Skip to main content

Main menu

  • Home
  • Articles
    • Current
    • Special Feature Articles - Most Recent
    • Special Features
    • Colloquia
    • Collected Articles
    • PNAS Classics
    • List of Issues
  • Front Matter
    • Front Matter Portal
    • Journal Club
  • News
    • For the Press
    • This Week In PNAS
    • PNAS in the News
  • Podcasts
  • Authors
    • Information for Authors
    • Editorial and Journal Policies
    • Submission Procedures
    • Fees and Licenses
  • Submit
  • Submit
  • About
    • Editorial Board
    • PNAS Staff
    • FAQ
    • Accessibility Statement
    • Rights and Permissions
    • Site Map
  • Contact
  • Journal Club
  • Subscribe
    • Subscription Rates
    • Subscriptions FAQ
    • Open Access
    • Recommend PNAS to Your Librarian

User menu

  • Log in
  • My Cart

Search

  • Advanced search
Home
Home
  • Log in
  • My Cart

Advanced Search

  • Home
  • Articles
    • Current
    • Special Feature Articles - Most Recent
    • Special Features
    • Colloquia
    • Collected Articles
    • PNAS Classics
    • List of Issues
  • Front Matter
    • Front Matter Portal
    • Journal Club
  • News
    • For the Press
    • This Week In PNAS
    • PNAS in the News
  • Podcasts
  • Authors
    • Information for Authors
    • Editorial and Journal Policies
    • Submission Procedures
    • Fees and Licenses
  • Submit
Research Article

Sibling conflict and dishonest signaling in birds

View ORCID ProfileShana M. Caro, Stuart A. West, and View ORCID ProfileAshleigh S. Griffin
  1. aDepartment of Zoology, University of Oxford, Oxford OX1 3PS, United Kingdom

See allHide authors and affiliations

PNAS November 29, 2016 113 (48) 13803-13808; first published November 7, 2016; https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1606378113
Shana M. Caro
aDepartment of Zoology, University of Oxford, Oxford OX1 3PS, United Kingdom
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
  • ORCID record for Shana M. Caro
Stuart A. West
aDepartment of Zoology, University of Oxford, Oxford OX1 3PS, United Kingdom
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Ashleigh S. Griffin
aDepartment of Zoology, University of Oxford, Oxford OX1 3PS, United Kingdom
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
  • ORCID record for Ashleigh S. Griffin
  • For correspondence: ashleigh.griffin@zoo.ox.ac.uk
  1. Edited by Joan E. Strassmann, Washington University in St. Louis, St. Louis, MO, and approved September 23, 2016 (received for review April 22, 2016)

  • Article
  • Figures & SI
  • Info & Metrics
  • PDF
Loading

Article Figures & SI

Figures

  • Tables
  • Fig. S1.
    • Download figure
    • Open in new tab
    • Download powerpoint
    Fig. S1.

    What is honesty? Species were considered honest if there was a strong relationship between offspring need and begging intensity, as measured by the correlation coefficient (37). (A) When the correlation coefficient approaches 1, begging is considered honest because it is almost perfectly informative. (B) When the correlation is weaker (e.g., r = 0.5), begging may provide some information about need, but it is less reliable. (C) When the correlation coefficient approaches 0, begging is considered dishonest because it provides no information about need. (D and E) Begging could also function as an honest signal of quality, rather than need, in which case the correlation coefficient would be negative rather than positive. Our analyses looked at whether differences in the relative strength of the correlation between need and begging across species can be explained by increases in within-family conflict as predicted by Hamilton’s rule.

  • Fig. 1.
    • Download figure
    • Open in new tab
    • Download powerpoint
    Fig. 1.

    The presence of siblings is associated with a reduction in offspring honesty. Data points represent species’ mean correlation coefficients of long-term need and begging intensity for species where parents rear only one (n = 6 species) or more than one offspring per brood (n = 54 species). Positive correlations indicate that offspring in worse condition beg more intensely, providing honest information about need. Gray lines represent the 95% CIs from the model, run on the full dataset, controlling for phylogeny and repeated measures. Species with siblings present have a weaker correlation between need and begging, suggesting less honest signaling of need (Wald = 6.69, P = 0.0097).

