Skip to main content

Main menu

  • Home
  • Articles
    • Current
    • Special Feature Articles - Most Recent
    • Special Features
    • Colloquia
    • Collected Articles
    • PNAS Classics
    • List of Issues
  • Front Matter
    • Front Matter Portal
    • Journal Club
  • News
    • For the Press
    • This Week In PNAS
    • PNAS in the News
  • Podcasts
  • Authors
    • Information for Authors
    • Editorial and Journal Policies
    • Submission Procedures
    • Fees and Licenses
  • Submit
  • Submit
  • About
    • Editorial Board
    • PNAS Staff
    • FAQ
    • Accessibility Statement
    • Rights and Permissions
    • Site Map
  • Contact
  • Journal Club
  • Subscribe
    • Subscription Rates
    • Subscriptions FAQ
    • Open Access
    • Recommend PNAS to Your Librarian

User menu

  • Log in
  • My Cart

Search

  • Advanced search
Home
Home
  • Log in
  • My Cart

Advanced Search

  • Home
  • Articles
    • Current
    • Special Feature Articles - Most Recent
    • Special Features
    • Colloquia
    • Collected Articles
    • PNAS Classics
    • List of Issues
  • Front Matter
    • Front Matter Portal
    • Journal Club
  • News
    • For the Press
    • This Week In PNAS
    • PNAS in the News
  • Podcasts
  • Authors
    • Information for Authors
    • Editorial and Journal Policies
    • Submission Procedures
    • Fees and Licenses
  • Submit
Research Article

Female sociality and sexual conflict shape offspring survival in a Neotropical primate

Urs Kalbitzer, Mackenzie L. Bergstrom, Sarah D. Carnegie, Eva C. Wikberg, Shoji Kawamura, Fernando A. Campos, Katharine M. Jack, and Linda M. Fedigan
  1. aDepartment of Anthropology and Archaeology, University of Calgary, Calgary, AB, Canada T2N 1N4;
  2. bDepartment of Integrated Biosciences, University of Tokyo, Kashiwa, Chiba 277-8562, Japan;
  3. cDepartment of Anthropology, Tulane University, New Orleans, LA 70118

See allHide authors and affiliations

PNAS February 21, 2017 114 (8) 1892-1897; first published February 6, 2017; https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1608625114
Urs Kalbitzer
aDepartment of Anthropology and Archaeology, University of Calgary, Calgary, AB, Canada T2N 1N4;
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
  • For correspondence: urs.kalbitzer@ucalgary.ca
Mackenzie L. Bergstrom
aDepartment of Anthropology and Archaeology, University of Calgary, Calgary, AB, Canada T2N 1N4;
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Sarah D. Carnegie
aDepartment of Anthropology and Archaeology, University of Calgary, Calgary, AB, Canada T2N 1N4;
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Eva C. Wikberg
aDepartment of Anthropology and Archaeology, University of Calgary, Calgary, AB, Canada T2N 1N4;
bDepartment of Integrated Biosciences, University of Tokyo, Kashiwa, Chiba 277-8562, Japan;
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Shoji Kawamura
bDepartment of Integrated Biosciences, University of Tokyo, Kashiwa, Chiba 277-8562, Japan;
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Fernando A. Campos
aDepartment of Anthropology and Archaeology, University of Calgary, Calgary, AB, Canada T2N 1N4;
cDepartment of Anthropology, Tulane University, New Orleans, LA 70118
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Katharine M. Jack
cDepartment of Anthropology, Tulane University, New Orleans, LA 70118
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Linda M. Fedigan
aDepartment of Anthropology and Archaeology, University of Calgary, Calgary, AB, Canada T2N 1N4;
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
  1. Edited by Ryne Palombit, Rutgers University, and accepted by Editorial Board Member James O’Connell January 5, 2017 (received for review May 31, 2016)

  • Article
  • Figures & SI
  • Info & Metrics
  • PDF
Loading

Significance

In group-living mammals, infants of females with strong social relationships sometimes exhibit higher survivorship than infants of less social females, a finding that holds true in our study population of wild white-faced capuchin monkeys. However, as in many mammals, new alpha male capuchins often kill young infants sired by other males. Our long-term research shows that infants of highly social females are at greater risk of dying or disappearing during periods of alpha male replacements than infants of less social females. These findings indicate that new alpha males are more likely to target the infants of more social, and therefore central, females. Our study provides evidence that female sociality can negatively affect offspring survival by increasing the likelihood of infanticide.

Abstract

Most mammals live in social groups in which members form differentiated social relationships. Individuals may vary in their degree of sociality, and this variation can be associated with differential fitness. In some species, for example, female sociality has a positive effect on infant survival. However, investigations of such cases are still rare, and no previous study has considered how male infanticide might constrain effects of female sociality on infant survival. Infanticide is part of the male reproductive strategy in many mammals, and it has the potential to override, or even reverse, effects of female reproductive strategies, including sociality. Therefore, we investigated the relationships between female sociality, offspring survival, and infanticide risk in wild white-faced capuchin monkeys using long-term data from Santa Rosa, Costa Rica. Female capuchins formed differentiated bonds, and bond strength was predicted by kin relationship, rank difference, and the presence of female infants. Most females formed stable bonds with their top social partners, although bond stability varied considerably. Offspring of highly social females, who were often high-ranking females, exhibited higher survivorship during stable periods compared with offspring of less social females. However, offspring of highly social females were more likely to die or disappear during periods of alpha male replacements, probably because new alpha males are central to the group, and therefore more likely to target the infants of highly social, central females. This study shows that female sociality in mammals can have negative fitness consequences that are imposed by male behavior.

  • social bond
  • reproductive success
  • mammal
  • capuchin
  • infanticide

In group-living mammals, individuals often form differentiated social relationships with other group members, resulting in variation in their degree of sociality (1). Over the past 14 y, several studies have investigated the fitness consequences of individual variation in sociality. For example, female chacma baboons (Papio ursinus) (2) and female yellow baboons (Papio cynocephalus) (3) with strong and stable bonds have a longer life expectancy than females with weaker and less stable bonds. More social male and female Barbary macaques (Macaca sylvanus) also have a higher chance of survival during adverse conditions, such as very cold winters (4, 5). Although published data with regard to sociality and male reproductive success are rare, Schülke et al. (6) found that male Assamese macaques (Macaca assamensis) with strong bonds sire more offspring than do males with weaker bonds. In terms of female sociality and reproductive success, studies on yellow baboons (7), chacma baboons (8), bottlenose dolphins (Tursiops sp.) (9), and feral horses (Equus caballus) (10) have shown that offspring survival is higher for females with stronger social bonds or better social integration compared with less social females. However, the number of publications on sociality and fitness is still relatively small. Even primate research, which represents a considerable proportion of the literature on this topic, has been limited to species belonging to the Old World monkey tribe of Papionini.

