Skip to main content

Main menu

  • Home
  • Articles
    • Current
    • Special Feature Articles - Most Recent
    • Special Features
    • Colloquia
    • Collected Articles
    • PNAS Classics
    • List of Issues
  • Front Matter
    • Front Matter Portal
    • Journal Club
  • News
    • For the Press
    • This Week In PNAS
    • PNAS in the News
  • Podcasts
  • Authors
    • Information for Authors
    • Editorial and Journal Policies
    • Submission Procedures
    • Fees and Licenses
  • Submit
  • Submit
  • About
    • Editorial Board
    • PNAS Staff
    • FAQ
    • Accessibility Statement
    • Rights and Permissions
    • Site Map
  • Contact
  • Journal Club
  • Subscribe
    • Subscription Rates
    • Subscriptions FAQ
    • Open Access
    • Recommend PNAS to Your Librarian

User menu

  • Log in
  • My Cart

Search

  • Advanced search
Home
Home
  • Log in
  • My Cart

Advanced Search

  • Home
  • Articles
    • Current
    • Special Feature Articles - Most Recent
    • Special Features
    • Colloquia
    • Collected Articles
    • PNAS Classics
    • List of Issues
  • Front Matter
    • Front Matter Portal
    • Journal Club
  • News
    • For the Press
    • This Week In PNAS
    • PNAS in the News
  • Podcasts
  • Authors
    • Information for Authors
    • Editorial and Journal Policies
    • Submission Procedures
    • Fees and Licenses
  • Submit
Research Article

Arc/Arg3.1 mediates a critical period for spatial learning and hippocampal networks

View ORCID ProfileXiaoyan Gao, View ORCID ProfileSergio Castro-Gomez, Jasper Grendel, Sabine Graf, Ute Süsens, Lars Binkle, Daniel Mensching, Dirk Isbrandt, View ORCID ProfileDietmar Kuhl, and View ORCID ProfileOra Ohana
  1. aInstitute for Molecular and Cellular Cognition, Center for Molecular Neurobiology Hamburg, University Medical Center Hamburg–Eppendorf, 20251 Hamburg, Germany;
  2. bExperimental Neuropediatrics, Center for Molecular Neurobiology Hamburg, University Medical Center Hamburg–Eppendorf, 20251 Hamburg, Germany;
  3. cInstitute for Molecular and Behavioral Neuroscience, German Center for Neurodegenerative Diseases and University of Cologne Research Team Experimental Neurophysiology, University of Cologne, 50937 Cologne, Germany;
  4. dGerman Center for Neurodegenerative Diseases, 53175 Bonn, Germany

See allHide authors and affiliations

PNAS December 4, 2018 115 (49) 12531-12536; first published November 15, 2018; https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1810125115
Xiaoyan Gao
aInstitute for Molecular and Cellular Cognition, Center for Molecular Neurobiology Hamburg, University Medical Center Hamburg–Eppendorf, 20251 Hamburg, Germany;
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
  • ORCID record for Xiaoyan Gao
Sergio Castro-Gomez
aInstitute for Molecular and Cellular Cognition, Center for Molecular Neurobiology Hamburg, University Medical Center Hamburg–Eppendorf, 20251 Hamburg, Germany;
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
  • ORCID record for Sergio Castro-Gomez
Jasper Grendel
aInstitute for Molecular and Cellular Cognition, Center for Molecular Neurobiology Hamburg, University Medical Center Hamburg–Eppendorf, 20251 Hamburg, Germany;
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Sabine Graf
aInstitute for Molecular and Cellular Cognition, Center for Molecular Neurobiology Hamburg, University Medical Center Hamburg–Eppendorf, 20251 Hamburg, Germany;
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Ute Süsens
aInstitute for Molecular and Cellular Cognition, Center for Molecular Neurobiology Hamburg, University Medical Center Hamburg–Eppendorf, 20251 Hamburg, Germany;
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Lars Binkle
aInstitute for Molecular and Cellular Cognition, Center for Molecular Neurobiology Hamburg, University Medical Center Hamburg–Eppendorf, 20251 Hamburg, Germany;
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Daniel Mensching
aInstitute for Molecular and Cellular Cognition, Center for Molecular Neurobiology Hamburg, University Medical Center Hamburg–Eppendorf, 20251 Hamburg, Germany;
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Dirk Isbrandt
bExperimental Neuropediatrics, Center for Molecular Neurobiology Hamburg, University Medical Center Hamburg–Eppendorf, 20251 Hamburg, Germany;
cInstitute for Molecular and Behavioral Neuroscience, German Center for Neurodegenerative Diseases and University of Cologne Research Team Experimental Neurophysiology, University of Cologne, 50937 Cologne, Germany;
dGerman Center for Neurodegenerative Diseases, 53175 Bonn, Germany
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Dietmar Kuhl
aInstitute for Molecular and Cellular Cognition, Center for Molecular Neurobiology Hamburg, University Medical Center Hamburg–Eppendorf, 20251 Hamburg, Germany;
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
  • ORCID record for Dietmar Kuhl
  • For correspondence: kuhl@zmnh.uni-hamburg.de ora.ohana@zmnh.uni-hamburg.de
Ora Ohana
aInstitute for Molecular and Cellular Cognition, Center for Molecular Neurobiology Hamburg, University Medical Center Hamburg–Eppendorf, 20251 Hamburg, Germany;
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
  • ORCID record for Ora Ohana
  • For correspondence: kuhl@zmnh.uni-hamburg.de ora.ohana@zmnh.uni-hamburg.de
  1. Edited by Richard L. Huganir, The Johns Hopkins University School of Medicine, Baltimore, MD, and approved October 19, 2018 (received for review June 14, 2018)