  • Fig. 2.
    • Download figure
    • Open in new tab
    • Download powerpoint
    Fig. 2.

    Conflict with future siblings is associated with a reduction in offspring honesty. Data points show species’ mean correlation coefficient of long-term need and begging intensity. The number of potential future broods is the adult life expectancy multiplied by the number of successful broods that can be reared each breeding season (n = 51 species). The gray line is the regression coefficient from the model, run on the full dataset, controlling for phylogeny and repeated measures. Positive correlations indicate that offspring in worse condition beg more intensely, providing honest information about need. The dashed line at zero indicates no relationship between condition and begging. The correlation between need and begging is weaker in species where parents can potentially produce more broods over their lifetimes (Wald = 7.22, P = 0.0087), suggesting that future reproduction selects for less honest signaling of need.

  • Fig. 3.
    • Download figure
    • Open in new tab
    • Download powerpoint
    Fig. 3.

    Parental divorce and death is associated with a reduction in offspring honesty. Data points represent species’ mean correlation coefficients of long-term need and begging intensity for species. We divided species by whether there is a higher or lower than 50% chance that parents will breed together again in the next year, based on survival and divorce rates (n = 19 species where full siblings are expected; n = 30 species where half siblings are expected). Positive correlations indicate that offspring in worse condition beg more intensely, providing honest information about need. Gray lines represent the 95% CIs from our analyses. The correlation between need and begging is weaker in species with higher rates of divorce and lower rates of survival, where half siblings are expected (Wald = 5.98, P = 0.016), suggesting less honest signaling of need.

  • Fig. S2.
    • Download figure
    • Open in new tab
    • Download powerpoint
    Fig. S2.

    Relatedness to the whole brood decreases as brood size increases. The average relatedness of a focal offspring to the whole brood decreases with each additional sibling, until reaching an asymptote r = 0.5 for full siblings (●) and r = 0.25 for half siblings (○). Whole brood relatedness is calculated by averaging a focal offspring’s relatedness to itself and each of its siblings. For example, r = 0.75 for two full siblings (1 + 0.5)/2, and r = 0.5 for three half siblings: (1 + 0.25 + 0.25)/3. Even without any extrapair paternity, the relatedness of a focal chick to the whole brood decreases rapidly as brood size increases: average relatedness to the whole brood is 75% with one full sibling, drops to 67% with two full siblings, and to 55% with ten full siblings.

  • Fig. 4.
    • Download figure
    • Open in new tab
    • Download powerpoint
    Fig. 4.

    Family conflict is associated with a reduction in the honesty of offspring signals of need. Offspring in species with siblings present are less honest when signaling need (Wald statistic = 6.69, P = 0.0097). The probability that parents will breed again is also associated with more dishonest offspring, as the current brood competes against future broods for parental investment (Wald = 7.22, P = 0.0087). Finally, offspring are less honest in species where parents are likely to divorce or die before breeding again, and all future siblings will be half siblings (Wald = 5.98, P = 0.016).

  • Fig. S3.
    • Download figure
    • Open in new tab
    • Download powerpoint
    Fig. S3.

    PRISMA flowchart of search results and the study selection process. See Dataset S2 for a list of papers excluded from the analysis. The final dataset for begging analyses contained 336 effect sizes from 108 studies on 60 species of birds. The final dataset for structural signal analyses contained 140 effect sizes from 33 studies on 18 species of birds. Overall, we found data in 136 studies on 72 species.

Tables

  • Figures
    • View popup
    Table 1.