Furthermore, previous studies on female sociality and reproductive success mainly focused on the role of female strategies, but such strategies do not occur in isolation and can be affected by male strategies. Male and female reproductive strategies differ from one another considerably in most mammals: Female mammals usually invest more energy in each offspring via gestation and lactation, whereas males invest more into intense competition over access to a larger number of receptive females (11). These divergent reproductive strategies can, and do, often result in highly pronounced sexual conflict (11, 12). The killing of dependent offspring by unrelated males, infanticide, is one of several manifestations of sexual conflict that has been reported in many species of social mammals (13⇓–15). The most widely supported explanation for this behavior is that infanticide is a reproductive strategy whereby males can maximize their number of offspring by killing unrelated dependent infants (16). In his review of infanticide across primates, Palombit (16) found that infanticide as a male reproductive strategy coincides with a changeover in the group’s alpha male, and is particularly relevant for species showing a high reproductive skew. The killing of dependent infants can induce a female to resume cycling earlier than if the infant survives and open up the possibility that she will reproduce with the new alpha male. For females, losing an infant to male infanticide can destroy substantial reproductive effort within moments and negate female social strategies that would otherwise be beneficial to infant survival, such as forming social bonds with other females.

To extend our knowledge about the adaptive value of female social bonds in mammals, and to investigate whether male infanticide can interfere with female sociality as a strategy that increases infant survival, we analyzed behavioral and life-history data from a population of white-faced capuchin monkeys (Cebus capucinus imitator) in Santa Rosa, Costa Rica. This species of New World monkey (Platyrrhini) lives in multimale–multifemale groups in which the philopatric females are highly social (17⇓–19). Furthermore, females that groom each other more often also tend to form coalitions more often (19). However, the fitness repercussions of such bonds have not been thoroughly investigated. Males, on the other hand, disperse from their natal group at ∼4 y of age and continue to disperse throughout their lives, changing groups at ∼4-y intervals (20, 21). This pattern of frequent male movement between groups leads to complete (or near-complete) changeovers in male group members, including alpha males. Infant mortality is significantly greater in the context of alpha male replacements (AMRs) than during times of group stability (18, 22⇓–24).

To clarify the effect of female sociality on infant survival in the context of both male and female reproductive strategies, we investigated the predictors of female bond strength, the stability of female bonds, and the consequences of female sociality for offspring survival in wild capuchin monkeys across periods of varying group stability. Because of the similarities between the capuchin social system and the social system of baboons, we expected that offspring survival would be correlated with female sociality. However, because of the impact of AMRs on infant survival, we expected that female sociality would only have a positive effect on infant survival during times of group stability. Alpha males are usually located centrally within the group (25); therefore, we had earlier postulated that the offspring of more central females would be more vulnerable to aggression by new alpha males than the offspring of more peripheral females (26). Because we predicted that the most social females are also the most central ones, we expected that when infanticide risk is high during AMR periods, highly social females would have poorer infant survival rates than do less social ones.

Results

Predictors of Dyadic Bonds.

Female social bonds were highly variable between dyads and within the same dyads over time. The average coefficient of variation (CV) for the dyadic sociality index (DSI) was 86% between different dyads within the same year, compared with 67% within the same dyad over different years. This variation in bond strength within and between dyads was significantly related to three of the tested predictor variables: relatedness, dominance rank difference, and presence of female infants (Table 1). Dyads with higher R-values (i.e., estimated relatedness) and dyads in which female infants were present showed stronger bonds than did other dyads. Also, dyads with larger differences in rank showed weaker bonds than did dyads with more adjacent rank positions. The presence of male infants and age differences had no effect on bond strength.

View this table:
  • View inline
  • View popup
Table 1.

Predictors of dyadic bond strength

Running the same model with kinship categories instead of R-values produced similar results. We found, as before, that kinship (χ2 = 33.079, df = 4, P < 0.001), rank differences (χ2 = 18.791, df = 1, P < 0.001), and presence of female infants (χ2 = 26.617, df = 1, P < 0.001) had a significant effect on bond strength (Table S1). More specifically, mother–daughter dyads formed stronger bonds than did maternal sibs, paternal sibs, and nonkin dyads, but did not differ in bond strength from full-sib dyads (Fig. 1). Full sibs formed stronger bonds than did nonkin dyads, whereas maternal and paternal sibs could not be distinguished from each other or from nonkin dyads.

Fig. 1.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Fig. 1.

Differences in bond strength among different kin categories. Each point represents the log-transformed DSI (+0.1) value for each dyad during each year in which both females were coresident (n = 499 data points from 134 dyads and 32 females; dyads with insufficient information about kin relationships and grandmother–granddaughter dyads, of which only two existed within our dataset, were sorted out). The boxes represent the estimate of the LMM with upper and lower 95% confidence limits for each of the kin categories (assuming mean values for all other variables in the model). The lines and asterisks at the top of the figure denote the significant differences between kin categories. *P < 0.05; ***P < 0.001 (P values were adjusted using the Tukey method).

View this table:
  • View inline
  • View popup
Table S1.

Predictors of dyadic bond strength using kinship categories instead of R-values

Number of Top Partners.

The DSI values of the three strongest bonds per female were, on average, significantly stronger than the mean DSI of 1 (results from one-sided t tests using log-transformed DSI + 0.1 values: first top partner vs. mean DSI: t = 11.746, df = 31, P < 0.001; secondary bond partner vs. mean DSI: t = 7.481, df = 31, P < 0.001; tertiary bond partner vs. mean DSI: t = 2.805, df = 31, P < 0.01; and quaternary bond partner vs. mean DSI: t = −2.313, df = 31, P = 0.986; Fig. S1). The three partners with the highest DSI values were therefore considered as top partners.

Fig. S1.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Fig. S1.

Averaged bond strengths for different partner ranks (i.e., first top partner, secondary top partner, etc.). Box plots depict log-transformed DSI (+0.1) values (n = 285), for which all dyads were included during each year both females were coresident. The vertical dashed line indicates the average bond strength [which is, per definition, DSI = 1 (SI Materials and Methods, Method S1: Calculation of DSI), and therefore log (1.1) in this plot]. Asterisks above boxplots indicate significant differences between bond strength for each partner rank and average bond strength: **P < 0.01 and ***P < 0.001 (results from t tests using log-transformed DSI + 0.1 values).

Stability of Bonds with Top Partners.

Despite the large temporal variation in bond strength within dyads over years (CV = 67%, discussed above), the comparison between observed partner stability index (PSI) values calculated for the top three partners and randomized PSI values indicated that observed bonds were more stable than expected by chance (one-sided Wilcoxon test: Wilcoxon test statistic V = 133, P < 0.05, n = 31). However, the difference between observed and randomized PSI values was relatively small (mean ± SD: observed PSI = 0.382 ± 0.207, simulated PSI = 0.309 ± 0.072), and females showed considerable variation in observed bond stability, ranging from 0 to 1, whereas simulated PSI values only ranged from 0.206 to 0.430 (Fig. S2). Thus, some female capuchin monkeys formed stable strong bonds, but other females changed their top three partners often.

Fig. S2.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Fig. S2.

Stability of bonds with top three partners. The plot shows the histograms for observed and simulated PSI values.

