life span of olfactory neurons. eLife 1:e00070
    .
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  30. ↵
    1. Cadiou H, et al.
    (2014) Postnatal odorant exposure induces peripheral olfactory plasticity at the cellular level. J Neurosci 34(14):4857–4870
    .
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  31. ↵
    1. Wang HW,
    2. Wysocki CJ,
    3. Gold GH
    (1993) Induction of olfactory receptor sensitivity in mice. Science 260(5110):998–1000
    .
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  32. ↵
    1. Yee KK,
    2. Wysocki CJ
    (2001) Odorant exposure increases olfactory sensitivity: Olfactory epithelium is implicated. Physiol Behav 72(5):705–711
    .
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  33. ↵
    1. Brown RE,
    2. Singh PB,
    3. Roser B
    (1987) The major histocompatibility complex and the chemosensory recognition of individuality in rats. Physiol Behav 40(1):65–73
    .
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  34. ↵
    1. Freeman-Gallant CR,
    2. Meguerdichian M,
    3. Wheelwright NT,
    4. Sollecito SV
    (2003) Social pairing and female mating fidelity predicted by restriction fragment length polymorphism similarity at the major histocompatibility complex in a songbird. Mol Ecol 12(11):3077–3083
    .
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  35. ↵
    1. Reusch TB,
    2. Häberli MA,
    3. Aeschlimann PB,
    4. Milinski M
    (2001) Female sticklebacks count alleles in a strategy of sexual selection explaining MHC polymorphism. Nature 414(6861):300–302
    .
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  36. ↵
    1. Yamazaki K, et al.
    (1976) Control of mating preferences in mice by genes in the major histocompatibility complex. J Exp Med 144(5):1324–1335
    .
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  37. ↵
    1. McRae JF, et al.
    (2012) Genetic variation in the odorant receptor OR2J3 is associated with the ability to detect the “grassy” smelling odor, cis-3-hexen-1-ol. Chem Senses 37(7):585–593
    .
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  38. ↵
    1. Kang N,
    2. Koo J
    (2012) Olfactory receptors in non-chemosensory tissues. BMB Rep 45(11):612–622
    .
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  39. ↵
    1. Feldmesser E, et al.
    (2006) Widespread ectopic expression of olfactory receptor genes. BMC Genomics 7:121
    .
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  40. ↵
    1. Busse D, et al.
    (2014) A synthetic sandalwood odorant induces wound-healing processes in human keratinocytes via the olfactory receptor OR2AT4. J Invest Dermatol 134(11):2823–2832
    .
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  41. ↵
    1. Pinto JM,
    2. Wroblewski KE,
    3. Kern DW,
    4. Schumm LP,
    5. McClintock MK
    (2014) Olfactory dysfunction predicts 5-year mortality in older adults. PLoS ONE 9(10):e107541
    .
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  42. ↵
    1. Frasnelli J,
    2. Hummel T
    (2005) Olfactory dysfunction and daily life. Eur Arch Otorhinolaryngol 262(3):231–235
    .
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  43. ↵
    1. Doty RL, et al.
    (1991) Olfactory dysfunction in three neurodegenerative diseases. Geriatrics 46(Suppl 1):47–51
    .
    OpenUrlPubMed
  44. ↵
    1. Perricone C, et al.
    (2013) Smell and autoimmunity: A comprehensive review. Clin Rev Allergy Immunol 45(1):87–96
    .
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
PreviousNext
Back to top
Article Alerts
Email Article

Thank you for your interest in spreading the word on PNAS.

NOTE: We only request your email address so that the person you are recommending the page to knows that you wanted them to see it, and that it is not junk mail. We do not capture any email address.