    Results for all models: fixed effects

    Model no.Fixed effectsResultsN speciesN studyN observations
    1Siblings present y/nF1,71.8 = 6.69, P = 0.009760108336
    2Brood sizeF1,31.0 = 4.57, P = 0.033
    3Siblings present y/nF1,67.0 = 8.45, P = 0.00505198317
    No. of future broods possibleF1,87.9 = 3.13, P = 0.080
    4Brood sizeF1,82.2 = 11.87, P = 0.0009
    No. of future broods possibleF1,83.5 = 7.22, P = 0.0087
    5Siblings present y/nF1,84.2 = 0.42, P = 0.424996314
    No. of future broods possibleF1,95.4 = 9.39, P = 0.0028
    Full vs. half siblings likelihoodF1,112.5 = 6.94, P = 0.0096
    6Brood sizeF1,87.1 = 3.07, P = 0.083
    No. of future broods possibleF1,86.4 = 13.09, P = 0.0005
    Full vs. half siblings likelihoodF1,89.6 = 5.98, P = 0.016
    7Siblings present y/nF1,60.5 = 0, P = 13168230
    No. of future broods possibleF1,58.4 = 3.10, P = 0.083
    Full vs. half siblings likelihoodF1,76.0 = 3.77, P = 0.056
    Extrapair paternityF1,58.5 = 0.04, P = 0.84
    8Brood sizeF1,66.2 = 3.87, P = 0.053
    No. of future broods possibleF1,58.3 = 4.94, P = 0.038
    Full vs. half siblings likelihoodF1,67.1 = 1.00, P = 0.32
    Extrapair paternityF1,58.5 = 0.27, P = 0.61
    • Mean results (conditional Wald statistics) of 500 ASReml linear mixed models. Models controlled for phylogeny, repeated measures on studies, and species, and were weighted by study sample size (the number of broods used to calculate the original test statistic). Fixed effects in bold are significant at the P < 0.05 level and in italics at the P < 0.10 level. Models are grouped by the dataset used for analysis, as sample size decreased in later analyses due to unavailable life history data.

    • View popup
    Table S1.

    Results for all models: Random effects

    No.Log likelihoodTotal I2Random effectsComponentSEN speciesN studyN observationsNotes
    Null (a)123.512.8%Phylogeny0.00860.018260108336Full dataset
    Species0.00050.0254
    Study0.10590.0265
    Residual0.03200.0077
    1124.312.0%Phylogeny0.0000NA
    Species0.0000NA
    Study0.10390.0224
    Residual0.03210.0077
    2121.712.0%Phylogeny0.0000NA
    Species0.00560.0187
    Study0.09900.0277
    Residual0.03240.0078
    Null (b)117.512.7%Phylogeny0.00600.02145198317Excluding 9 species missing data on adult mortality or the broods possible each breeding season
    Species0.00480.0266
    Study0.10140.0294
    Residual0.03300.0080
    3115.011.6%Phylogeny0.0000NA
    Species0.0000NA
    Study0.09860.0227
    Residual0.03300.0080
    4114.811.0%Phylogeny0.0000NA
    Species0.0000NA
    Study0.08960.0213
    Residual0.03350.0080
    Null (c)119.012.4%Phylogeny0.01040.01894996314Excluding 11 species missing data on adult mortality, the broods possible each breeding season, or mating system
    Species0.00010.0245
    Study0.09840.0255
    Residual0.03280.0079
    5118.010.5%Phylogeny0.0000NA
    Species0.0000NA
    Study0.08500.0205
    Residual0.03290.0079
    6117.610.1%Phylogeny0.0000NA
    Species0.0000NA
    Study0.07870.0196
    Residual0.03330.0080
    Null (d)104.512.9%Phylogeny0.02410.02743168230Excluding 29 species missing data on adult mortality, the broods possible each breeding season, mating system, or extra pair paternity
    Species0.0000NA
    Study0.09550.0283
    Residual0.02880.0079
    795.811.3%Phylogeny0.0000NA
    Species0.0000NA
    Study0.09850.0262
    Residual0.02850.0079
    895.910.6%Phylogeny0.0000NA
    Species0.0000NA
    Study0.09000.0244
    Residual0.02890.0079
    • Mean results of 500 ASReml linear mixed models. Models were weighted by study sample size (the number of broods used to calculate the original test statistic). Models are grouped by the dataset used for analysis, and the notes column describes the loss in sample size due to missing life history data. Sample error variance was constrained to 1. Total heterogeneity (I2) is a measure of the proportion of observed variance due to true differences in effect sizes in the null model (34). I2 values of 25%, 50%, and 75% indicate low, moderate, and high levels ratios of signal to noise (34). This measure does not take effect size dispersion into account (34). Model numbers correspond to Table 1. NA indicates that models could not estimate the standard error of this variance component.