Predictors of Annual Sociality Indices.

Overall sociality of each female during each year was significantly related to two of the tested predictor variables (Table 2). First, high-ranking females were more social than low-ranking females [Table 2; plus a Pearson’s correlation test between annual sociality indices (ASI) and rank indicators: r = 0.499 and P < 0.001]. Second, adult females with female infants were more social than females without female infants. In accordance with our results on dyadic bond strength, the presence of male infants did not have such an effect.

View this table:
  • View inline
  • View popup
Table 2.

Predictors for ASI

Sociality and Centrality.

Annually calculated centrality values of females ranged from 0.591 to 0.991 (mean ± SD = 0.847 ± 0.094). Thus, despite considerable variation among females, no female was exclusively peripheral. Nevertheless, results from a linear mixed model (LMM) with ASI as the predictor variable for centrality values indicate that highly social females were more central than less social females (estimate ± SE: ASI = 0.089 ± 0.029, χ2 = 7.319, P < 0.01, n = 84 data points from 31 different females and 3 different years; Fig. S3).

Fig. S3.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Fig. S3.

Relationship between annual centrality values and sociality (ASI) of females. The dashed line depicts the prediction of the LMM, and dotted lines depict the bootstrapped 95% confidence intervals (n = 84 data points from 31 different females and 3 different years).

Infant Survival.

In total, 75 infants were included in this study. Fifty of these infants were at low risk of being killed by a new alpha male, and 76% of these 50 infants survived their first year of life. The other 25 infants were at high infanticide risk and only 44% survived their first year of life (Table S2). Infant survival was significantly predicted by the interaction between ASI and infanticide risk, which means that the effect of female sociality on infant survival was dependent on the level of infanticide risk (Fig. 2 and Table 3). In comparison to infants of mothers with a low sociality value, infants of highly social mothers had a higher probability of surviving their first year of life during times of low infanticide risk but a lower probability of surviving during periods of high infanticide risk. This result was confirmed when we divided the data into two sets: one that only included infants at low infanticide risk and the other that only included infants at high infanticide risk. We ran a generalized linear mixed model (GLMM) with each of these two datasets testing ASI as a predictor variable (low infanticide risk, estimate ± SE: ASI = 0.946 ± 0.400, χ2 = 6.706, P < 0.01; high infanticide risk, estimate ± SE: ASI = −1.180 ± 0.729, χ2 = 4.799, P < 0.05).

Fig. 2.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Fig. 2.

Effect of female sociality on infant survival for infants at low risk of infanticide (A; n = 50 infants) and infants at high risk of infanticide (B; n = 25 infants). The circles (●) show the observed survival probability, summarized for five equally large intervals of ASI, and the area of the circles is proportional to the (square rooted) number of infants included in each of these intervals. The dashed lines show the survival probability predicted by the GLMM, and the dotted lines depict the bootstrapped 95% confidence intervals (number of bootstraps = 1,000). The GLMM was calculated with ASI scaled to a mean of 0 and SD of 1 but shown here on the original scale from 0 to 1.

View this table:
  • View inline
  • View popup
Table 3.

Predictors of infant survival

View this table:
  • View inline
  • View popup
Table S2.

Infanticide risk and survival rates for infants born during the study periods

From a lifetime perspective, neither infants of highly social or less social females had a higher survival probability, because a model excluding the interaction between ASI and infanticide risk showed no main effect of female sociality (estimate ± SE: ASI = 0.150 ± 0.288, χ2 = 0.277, P = 0.598; Table S3, model b). Nevertheless, the model confirmed previous results (22, 23) that infants at low infanticide risk show a higher survival probability than infants at high infanticide risk (estimate ± SE: infanticide risk (low) = 1.601 ± 0.643, χ2 = 8.008, P < 0.01). Infants of females with stable top-bond partners had no survival advantage either. A model with PSI as the predictor variable showed no significant relation between PSI and infant survival (estimate ± SE: PSI = 0.478 ± 0.356, χ2 = 2.266, P = 0.132; Table S3, model c).

View this table:
  • View inline
  • View popup
Table S3.

Results of different GLMMs testing for predictors of infant survival (first year of life) as the response variable

Because maternal rank and ASI were highly correlated (discussed above), we did not include rank in the original GLMM testing for the effect of sociality on infant survival. When we replaced ASI with rank in this model, we obtained similar results: The interaction between rank and infanticide risk was significantly related to infant survival (χ2 = 4.287, P < 0.05; Fig. S4 and Table S3, model d). Infants of high-ranking mothers showed a higher chance of survival when they were at low infanticide risk (estimate ± SE: rank = 0.788 ± 0.429, χ2 = 4.711, P < 0.05). However, we did not detect a significant effect of rank on survival of infants at high risk (estimate ± SE: rank = −0.774 ± 1.445, χ2 = 0.994, P = 0.319). Similar to sociality, a model excluding the interaction between rank and infanticide risk did not indicate a lifetime effect of rank on infant survival (estimate ± SE: rank = 0.338 ± 0.300, χ2 = 1.321, P = 0.250; Table S3, model e).

Fig. S4.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Fig. S4.

Effect of female rank on infant survival for infants at low risk of infanticide (A; n = 50 infants), and infants at high risk of infanticide (B; n = 25 infants). The circles (●) show the observed survival probability, summarized for five equally large intervals of rank, and the area of the circles is proportional to the (square rooted) number of infants included in each of these intervals. The dashed line shows the survival probability predicted by the GLMM, and the dotted lines depict the bootstrapped 95% confidence intervals (number of bootstraps = 1,000).

The comparison of different model fits indicated that sociality accounted for components of variation in infant survival not explained by rank. The model with both interactions included (ASI:infanticide risk + rank:infanticide risk) was significantly better than the model only including the interaction rank:infanticide risk (χ2 = 7.589, df = 2, P < 0.05; Table S3, models d and f). However, the model with both interactions included was not significantly better than the model only including the interaction ASI:infanticide risk (χ2 = 1.551, df = 2, P = 0.4605; Table S3, models a and f). Thus, although sociality explained variation in infant survival distinct from the effect of rank, the addition of rank did not significantly increase the explanatory value of the model.

Discussion

Female white-faced capuchin monkeys form highly variable social bonds with other females, and variation in individual sociality has consequences for offspring survival. Similar to other highly social mammals (27⇓⇓–30), the strength of social bonds was associated with relatedness and rank differences (also ref. 31). Furthermore, “natal attraction,” a phenomenon shown in numerous studies whereby females with infants form stronger bonds than females without infants (e.g., refs 32, 33), was also exhibited by females in our study population, but this effect was limited to mothers with female infants. Perhaps such is the case because female offspring usually stay in the group, and therefore represent prospective bond partners, whereas males emigrate as subadults or adults (18, 26). Alternatively, females may simply have fewer opportunities to interact with male offspring because adult males in this species show great interest in infants (34), particularly in male infants (35).