Enter multiple addresses on separate lines or separate them with commas.
Individual olfactory perception reveals meaningful nonolfactory genetic information
(Your Name) has sent you a message from PNAS
(Your Name) thought you would like to see the PNAS web site.
CAPTCHA
This question is for testing whether or not you are a human visitor and to prevent automated spam submissions.
Citation Tools
Individual olfactory perception
Lavi Secundo, Kobi Snitz, Kineret Weissler, Liron Pinchover, Yehuda Shoenfeld, Ron Loewenthal, Nancy Agmon-Levin, Idan Frumin, Dana Bar-Zvi, Sagit Shushan, Noam Sobel
Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences Jul 2015, 112 (28) 8750-8755; DOI: 10.1073/pnas.1424826112

Citation Manager Formats

  • BibTeX
  • Bookends
  • EasyBib
  • EndNote (tagged)
  • EndNote 8 (xml)
  • Medlars
  • Mendeley
  • Papers
  • RefWorks Tagged
  • Ref Manager
  • RIS
  • Zotero
Request Permissions
Share
Individual olfactory perception
Lavi Secundo, Kobi Snitz, Kineret Weissler, Liron Pinchover, Yehuda Shoenfeld, Ron Loewenthal, Nancy Agmon-Levin, Idan Frumin, Dana Bar-Zvi, Sagit Shushan, Noam Sobel
Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences Jul 2015, 112 (28) 8750-8755; DOI: 10.1073/pnas.1424826112
del.icio.us logo Digg logo Reddit logo Twitter logo CiteULike logo Facebook logo Google logo Mendeley logo
  • Tweet Widget
  • Facebook Like
  • Mendeley logo Mendeley

Article Classifications

  • Biological Sciences
  • Neuroscience
  • Social Sciences
  • Psychological and Cognitive Sciences
Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences: 112 (28)
Table of Contents

Submit

Sign up for Article Alerts

Jump to section

  • Article
    • Abstract
    • Results
    • Discussion
    • Methods
    • Acknowledgments
    • Footnotes
    • References
  • Figures & SI
  • Info & Metrics
  • PDF

You May Also be Interested in

Water from a faucet fills a glass.
News Feature: How “forever chemicals” might impair the immune system
Researchers are exploring whether these ubiquitous fluorinated molecules might worsen infections or hamper vaccine effectiveness.
Image credit: Shutterstock/Dmitry Naumov.
Reflection of clouds in the still waters of Mono Lake in California.
Inner Workings: Making headway with the mysteries of life’s origins
Recent experiments and simulations are starting to answer some fundamental questions about how life came to be.
Image credit: Shutterstock/Radoslaw Lecyk.
Cave in coastal Kenya with tree growing in the middle.
Journal Club: Small, sharp blades mark shift from Middle to Later Stone Age in coastal Kenya
Archaeologists have long tried to define the transition between the two time periods.
Image credit: Ceri Shipton.
Illustration of groups of people chatting
Exploring the length of human conversations
Adam Mastroianni and Daniel Gilbert explore why conversations almost never end when people want them to.
Listen
Past PodcastsSubscribe
Panda bear hanging in a tree
How horse manure helps giant pandas tolerate cold
A study finds that giant pandas roll in horse manure to increase their cold tolerance.
Image credit: Fuwen Wei.

Similar Articles

Site Logo
Powered by HighWire
  • Submit Manuscript
  • Twitter
  • Facebook
  • RSS Feeds
  • Email Alerts

Articles

  • Current Issue
  • Special Feature Articles – Most Recent
  • List of Issues

PNAS Portals

  • Anthropology
  • Chemistry
  • Classics
  • Front Matter
  • Physics
  • Sustainability Science
  • Teaching Resources

Information

  • Authors
  • Editorial Board
  • Reviewers
  • Subscribers
  • Librarians
  • Press
  • Cozzarelli Prize
  • Site Map
  • PNAS Updates
  • FAQs
  • Accessibility Statement
  • Rights & Permissions
  • About
  • Contact

Feedback    Privacy/Legal

Copyright © 2021 National Academy of Sciences. Online ISSN 1091-6490