    • View popup
    Table S2.

    Relationships between environmental and life history variables

    Life history variablesEnvironmental variablesN species
    PredictabilityQuality
    Siblings y/n−33.07 to −2.56−4.24 to 23.2860
    Brood size−0.76 to 0.47−0.28 to 0.59
    Siblings y/n−19.37 to −9.42−2.18 to 9.0351
    Brood size−0.72 to 0.56−0.31 to 0.52
    No. future broods possible−0.02 to 0.02−0.01 to 0.01
    Siblings y/n−33.19 to −13.06−3.69 to 8.1249
    Brood size−0.89 to 0.41−0.30 to 0.53
    No. future broods possible−0.02 to 0.02−0.01 to 0.01
    Full vs. half siblings likelihood−5.50 to 2.81−3.54 to 2.22
    Siblings y/n−61.65 to −5.45−6.66 to 25.0833
    Brood size−0.91 to 0.74−0.64 to 0.43
    No. future broods possible−0.03 to 0.03−0.01 to 0.01
    Full vs. half siblings likelihood−9.01 to 37.59−80.76 to 7.21
    Promiscuity−0.03 to 0.03−0.01 to 0.01
    • Table reports the 95% CI for the relationship of environmental variables on each of the life history variables used with each subset of the overall dataset (n = 60 species corresponds to models 1 and 2; n = 51 species corresponds to models 3 and 4; n = 49 species corresponds to models 5 and 6; n = 33 species corresponds to models 8 and 7). Results are from MCMCglmm models, controlling for phylogeny. MCMCglmm was used instead of ASReml because ASReml models did not converge properly when accounting for phylogenetic variance in some models with smaller sample sizes. Cells in bold indicate the relationship is significant (the 95% CI does not include 0).

    • View popup
    Table S3.