As predicted from studies of other species (7⇓⇓–10), variation in overall female sociality was associated with variation in offspring survival in white-faced capuchins. This effect, however, was dependent on male behavior; we observed a positive effect of maternal sociality on infant survival only for infants at low male infanticide risk, whereas infants born during periods of high infanticide risk (shortly before or after an AMR) had a higher chance of survival if their mothers were less social females. Infants at low infanticide risk and born to highly social females may benefit from having better access to preferred food resources and the protection provided by group males against predators. Additionally, infant white-faced capuchin monkeys often acquire supplementary milk from other females [i.e., allonursing (18, 19, 36)], and infants of highly social females may have more opportunities to engage in allonursing. Furthermore, our results show that highly social females are found significantly more often in the center of the group; therefore, their infants may be less vulnerable to predators (8).

However, alpha males are usually central to the group as well (25), and if the resident alpha male is defeated and disperses, the central females will slowly start to accept the presence of the new alpha male by grooming him when he approaches (18). During the period of accepting the new alpha male, the offspring of these resident females [which are rarely weaned before 1 y, and are therefore close to their mother (36)] experience high rates of wounding, death, and disappearance (this study and refs. 22⇓–24). Alternative explanations, such as increased predation pressure or feeding competition, cannot account for this pattern. A previous study showed that infant survival was much lower during periods of AMRs compared with ecologically comparable periods of group stability (22). Furthermore, annual rainfall, a proxy for resource availability, is not a significant predictor of infant survival in our population (23). Therefore, it appears that the central positions of highly social females are responsible for the higher vulnerability of their dependent infants during and after AMR periods (26). Offspring of less social females, on the other hand, may benefit from the peripheral position of their mothers during high-risk times.

If being highly social has a negative effect on infant survival during AMR periods, why do females not adjust their sociality accordingly? Overall, sociality appears to be relatively variable over time, and it would be reasonable to assume that lactating or pregnant females could become less social (and therefore more peripheral) during takeover times. The observation that infants of highly social females are at higher risk of infanticide during such periods, however, suggests that females either cannot or choose not to do so. Other fitness benefits of high sociality could make it worthwhile to remain highly social despite the high infanticide risk. The results of the main-effect models indicate that neither strategy, being highly social or less social, results in a long-term benefit with regard to infant survival. Thus, high sociality could provide advantages for female capuchins other than maximizing infant survival, such as increasing longevity, which has been shown for female baboons (2). Because long-lived iteroparous organisms should prioritize individual survival over the survival of any particular offspring, high sociality may be favored by selection for pregnant or lactating females even during AMRs if the benefits in terms of adult female survival are sufficiently strong.

Rank was positively correlated with individual sociality indices; therefore, it is not surprising that we detected a similar effect of rank on infant survival depending on infanticide risk. An association (usually positive) between maternal rank and offspring survival is prevalent in many mammalian species (37, 38), but was not documented in previous studies of our study population (39). In the present study, we were able to detect this link because we determined infanticide risk by assessing conception time and birth date in relation to AMR events instead of using (rarely) confirmed instances of infanticide, and we included the interaction between infanticide risk and rank (or sociality) in our models. The idea that maternal rank can affect the risk for infants of being attacked by infanticidal males has been suggested before (26, 40) but, to our knowledge, not yet systematically tested. Our study shows that the explicit consideration of infanticide risk is a promising approach to studying the fitness consequences of both sociality and rank in mammalian species that exhibit male infanticide.

The comparison of different models indicated that sociality explained variation in infant survival distinct from rank as an explanation for variation in infant survival. However, the reverse was not the case in white-faced capuchin monkeys. Rank positions in our population are relatively stable [about one rank change per female every 2.34 y as a result of active challenge (41)]. In contrast, bond strength was highly variable and individual sociality showed more variation over time than rank positions (Fig. S5). As outlined above, pregnant and lactating females seem not to be able to adapt their sociality during AMR periods, at least not sufficiently to mitigate the higher mortality risk to infants. Such an inability to adapt would be expected for a stable trait, such as rank, but not for an apparently highly dynamic trait, such as sociality. Therefore, future studies evaluating how females of different rank positions and in different reproductive stages change their social behavior during the establishment of new alpha males would help to clarify why highly social females do not simply become less social during periods of high infanticide risk.

Fig. S5.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Fig. S5.

Changes in ASI and dominance ranks over time. (Top) Three plots show changes over time in ASI values for each group and female separately. (Bottom) Three plots show the same for dominance ranks.

Our study contributes to the understanding of fitness implications of sociality in mammals by providing another example of the relationship between female sociality and offspring survival. In particular, it provides much needed data on a Neotropical primate by providing evidence that an association between sociality and offspring survival exists in a nonhuman primate species outside Papionini, making more generalizable the claim that there are fitness consequences of sociality. More importantly, however, we have shown how male behavior, namely, infanticide during AMRs, can periodically render sociality as a female reproductive strategy disadvantageous. This study also confirms that female–female bonds seem not to represent an effective strategy in reducing male infanticide risk; instead, they can even increase this risk. To our knowledge, no other studies have demonstrated such a trade-off for female sociality in a group-living mammal.

Materials and Methods

The research reported in this study adhered to the laws of Costa Rica, the United States, and Canada, and complied with protocols approved by the Área de Conservación Guanacaste, by Tulane University’s Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee, and by the Canada Research Council for Animal Care through the University of Calgary’s Life and Environmental Sciences Animal Care Committee.

Behavioral Observations and Life-History Data.

We derived data on the social behavior of females, and the dates of birth, death, or disappearance of females and infants from the long-term Santa Rosa Primate dataset (39). This project began in Costa Rica in 1983, and since its onset, life-history data have been collected on a continuous basis from females in several study groups. For this study, we considered females to be adult once they reached the age of 5 y, which is the youngest age at which females become pregnant (42).

We analyzed behavioral and life-history data from 32 adult females residing in three different groups: Los Valles (LV), Cerco de Piedra (CP), and Guanacaste (GN). For the LV and CP groups, we included data from 2005 until 2011, and for the GN group, we included data from 2007 until 2011. Each female provided between 1 and 7 y of data (mean ± SD = 4.7 ± 1.9 y), totaling to 149 female years. Groups included five to 11 females, resulting in 151 coresident dyads during this study period, and 557 data points if each dyad is counted once per year.

We derived information on social bonds from behavioral data collected during focal sample observations. During each study year, data were collected from each group for between 4 and 12 mo, and between 2.5 and 55.1 h per individual (mean ± SD = 13.7 ± 13.7 h), resulting in 5–107.7 h of observation time per dyad per year (mean ± SD = 22.5 ± 22 h). We calculated sociality indices per year (discussed below); thus, interannual variation in focal hours should not bias these indices, although indices may be more precise in years with more focal data.

Dominance Ranks and Kin Relationship.

We calculated ranks for each year separately, based on focal and ad libitum data using the I&SI (i.e., minimizing number and strength of inconsistencies in dominance matrices) method (41). We then scaled ranks from 0 (lowest rank in the group) to 1 (highest rank in the group) by calculating 1− Rank−1Ranks in group−1.

We used two different estimates to assess dyadic relatedness. First, we calculated R-values from genotypes at 20 short tandem-repeat loci (43). These values do not always fit the expected values for kin categories (44), but are well-suited as continuous predictor variables to estimate in general the effect of relatedness on social behavior. To obtain a better understanding of how kin relationships would affect social bonds, we established different kin categories, including mother–daughter, full sibs, maternal sibs, paternal sibs, and maternal grandmother–granddaughter dyads. We considered all other dyads as “nonkin.” We determined members of kinship categories from known mother–daughter relationships and from paternal relationships determined via parentage analysis (34).

Dyadic Bond Strength.

To make our results comparable to the results of previous studies, we closely followed the procedures described by Silk et al. (30) to determine social bonds in baboons and calculated the DSI, considering approaches and grooming (SI Materials and Methods, Method S1: Calculation of DSI).

Predictors of Bond Strength.

To investigate the determinants of bond strength, we ran LMMs; we included R-values, age, rank differences within dyad, and the number of male and female infants younger than 1 y present in the dyad as predictor variables. We considered infants as “present” if they were in the group for at least 20% of the time during which the DSI was assessed for their mother. We then recalculated the model using kin categories instead of R-values to obtain a better understanding as to how kinship affects social bonds, and to test whether maternal kinship is more important than paternal kinship.

Number of Top Partners and Stability of Top Bonds.

For each female, we calculated averaged DSI values for each partner rank (SI Materials and Methods, Method S2: Calculation of Average DSI Values for Each Partner Rank). We then used a one-sided t test with log-transformed values (+0.1) to test whether the averaged DSI for each partner rank was stronger than the average DSI in the group, the latter of which was 1 by definition (because dyadic rates of every behavior were divided by the average rate within the group). We defined all DSI partner ranks significantly stronger than 1 as “top partners.”

Using the number of top partners, we calculated the PSI as defined by Silk et al. (45), but corrected the index for small group sizes (SI Materials and Methods, Method S3: Calculation of the PSI and Simulated PSI Values). The resulting PSI values range from 0 to 1; 0 means that the female had the maximum number of different top partners, and 1 means that a female always kept her top partners and bonds were thus highly stable over time. To determine whether bonds were more stable than expected by chance, we simulated random PSI values (45) (SI Materials and Methods, Method S3: Calculation of the PSI and Simulated PSI Values). Then, we compared simulated PSI values of each female with the observed PSI values using a Wilcoxon signed rank test. We used a one-sided test because we were specifically interested in whether females would form more stable bonds than expected by chance.

Predictors of Sociality.

In addition to dyadic values, we were interested in assessing whether females varied with regard to sociality in general and in investigating the predictors and consequences of this variation. To obtain such values, previous studies (2, 8) calculated a lifetime sociality index by averaging DSI values with the top three partners during the entire study for each female. Our results indicated that females had stronger than average bonds with the top three partners, but there was a lot of variation over time with regard to bond stability and many females had highly unstable top partners. Therefore, we calculated ASI instead of a lifetime sociality index by averaging the top three partners each year. We scaled these values within each group and each year from 0 (the least social female) to 1 (the most social female). The resulting ASI reflects whether a female was more or less social compared with other females in the same group during a specific year. This approach might also be more appropriate if the basic ability to form strong bonds (compared with the ability to form very long-lasting strong bonds) is important.

We then used an LMM to test if ASI is related to the annual rank of a female and/or the presence of male or female infants during each year.

Sociality and Centrality.

To test whether highly social females are also more central to the group, we used data on spatial position collected during focal observations, which were available from 31 females in 3 different years (2009, 2010, and 2011). During each 10-min focal session, the position relative to the group’s center (1 = central, 0.5 = intermediate, 0 = peripheral) was noted every 2.5 min, resulting in five data points per focal session. We averaged these values for each focal observation, resulting in a centrality index ranging from 0 (completely peripheral) to 1 (completely central). Based on these values, we calculated average centrality values for each female per year.

Maternal Sociality and Infant Survival.

To assess the effect of sociality on infant survival, we calculated a GLMM using the binomial response of whether the infant survived its first year of life (Yes/No). The predictor variables included were ASI, male infanticide risk, and the interaction ASI:infanticide risk to test whether the effect of maternal sociality on infant survival depends on the level of infanticide risk. We considered infants to be “at high risk” if one of the following two conditions applied: (i) The infant was younger than 1 y during an AMR, or (ii) the infant was born during the 5.5 mo following the establishment of a new alpha male, which is the gestation length for this species (46), and we can thus assume that the infant was not sired by the new alpha male. Because dominance rank and ASI were highly correlated, we did not include rank in these models, but we ran alternative models using rank instead of ASI. To test for long-term effects of maternal sociality, rank, and partner stability on infant survival, we calculated models that included sociality, rank, or PSI, but not the interactions with infanticide risk.

To investigate whether the effect of sociality on infant survival simply reflects the effect of rank or, vice versa, rank simply reflects the effect of sociality, we compared different models using χ2 tests. Earlier, we did not calculate the model with both sociality and rank because these two variables were highly correlated. Here, however, we compared the model with both of these variables with the models with either sociality or rank included to investigate whether the addition of the second variable would significantly increase the model fit, and therefore the variation explained by the respective variable (sociality or rank).

Statistical Analyses.

We conducted all statistical analyses in R version 3.1.2 (47). We used the function “t.test” to conduct t tests, and the function “wilcox.test” to conduct a Wilcoxon signed rank test. We used the lme4 package version 1.1-11 (48) to calculate LMMs (function “lmer”) and GLMMs (function “glmer”), with respective response and test variables. All model procedures and the establishment of P values are described in detail in SI Materials and Methods, Method S4: Model Procedures. To conduct pairwise comparisons of DSI values between different kin categories, we used the function “lsmeans” from the package lsmeans version 2.20-23 (49) with the “Tukey” method to adjust for multiple testing. We used the function “anova” with the argument “test = ‘chisq’” to conduct χ2 tests for the comparison of (nested) models.

SI Materials and Methods

Method S1: Calculation of DSI.

We calculated DSI for all coresident dyads annually by applying the formula DSIxy=(∑i=1nfixy/fi)¯/n, where fixy is the rate of behavior i for dyad xy (30). Only focal observations while x and y were both adult and members of the same group were considered (i.e., rates were corrected for coresidence time). The variable fi¯ is the mean rate of behavior i across all dyads, and n is the number of behaviors that contribute to the index. Three different behaviors (n = 3) were considered in the analyses: (i) grooming, (ii) approaching within short distance, and (iii) approaching within long distance. In 2005 and 2006, approaching within physical contact was considered as a close approach and approaching within 2 m was considered as a long-distance approach. In the following years, close approaches were defined as approaching within one body length (∼0.5 m), whereas long-distance approaches were defined as approaching within five body lengths (∼2.5 m). This difference should not result in any bias because the DSI is a relative index that was calculated annually, and for each given year, both long and close approaches only had one single definition.

Method S2: Calculation of Average DSI Values for Each Partner Rank.