    Effect of adding environmental variables to models

    Model no.Fixed effectsVariables included in modelN
    Only life historyOnly environmentalAll variables
    Model 1InterceptCoef = 0.10 ± 0.04Coef = −0.09 ± 0.09Coef = −0.09 ± 0.0960 species, 108 studies, 336 effect sizes
    F1,89.9 = 13.57, P = 0.0005F1,91.0 = 13.23, P = 0.0002F1,88.9 = 13.57, P = 0.0005
    Environmental predictabilityCoef = 0.20 ± 0.08Coef = 0.14 ± 0.09
    F1,91.7 = 6.73, P = 0.011*F1,92.7 = 2.66, P = 0.11
    Environmental qualityCoef = 0.32 ± 0.09Coef = 0.32 ± 0.09
    F2,191.8 = 7.13, P = 0.0010**F2,192.2 = 7.14, P = 0.0010**
    Siblings present y/nCoef = −0.28 ± 0.11Coef = −0.20 ± 0.12
    F1,71.8 = 6.69, P = 0.0097**F1,73.7 = 2.86, P = 0.095.
    Model 2InterceptCoef = 0.31 ± 0.09Coef = −0.09 ± 0.09Coef = 0.05 ± 0.12
    F1,34.1 = 12.61, P = 0.0002F1,91.0 = 13.23, P = 0.0002F1,89.6 = 13.45, P = 0.0004
    Environmental predictabilityCoef = 0.20 ± 0.08Coef = 0.18 ± 0.08
    F1,91.7 = 6.73, P = 0.011*F1,92.6 = 4.86, P = 0.03*
    Environmental qualityCoef = 0.32 ± 0.09Coef = 0.32 ± 0.09
    F2,191.8 = 7.13, P = 0.0010**F2,192.2 = 7.18, P = 0.0010**
    Brood sizeCoef = −0.04 ± 0.02Coef = −0.04 ± 0.02
    F1,31.0 = 4.57, P = 0.033*F1,96.1 = 3.07, P = 0.083.
    Model 3InterceptCoef = 0.21 ± 0.07Coef = −0.06 ± 0.09Coef = 0.03 ± 0.1151 species, 98 studies, 317 effect sizes
    F1,79.1 = 9.14, P = 0.0034F1,81.7 = 15.70, P = 0.0002F1,78.6 = 9.04, P = 0.0036
    Environmental predictabilityCoef = 0.19 ± 0.08Coef = 0.09 ± 0.09
    F1,81.9 = 5.26, P = 0.024*F1,81.2 = 0.92, P = 0.34
    Environmental qualityCoef = 0.32 ± 0.09Coef = 0.32 ± 0.09
    F2,183.8 = 6.93, P = 0.0013**F2,182.4 = 6.86, P = 0.0013**
    Siblings present y/nCoef = −0.33 ± 0.11Coef = -0.27 ± 0.13
    F1,67.0 = 8.45, P = 0.0050**F1,69.3 = 4.14, P = 0.046*
    No. future broods possibleCoef = -0.02 ± 0.01Coef = −0.02 ± 0.01
    F1,87.9 = 3.13, P = 0.080.F1,89.9 = 2.03, P = 0.16
    Model 4InterceptCoef = 0.65 ± 0.14Coef = −0.06 ± 0.09Coef = 0.39 ± 0.18
    F1,78.1 = 20.21, P = 0.0000F1,81.7 = 15.70, P = 0.0002F1,80.6 = 19.77, P = 0.0000
    Environmental predictabilityCoef = 0.19 ± 0.08Coef = 0.12 ± 0.08
    F1,81.9 = 5.26, P = 0.024*F1,80.7 = 2.02, P = 0.16
    Environmental qualityCoef = 0.32 ± 0.09Coef = 0.32 ± 0.09
    F2,183.8 = 6.93, P = 0.0013**F2,184.3 = 6.86, P = 0.0013**
    Brood sizeCoef = −0.08 ± 0.02Coef = −0.07 ± 0.03
    F1,82.2 = 11.87, P = 0.0009***F1,85.1 = 8.2, P = 0.005**
    No. future broods possibleCoef = −0.03 ± 0.01Coef = −0.03 ± 0.01
    F1,83.5 = 7.22, P = 0.0087**F1,85.5 = 5.13, P = 0.026*
    Model 5InterceptCoef = 0.64 ± 0.18Coef = −0.06 ± 0.09Coef = 0.46 ± 0.2049 species, 96 studies, 314 effect sizes
    F1,76.5 = 8.58, P = 0.0045F1,80.7 = 14.47, P = 0.0003F1,76.1 = 8.42, P = 0.0049
    Environmental predictabilityCoef = 0.17 ± 0.08Coef = 0.07 ± 0.09
    F1,81.0 = 4.44, P = 0.038*F1,79.1 = 0.63, P = 0.43
    Environmental qualityCoef = 0.32 ± 0.09Coef = 0.32 ± 0.09
    F2,184.5 = 6.94, P = 0.0012**F2,185.3 = 6.79, P = 0.0014**
    Siblings present y/nCoef = −0.11 ± 0.14Coef = −0.07 ± 0.15
    F1,84.2 = 0.42, P = 0.42F1,83.7 = 0.19, P = 0.67
    No. future broods possibleCoef = −0.05 ± 0.01Coef = −0.04 ± 0.02
    F1,95.4 = 9.39, P = 0.0028**F1,96.4 = 7.87, P = 0.0061**