For each year and each female, we ranked all potential partners according to bond strength (DSI). For each female and each DSI rank (first rank, second rank, etc.), we calculated the average bond strength over all years during which the female was present. For example, with her top partner (which could be a different female each year), the female Baloo had a DSI of 4.510 in 2009, 1.244 in 2010, and 2.602 in 2011, resulting in an average DSI value for her top partner of 2.740. We did the same for the second-ranked partner, third-ranked partner, etc.

Method S3: Calculation of the PSI and Simulated PSI Values.

The original PSI (45) is defined as PSI=(M−U)/(M−S), where M is the maximum number of top partners, S is the number of rank slots being evaluated (i.e., number of top partners), and U is the observed number of unique partners (within the S rank slots). In the original paper by Silk et al. (45), M was calculated as n * S, where n = number of years for which the rank orders were evaluated for a specific female. However, the maximum number of top partners might be smaller than n * S if a female is observed for a long time or the group only contains a few females. For example, if a female was observed for 7 y and eight different females were coresident during that time, the maximum number of different top partners would be eight and not n * S = 7 * 3 = 21. Thus, M was corrected for this issue in the current study.

We calculated the simulated random PSI values by picking three top partners randomly from the pool of potential partners for each female for each year the female was present, and then calculated the PSI using these top partners. We iterated this procedure 10,000 times and created a frequency table of different PSI values for each female that showed how often they would occur by chance (table 3 of ref. 45 as an example). We then used the frequency table to calculate an average simulated PSI for each female.

Method S4: Model Procedures.

Before running the models, we scaled all numerical predictor variables to a mean of 0 and SD of 1. We included female and dyad identity in the LMMs testing for predictors of bond strength (DSI) and only female identity in the LMMs testing for predictors for ASI and in the GLMMs testing for infant survival. Furthermore, we included all necessary random slopes. All models were visually inspected for normal distribution and homogeneity of residuals and tested for model stability and multicollinearity of predictor variables. Binomial models were checked for overdispersion.

To establish the significance of full models, we compared them with null models (not containing the test variables but containing all other variables) using likelihood ratio tests (function “anova” with the argument “test=‘Chisq’”). Likelihood ratio tests were also used to obtain P values for single predictor variables using the function “drop1” (again with the argument “test=‘Chisq’”). To achieve more reliable P values, we fitted LMMs using maximum likelihood (rather than restricted maximum likelihood; ref. 50).

Acknowledgments

We thank the Costa Rican National Park Service and Administrators of the Área de Conservación Guanacaste (especially R. Blanco Segura) for permission to conduct research in the park since 1983. We thank Dr. J. Addicott, who designed and developed the Primate Adaptations to Changing Environments (PACE) database that made these analyses possible, and we thank the many students and field assistants who collected life-history data over the years, particularly S. Cheves Hernandez and M. Myers. We also thank Ryne Palombit and three anonymous reviewers for their valuable comments. This project was funded by the Eyes High Postdoctoral program of the University of Calgary (U.K.); the Leakey Foundation (M.L.B., K.M.J., and F.A.C.); Alberta Innovates (M.L.B., S.D.C., and F.A.C.), the Animal Behaviour Society (M.L.B.), the International Primatological Society and Sigma Xi (M.L.B. and S.D.C.); Japan Society for the Promotion of Science Grants JSPS 15H2421 (to S.K.) and JSPS 24-02739 (to E.C.W.); the American Society of Primatologists (F.A.C.); the Louisiana Board of Regents, Nacy Maggioncalda Foundation, National Geographic Society, Newcomb Institute, and the Stone Center for Latin American Studies at Tulane University (K.M.J.); the Natural Sciences and Engineering Research Council of Canada (S.D.C. and L.M.F.); and the Canada Research Chairs Program (L.M.F.).

Footnotes

  • ↵1To whom correspondence should be addressed. Email: urs.kalbitzer{at}ucalgary.ca.
  • Author contributions: U.K. and L.M.F. designed research; M.L.B. and S.D.C. contributed behavioral data; E.C.W. and S.K. analyzed and contributed genetic data; F.A.C. and K.M.J. analyzed life-history data; U.K. conducted statistical analyses; and U.K., M.L.B., S.D.C., E.C.W., S.K., F.A.C., K.M.J., and L.M.F. wrote the paper.

  • The authors declare no conflict of interest.

  • This article is a PNAS Direct Submission. R.P. is a Guest Editor invited by the Editorial Board.

  • Data deposition: The data reported in this paper have been deposited in the Dryad database at dx.doi.org/10.5061/dryad.93463.

  • This article contains supporting information online at www.pnas.org/lookup/suppl/doi:10.1073/pnas.1608625114/-/DCSupplemental.