    Full vs. half siblings likelihoodCoef = −0.39 ± 0.15Coef = −0.39 ± 0.15
    F1,112.5 = 6.94, P = 0.0096**F1,111.5 = 6.76, P = 0.011*
    Model 6InterceptCoef = 0.83 ± 0.16Coef = −0.06 ± 0.09Coef = 0.62 ± 0.20
    F1,76.5 = 21.95, P = 0.0000F1,80.7 = 14.47, P = 0.0003F1,77.7 = 21.23, P = 0.0000
    Environmental predictabilityCoef = 0.17 ± 0.08Coef = 0.06 ± 0.08
    F1,81.0 = 4.44, P = 0.038*F1,78.9 = 0.62, P = 0.43
    Environmental qualityCoef = 0.32 ± 0.09Coef = 0.32 ± 0.09
    F2,184.5 = 6.94, P = 0.0012**F2,186.8 = 6.78, P = 0.0014**
    Brood sizeCoef = −0.05 ± 0.03Coef = -0.04 ± 0.03
    F1,87.1 = 3.07, P = 0.083.F1,87.1 = 2.51, P = 0.12
    No. future broods possibleCoef = −0.05 ± 0.01Coef = −0.05 ± 0.01
    F1,86.4 = 13.09, P = 0.00050***F1,89.7 = 10.15, P = 0.0020**
    Full vs. half siblings likelihoodCoef = −0.33 ± 0.14Coef = −0.31 ± 0.14
    F1,89.6 = 5.98, P = 0.016*F1,90.3 = 5.14, P = 0.026*
    Model 7InterceptCoef = 0.65 ± 0.23Coef = 0.08 ± 0.11Coef = 0.56 ± 0.2531 species, 68 studies, 230 effect sizes
    F1,55.6 = 4.93, P = 0.030F1,2.7 = 9.24, P = 0.0000F1,54.8 = 4.32, P = 0.045
    Environmental predictabilityCoef = 0.13 ± 0.10Coef = 0.07 ± 0.11
    F1,33.1 = 1.43, P = 0.24F1,58.1 = 4.73, P = 0.036*
    Environmental qualityCoef = 0.22 ± 0.09Coef = 0.22 ± 0.09
    F2,128.2 = 2.95, P = 0.056.F2,127.4 = 3.09, P = 0.049*
    Siblings present y/nCoef = 0.00 ± 0.22Coef = 0.06 ± 0.24
    F1,60.5 = 0, P = 1F1,60.2 = 1.11, P = 0.30
    No. future broods possibleCoef = -0.03 ± 0.02Coef = −0.03 ± 0.02
    F1,58.4 = 3.10, P = 0.083.F1,60.0 = 0.08, P = 0.79
    Full vs. half siblings likelihoodCoef = -0.43 ± 0.22Coef = −0.44 ± 0.22
    F1,76.0 = 3.77, P = 0.056.F1,74.9 = 1.07, P = 0.31
    Promiscuity (% EPPbr)Coef = 0.00 ± 0.01Coef = 0.00 ± 0.01
    F1,58.5 = 0.04, P = 0.84F1,57.9 = 1.22, P = 0.28
    Model 8InterceptCoef = 0.81 ± 0.19Coef = 0.08 ± 0.11Coef = 0.68 ± 0.23
    F1,55.9 = 17.26, P = 0.0001F1,2.7 = 9.24, P = 0.0000F1,55.4 = 18.95, P = 0.0001
    Environmental predictabilityCoef = 0.13 ± 0.10Coef = 0.05 ± 0.10
    F1,33.1 = 1.43, P = 0.24F1,57.5 = 4.73, P = 0.035*
    Environmental qualityCoef = 0.22 ± 0.09Coef = 0.22 ± 0.09
    F2,128.2 = 2.95, P = 0.056.F2,128.0 = 2.94, P = 0.057.
    Brood sizeCoef −0.07 ± 0.03Coef= −0.06 ± 0.03
    F1,66.2 = 3.87, P = 0.053.F1,64.6 = 3.29, P = 0.076.
    No. future broods possibleCoef = −0.04 ± 0.02Coef = −0.04 ± 0.02
    F1,58.3 = 4.94, P = 0.038*F1,60.5 = 0.03, P = 0.87
    Full vs. half siblings likelihoodCoef = -0.19 ± 0.18Coef = −0.18 ± 0.19
    F1,67.1 = 1.00, P = 0.32F1,67.8 = 2.45, P = 0.12
    Promiscuity (% EPPbr)Coef = 0.00 ± 0.01Coef = 0.00 ± 0.01
    F1,58.5 = 0.27, P = 0.61F1,57.5 = 1.24, P = 0.27
    • Coefficient, Wald statistic, and P value for fixed effects for three sets of models: (i) a model with life history variables only (models correspond to Table 1); (ii) a model with environmental variables only; and (iii) a model with all fixed effects included. Results are the mean from 500 ASReml linear mixed models. Models controlled for phylogeny, repeated measures, and were weighted by study sample size (the number of broods used to calculate the original test statistic). Fixed effects in bold are significant at the P < 0.05 level, and in italics at the P < 0.10 level.