References

  1. ↵
    1. Seyfarth RM,
    2. Silk JB,
    3. Cheney DL
    (2012) Variation in personality and fitness in wild female baboons. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 109(42):16980–16985.
    .
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  2. ↵
    1. Silk JB, et al.
    (2010) Strong and consistent social bonds enhance the longevity of female baboons. Curr Biol 20(15):1359–1361.
    .
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  3. ↵
    1. Archie EA,
    2. Tung J,
    3. Clark M,
    4. Altmann J,
    5. Alberts SC
    (2014) Social affiliation matters: Both same-sex and opposite-sex relationships predict survival in wild female baboons. Proc R Soc B Biol Sci 281(1793):20141261.
    .
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  4. ↵
    1. McFarland R,
    2. Majolo B
    (2013) Coping with the cold: Predictors of survival in wild Barbary macaques, Macaca sylvanus. Biol Lett 9(4):20130428.
    .
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  5. ↵
    1. Lehmann J,
    2. Majolo B,
    3. McFarland R
    (2016) The effects of social network position on the survival of wild Barbary macaques, Macaca sylvanus. Behav Ecol 27(1):20–28.
    .
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  6. ↵
    1. Schülke O,
    2. Bhagavatula J,
    3. Vigilant L,
    4. Ostner J
    (2010) Social bonds enhance reproductive success in male macaques. Curr Biol 20(24):2207–2210.
    .
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  7. ↵
    1. Silk JB,
    2. Alberts SC,
    3. Altmann J
    (2003) Social bonds of female baboons enhance infant survival. Science 302(5648):1231–1234.
    .
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  8. ↵
    1. Silk JB, et al.
    (2009) The benefits of social capital: Close social bonds among female baboons enhance offspring survival. Proc R Soc B Biol Sci 276(1670):3099.
    .
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  9. ↵
    1. Frère CH, et al.
    (2010) Social and genetic interactions drive fitness variation in a free-living dolphin population. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 107(46):19949–19954.
    .
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  10. ↵
    1. Cameron EZ,
    2. Setsaas TH,
    3. Linklater WL
    (2009) Social bonds between unrelated females increase reproductive success in feral horses. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 106(33):13850–13853.
    .
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  11. ↵
    1. Campbell BG
    1. Trivers R
    (1972) Parental investment and sexual selection. Sexual Selection and the Descent of Man, ed Campbell BG (Heinemann, London), pp 136–179.
    .
  12. ↵
    1. Blum MS,
    2. Blum NA
    1. Parker GA
    (1979) Sexual selection and sexual conflict. Sexual Selection and Reproductive Competition in Insects, eds Blum MS, Blum NA (Academic, New York), pp 123–166.
    .
  13. ↵
    1. Hrdy SB
    (1974) Male-male competition and infanticide among the langurs (Presbytis entellus) of Abu, Rajasthan. Folia Primatol (Basel) 22(1):19–58.
    .
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  14. ↵
    1. Lukas D,
    2. Huchard E
    (2014) Sexual conflict. The evolution of infanticide by males in mammalian societies. Science 346(6211):841–844.
    .
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  15. ↵
    1. Opie C,
    2. Atkinson QD,
    3. Dunbar RIM,
    4. Shultz S
    (2013) Male infanticide leads to social monogamy in primates. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 110(33):13328–13332.
    .
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  16. ↵
    1. Mitani JC,
    2. Call J,
    3. Kappeler PM,
    4. Palombit RA,
    5. Silk JB
    1. Palombit RA
    (2012) Infanticide: Male strategies and female counterstrategies. The Evolution of Primate Societies, eds Mitani JC, Call J, Kappeler PM, Palombit RA, Silk JB (Univ of Chicago Press, Chicago), pp 432–468.
    .
  17. ↵
    1. Fragaszy DM,
    2. Visalberghi E,
    3. Fedigan LM
    (2004) The Complete Capuchin: The Biology of the Genus Cebus (Cambridge Univ Press, Cambridge, UK), 1st Ed.
    .
  18. ↵
    1. Perry S
    (2012) The behavior of wild white-faced capuchins. Adv Stud Behav 44:135–181.
    .
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  19. ↵
    1. Perry S
    (1996) Female-female social relationships in wild white-faced capuchin monkeys, Cebus capucinus. Am J Primatol 40(2):167–182.
    .
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  20. ↵
    1. Jack KM,
    2. Fedigan L
    (2004) Male dispersal patterns in white-faced capuchins, Cebus capucinus: Part 1: Patterns and causes of natal emigration. Anim Behav 67(4):761–769.
    .
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  21. ↵
    1. Jack KM,
    2. Fedigan L
    (2004) Male dispersal patterns in white-faced capuchins, Cebus capucinus: Part 2: Patterns and causes of secondary dispersal. Anim Behav 67(4):771–782.
    .
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  22. ↵
    1. Fedigan LM
    (2003) Impact of male takeovers on infant deaths, births and conceptions in Cebus capucinus at Santa Rosa, Costa Rica. Int J Primatol 24(4):723–741.
    .
    OpenUrl
  23. ↵
    1. Fedigan LM,
    2. Carnegie SD,
    3. Jack KM
    (2008) Predictors of reproductive success in female white-faced capuchins (Cebus capucinus). Am J Phys Anthropol 137(1):82–90.
    .
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  24. ↵
    1. Jack KM,
    2. Fedigan LM
    (2016) Infant mortality in white-faced capuchins: The impact of alpha male replacements. Am J Phys Anthropol 159(S62):183.
    .
    OpenUrl
  25. ↵
    1. Hall CL,
    2. Fedigan LM
    (1997) Spatial benefits afforded by high rank in white-faced capuchins. Anim Behav 53(5):1069–1082.
    .
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  26. ↵
    1. Fisher ML,
    2. Garcia JR
    1. Fedigan LM,
    2. Jack KM
    (2013) Sexual conflict in white-faced capuchins. Evolution’s Empress, eds Fisher ML, Garcia JR (Oxford Univ Press, New York), pp 281–303.
    .
  27. ↵
    1. Archie EA,
    2. Moss CJ,
    3. Alberts SC
    (2006) The ties that bind: Genetic relatedness predicts the fission and fusion of social groups in wild African elephants. Proc Biol Sci 273(1586):513–522.
    .
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  28. ↵
    1. Frère CH, et al.
    (2010) Home range overlap, matrilineal and biparental kinship drive female associations in bottlenose dolphins. Anim Behav 80(3):481–486.
    .
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  29. ↵
    1. Silk JB,
    2. Alberts SC,
    3. Altmann J
    (2006) Social relationships among adult female baboons (Papio cynocephalus) II. Variation in the quality and stability of social bonds. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 61(2):197–204.
    .
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  30. ↵
    1. Silk JB,
    2. Altmann J,
    3. Alberts SC
    (2006) Social relationships among adult female baboons (Papio cynocephalus) I. Variation in the strength of social bonds. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 61(2):183–195.
    .
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  31. ↵
    1. Perry S,
    2. Manson JH,
    3. Muniz L,
    4. Gros-Louis J,
    5. Vigilant L
    (2008) Kin-biased social behaviour in wild adult female white-faced capuchins, Cebus capucinus. Anim Behav 76(1):187–199.
    .
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  32. ↵
    1. Bădescu I,
    2. Sicotte P,
    3. Ting N,
    4. Wikberg EC
    (2015) Female parity, maternal kinship, infant age and sex influence natal attraction and infant handling in a wild colobine (Colobus vellerosus). Am J Primatol 77(4):376–387.
    .
    OpenUrl
  33. ↵
    1. Manson JH,
    2. Rose LM,
    3. Perry S,
    4. Gros-Louis J
    (1999) Dynamics of female–female relationships in wild Cebus capucinus: Data from two Costa Rican sites. Int J Primatol 20(5):679–706.
    .
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  34. ↵
    1. Sargeant EJ,
    2. Wikberg EC,
    3. Kawamura S,
    4. Jack KM,
    5. Fedigan LM
    (2016) Paternal kin recognition and infant care in white-faced capuchins (Cebus capucinus). Am J Primatol 78(6):659–668.
    .
    OpenUrl
  35. ↵
    1. Sheller CR,
    2. King Z,
    3. Jack K
    (2009) The effects of infant births on male-female relationships in Cebus capucinus. Am J Primatol 71(5):380–383.
    .
    OpenUrlPubMed
  36. ↵
    1. Sargeant EJ,
    2. Wikberg EC,
    3. Kawamura S,
    4. Fedigan LM
    (2015) Allonursing in white-faced capuchins (Cebus capucinus) provides evidence for cooperative care of infants. Behaviour 152(12–13):1841–1869.
    .
    OpenUrl
  37. ↵
    1. Ellis L
    (1995) Dominance and reproductive success among nonhuman animals: A cross-species comparison. Ethol Sociobiol 16(4):257–333.
    .
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  38. ↵
    1. Majolo B,
    2. Lehmann J,
    3. de Bortoli Vizioli A,
    4. Schino G
    (2012) Fitness-related benefits of dominance in primates. Am J Phys Anthropol 147(4):652–660.
    .
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  39. ↵
    1. Kappeler PM,
    2. Watts DP
    1. Fedigan LM,
    2. Jack KM
    (2012) Tracking neotropical monkeys in Santa Rosa: Lessons from a regenerating Costa Rican dry forest. Long-Term Field Studies of Primates, eds Kappeler PM, Watts DP (Springer, Berlin), pp 165–184.
    .
  40. ↵
    1. Cheney DL, et al.
    (2004) Factors affecting reproduction and mortality among baboons in the Okavango Delta, Botswana. Int J Primatol 25(2):401–428.
    .
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  41. ↵
    1. Bergstrom ML,
    2. Fedigan LM
    (2010) Dominance among female white-faced capuchin monkeys (Cebus capucinus): Hierarchical linearity, nepotism, strength and stability. Behaviour 147(7):899–931.
    .
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  42. ↵
    1. Carnegie SD,
    2. Fedigan LM,
    3. Ziegler TE
    (2011) Social and environmental factors affecting fecal glucocorticoids in wild, female white-faced capuchins (Cebus capucinus). Am J Primatol 73(9):861–869.
    .
    OpenUrlPubMed
  43. ↵
    1. Wikberg EC, et al.
    (2014) The effect of male parallel dispersal on the kin composition of groups in white-faced capuchins. Anim Behav 96:9–17.
    .
    OpenUrl
  44. ↵
    1. Van Horn RC,
    2. Altmann J,
    3. Alberts SC
    (2008) Can’t get there from here: Inferring kinship from pairwise genetic relatedness. Anim Behav 75(3):1173–1180.
    .
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  45. ↵
    1. Silk JB,
    2. Alberts SC,
    3. Altmann J,
    4. Cheney DL,
    5. Seyfarth RM
    (2012) Stability of partner choice among female baboons. Anim Behav 83(6):1511–1518.
    .
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  46. ↵
    1. Carnegie SD,
    2. Fedigan LM,
    3. Melin AD
    (2011) Reproductive seasonality in female capuchins (Cebus capucinus) in Santa Rosa (Area de Conservación Guanacaste), Costa Rica. Int J Primatol 32(5):1076–1090.
    .
    OpenUrl
  47. ↵
    1. R Core Team
    (2014) R: A Language and Environment for Statistical Computing (R Foundation for Statistical Computing, Vienna). Available at www.R-project.org/. Accessed January 27, 2015.
    .
  48. ↵
    1. Bates D,
    2. Mächler M,
    3. Bolker B,
    4. Walker S
    (2015) Fitting linear mixed-effects models using lme4. J Stat Softw 67(1):1–48.
    .
    OpenUrl
  49. ↵
    1. Lenth R
    (2015) lsmeans: Least-Squares Means. Available at: cran.r-project.org/packages/lsmeans/index.html. Accessed April 14, 2016.
    .
  50. ↵
    1. Bolker BM, et al.
    (2009) Generalized linear mixed models: A practical guide for ecology and evolution. Trends Ecol Evol 24(3):127–135.
    .
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
PreviousNext
Back to top
Article Alerts
Email Article