    • View popup
    Table S4.

    Results for all signal-of-quality models: fixed effects

    Model no.Log LikFixed effectsdfWald statisticP valueN speciesN studyN observationsNotes
    9107.5Intercept1, 16.84.230.0561833140Full dataset
    Brood size1, 18.81.060.32
    10107.0Intercept1, 13.92.670.12
    Brood size1, 17.00.180.68
    Predictability1, 11.30.690.42
    Quality2, 133.07.610.00075
    Predictability * Quality2, 133.03.740.026
    1184.0Intercept1, 12.42.240.161630115Excluding 2 species missing data on adult mortality or the broods possible each breeding season
    Brood size1, 14.70.880.36
    No. of future broods possible1, 10.10.110.75
    1286.3Intercept1, 10.51.830.20
    Brood size1, 12.10.380.55
    No. of future broods possible1, 11.40.210.66
    Predictability1, 9.60.030.88
    Quality2, 107.010.180.00009
    1375.0Intercept1, 5.20.780.42102080Excluding 8 species missing data on adult mortality, the broods possible each breeding season, or mating system
    Brood size1, 5.70.000.97
    No. of future broods possible1, 4.30.240.65
    Full vs. half siblings likelihood1, 4.30.460.53
    1473.6Intercept1, 4.00.990.38
    Brood size1, 4.30.100.77
    No. of future broods possible1, 3.70.130.74
    Full vs. half siblings likelihood1, 3.40.060.82
    Predictability1, 3.60.160.72
    Quality1, 73.04.830.011
    • Mean results of 500 ASReml linear mixed models. Models controlled for phylogeny, repeated measures on studies, and species, and were weighted by study sample size (the number of broods used to calculate the original test statistic). Fixed effects in bold are significant at the P < 0.05 level and in italics at the P < 0.10 level. Models are grouped by the dataset used for analysis, and the notes column describes the loss in sample size due to missing life history data. Models did not converge when promiscuity was included as a fixed effect.

    • View popup
    Table S5.

    Results for all signal-of-quality models: random effects

    No.Log likelihoodTotal I2Random effectsComponentSEN speciesN studyN observationsNotes
    Null (a)110.45.87%Phylogeny0.01430.03291833140Full dataset
    Species0.01970.0259
    Study0.01180.0105
    Residual0.01660.0068
    9107.55.79%Phylogeny0.01490.0267
    Species0.01470.0215
    Study0.01460.0116
    Residual0.01620.0067
    Null (b)90.213.1%Phylogeny0.09800.03541630115Excluding 2 species missing data on adult mortality or the broods possible each breeding season
    Species0.02220.0285
    Study0.01060.0108
    Residual0.01970.0078
    1184.06.04%Phylogeny0.01100.0320
    Species0.02130.0259
    Study0.01280.0118
    Residual0.01920.0077
    Null (c)82.24.74%Phylogeny0.00460.1095102080Excluding 8 species missing data on adult mortality, the broods possible each breeding season, or mating system
    Species0.04500.0460
    Study0.00020.0035
    Residual0.0000NA
    1375.09.07%Phylogeny0.09880.0769
    Species0.0000NA
    Study0.00090.0044
    Residual0.0000NA
    • Mean results of 500 ASReml linear mixed models. Models were weighted by study sample size (the number of broods used to calculate the original test statistic). Models are grouped by the dataset used for analysis, and the notes column describes the loss in sample size due to missing life history data. Sample error variance was constrained to 1. Total heterogeneity (I2) is a measure of the proportion of observed variance due to true differences in effect sizes in the null model (34). I2 values of 25%, 50%, and 75% indicate low, moderate, and high levels ratios of signal to noise (34). This measure does not take effect size dispersion into account (34). Model numbers correspond to Table 1. NA indicates that models could not estimate the standard error of this variance component.

Data supplements

  • Supporting Information

    • Download Supporting Information (PDF)
    • Download Dataset_S01 (XLS)
    • Download Dataset_S02 (XLSX)
PreviousNext
Back to top
Article Alerts
Email Article

Thank you for your interest in spreading the word on PNAS.