Thank you for your interest in spreading the word on PNAS.

NOTE: We only request your email address so that the person you are recommending the page to knows that you wanted them to see it, and that it is not junk mail. We do not capture any email address.

Enter multiple addresses on separate lines or separate them with commas.
Female sociality and sexual conflict shape offspring survival in a Neotropical primate
(Your Name) has sent you a message from PNAS
(Your Name) thought you would like to see the PNAS web site.
CAPTCHA
This question is for testing whether or not you are a human visitor and to prevent automated spam submissions.
Citation Tools
Female sociality and infant survival in capuchins
Urs Kalbitzer, Mackenzie L. Bergstrom, Sarah D. Carnegie, Eva C. Wikberg, Shoji Kawamura, Fernando A. Campos, Katharine M. Jack, Linda M. Fedigan
Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences Feb 2017, 114 (8) 1892-1897; DOI: 10.1073/pnas.1608625114

Citation Manager Formats

  • BibTeX
  • Bookends
  • EasyBib
  • EndNote (tagged)
  • EndNote 8 (xml)
  • Medlars
  • Mendeley
  • Papers
  • RefWorks Tagged
  • Ref Manager
  • RIS
  • Zotero
Request Permissions
Share
Female sociality and infant survival in capuchins
Urs Kalbitzer, Mackenzie L. Bergstrom, Sarah D. Carnegie, Eva C. Wikberg, Shoji Kawamura, Fernando A. Campos, Katharine M. Jack, Linda M. Fedigan
Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences Feb 2017, 114 (8) 1892-1897; DOI: 10.1073/pnas.1608625114
del.icio.us logo Digg logo Reddit logo Twitter logo CiteULike logo Facebook logo Google logo Mendeley logo
  • Tweet Widget
  • Facebook Like
  • Mendeley logo Mendeley

Article Classifications

  • Biological Sciences
  • Anthropology
Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences: 114 (8)
Table of Contents

Submit

Sign up for Article Alerts

Jump to section

  • Article
    • Abstract
    • Results
    • Discussion
    • Materials and Methods
    • SI Materials and Methods
    • Acknowledgments
    • Footnotes
    • References
  • Figures & SI
  • Info & Metrics
  • PDF

You May Also be Interested in

Water from a faucet fills a glass.
News Feature: How “forever chemicals” might impair the immune system
Researchers are exploring whether these ubiquitous fluorinated molecules might worsen infections or hamper vaccine effectiveness.
Image credit: Shutterstock/Dmitry Naumov.
Reflection of clouds in the still waters of Mono Lake in California.
Inner Workings: Making headway with the mysteries of life’s origins
Recent experiments and simulations are starting to answer some fundamental questions about how life came to be.
Image credit: Shutterstock/Radoslaw Lecyk.
Cave in coastal Kenya with tree growing in the middle.
Journal Club: Small, sharp blades mark shift from Middle to Later Stone Age in coastal Kenya
Archaeologists have long tried to define the transition between the two time periods.
Image credit: Ceri Shipton.
Mouse fibroblast cells. Electron bifurcation reactions keep mammalian cells alive.
Exploring electron bifurcation
Jonathon Yuly, David Beratan, and Peng Zhang investigate how electron bifurcation reactions work.
Listen
Past PodcastsSubscribe
Panda bear hanging in a tree
How horse manure helps giant pandas tolerate cold
A study finds that giant pandas roll in horse manure to increase their cold tolerance.
Image credit: Fuwen Wei.

Similar Articles

Site Logo
Powered by HighWire
  • Submit Manuscript
  • Twitter
  • Facebook
  • RSS Feeds
  • Email Alerts

Articles

  • Current Issue
  • Special Feature Articles – Most Recent
  • List of Issues

PNAS Portals

  • Anthropology
  • Chemistry
  • Classics
  • Front Matter
  • Physics
  • Sustainability Science
  • Teaching Resources

Information

  • Authors
  • Editorial Board
  • Reviewers
  • Subscribers
  • Librarians
  • Press
  • Cozzarelli Prize
  • Site Map
  • PNAS Updates
  • FAQs
  • Accessibility Statement
  • Rights & Permissions
  • About
  • Contact

Feedback    Privacy/Legal

Copyright © 2021 National Academy of Sciences. Online ISSN 1091-6490