NOTE: We only request your email address so that the person you are recommending the page to knows that you wanted them to see it, and that it is not junk mail. We do not capture any email address.

Enter multiple addresses on separate lines or separate them with commas.
Sibling conflict and dishonest signaling in birds
(Your Name) has sent you a message from PNAS
(Your Name) thought you would like to see the PNAS web site.
CAPTCHA
This question is for testing whether or not you are a human visitor and to prevent automated spam submissions.
Citation Tools
Sibling rivalry and the evolution of dishonesty
Shana M. Caro, Stuart A. West, Ashleigh S. Griffin
Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences Nov 2016, 113 (48) 13803-13808; DOI: 10.1073/pnas.1606378113

Citation Manager Formats

  • BibTeX
  • Bookends
  • EasyBib
  • EndNote (tagged)
  • EndNote 8 (xml)
  • Medlars
  • Mendeley
  • Papers
  • RefWorks Tagged
  • Ref Manager
  • RIS
  • Zotero
Request Permissions
Share
Sibling rivalry and the evolution of dishonesty
Shana M. Caro, Stuart A. West, Ashleigh S. Griffin
Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences Nov 2016, 113 (48) 13803-13808; DOI: 10.1073/pnas.1606378113
del.icio.us logo Digg logo Reddit logo Twitter logo CiteULike logo Facebook logo Google logo Mendeley logo
  • Tweet Widget
  • Facebook Like
  • Mendeley logo Mendeley

Article Classifications

  • Biological Sciences
  • Evolution

See related content:

  • Begging, bragging, and deceit
    - Nov 21, 2016
Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences: 113 (48)
Table of Contents

Submit

Sign up for Article Alerts

Jump to section

  • Article
    • Abstract
    • SI Materials and Methods
    • Results and Discussion
    • Conclusion
    • Materials and Methods
    • Acknowledgments
    • Footnotes
    • References
  • Figures & SI
  • Info & Metrics
  • PDF

You May Also be Interested in

Setting sun over a sun-baked dirt landscape
Core Concept: Popular integrated assessment climate policy models have key caveats
Better explicating the strengths and shortcomings of these models will help refine projections and improve transparency in the years ahead.
Image credit: Witsawat.S.
Model of the Amazon forest
News Feature: A sea in the Amazon
Did the Caribbean sweep into the western Amazon millions of years ago, shaping the region’s rich biodiversity?
Image credit: Tacio Cordeiro Bicudo (University of São Paulo, São Paulo, Brazil), Victor Sacek (University of São Paulo, São Paulo, Brazil), and Lucy Reading-Ikkanda (artist).
Syrian archaeological site
Journal Club: In Mesopotamia, early cities may have faltered before climate-driven collapse
Settlements 4,200 years ago may have suffered from overpopulation before drought and lower temperatures ultimately made them unsustainable.
Image credit: Andrea Ricci.
Steamboat Geyser eruption.
Eruption of Steamboat Geyser
Mara Reed and Michael Manga explore why Yellowstone's Steamboat Geyser resumed erupting in 2018.
Listen
Past PodcastsSubscribe
Birds nestling on tree branches
Parent–offspring conflict in songbird fledging
Some songbird parents might improve their own fitness by manipulating their offspring into leaving the nest early, at the cost of fledgling survival, a study finds.
Image credit: Gil Eckrich (photographer).

Similar Articles

Site Logo
Powered by HighWire
  • Submit Manuscript
  • Twitter
  • Facebook
  • RSS Feeds
  • Email Alerts

Articles

  • Current Issue
  • Special Feature Articles – Most Recent
  • List of Issues

PNAS Portals

  • Anthropology
  • Chemistry
  • Classics
  • Front Matter
  • Physics
  • Sustainability Science
  • Teaching Resources

Information

  • Authors
  • Editorial Board
  • Reviewers
  • Subscribers
  • Librarians
  • Press
  • Site Map
  • PNAS Updates
  • FAQs
  • Accessibility Statement
  • Rights & Permissions
  • About
  • Contact

Feedback    Privacy/Legal

Copyright © 2021 National Academy of Sciences. Online ISSN 1091-6490