  • Article
  • Figures & SI
  • Info & Metrics
  • PDF
Loading

Significance

Spatial learning and memory are hippocampal functions that emerge and mature during early postnatal development. The molecular mechanisms which shape this process are largely unknown. Here, we present evidence that the activity-regulated gene Arc/Arg3.1 is transiently up-regulated in the hippocampus of neonatal mice where it is required for establishing appropriate hippocampal network activity essential for spatial learning. Once established, network activity supports normal spatial learning in the absence of Arc/Arg3.1 while long-term memory storage continues to rely on Arc/Arg3.1 expression throughout life. These results demonstrate that hippocampal networks undergo a critical period of development mediated by Arc/Arg3.1 and open opportunities to investigate normal and pathological neurodevelopment of higher brain functions.

Abstract

During early postnatal development, sensory regions of the brain undergo periods of heightened plasticity which sculpt neural networks and lay the foundation for adult sensory perception. Such critical periods were also postulated for learning and memory but remain elusive and poorly understood. Here, we present evidence that the activity-regulated and memory-linked gene Arc/Arg3.1 is transiently up-regulated in the hippocampus during the first postnatal month. Conditional removal of Arc/Arg3.1 during this period permanently alters hippocampal oscillations and diminishes spatial learning capacity throughout adulthood. In contrast, post developmental removal of Arc/Arg3.1 leaves learning and network activity patterns intact. Long-term memory storage continues to rely on Arc/Arg3.1 expression throughout life. These results demonstrate that Arc/Arg3.1 mediates a critical period for spatial learning, during which Arc/Arg3.1 fosters maturation of hippocampal network activity necessary for future learning and memory storage.

  • Arc/Arg3.1
  • hippocampal oscillations
  • critical period
  • spatial learning
  • memory

During early postnatal development sensory regions of the brain undergo periods of heightened plasticity which sculpt and tune neural networks for adult sensory perception (1⇓–3). It has long been debated whether critical periods exist for complex behaviors, such as orientation and learning and memory. Recent evidence unraveled a critical period for processing of emotional memories which depends on activity and plasticity mechanisms within the developing hippocampus (4, 5). These findings raise the question whether other hippocampal functions are subject to critical periods. Also, what consequences do these critical periods have for hippocampal network activity?

The hippocampus is essential for encoding spatial information and memories (6, 7), which might be achieved through linked neural representations of space and time (8⇓–10). Hippocampal network oscillations in the theta, gamma, and ripple frequency bands are believed to play a central role in the generation and coordination of these representations, and their disruption diminishes spatial learning and memory retrieval (11⇓⇓⇓⇓–16). Hippocampal oscillatory activity emerges and develops within the first three postnatal weeks in rodents (17⇓⇓–20) whereupon the abilities to perform spatial learning and to store memories arise between postnatal day 20 (P20) and P24 (21⇓⇓⇓⇓⇓–27). During this premnemonic period, hippocampal networks are built through neuronal growth and synaptogenesis, which provide the framework of the oscillatory activity (28, 29). Activity- and plasticity-dependent mechanisms shape network connectivity, but how they affect oscillatory activity and whether this has any consequences for spatial cognition are insufficiently understood.

Here, we addressed the role of the activity-regulated gene Arc/Arg3.1 in the development and maturation of spatial learning, explicit memory, and hippocampal network activity. Arc/Arg3.1 expression is rapidly induced by plasticity-producing stimuli (30⇓⇓–33) and following memory acquisition and retrieval (34). We previously demonstrated a profound loss of long-term memory and impaired synaptic plasticity in conventional Arc/Arg3.1 knockout (KO) mice, despite intact sensory perception and short-term memory, and normal brain morphology, neural activity, and synaptic transmission (35). These properties make Arc/Arg3.1 a particularly suited target for specific modulations of activity-dependent plasticity, distinct from neurogenesis and synaptogenesis.

Results

Deletion of Arc/Arg3.1 Before but Not After P21 Impairs Spatial Learning in Adult Mice.

We used radioactive in situ hybridization (rISH) to examine natural Arc/Arg3.1 expression in the hippocampi of WT mice. Arc/Arg3.1 mRNA was detectable from P7 onwards, increased dramatically between P14 and P21, and subsequently decreased to a low baseline level in adulthood (Fig. 1A). This spontaneous up-regulation of Arc/Arg3.1, during a period in which mice do not yet store long-term memories, suggests an additional, novel role for Arc/Arg3.1 during the development of mnemonic circuitry. To investigate this role, we generated mice with a floxed Arc/Arg3.1 allele in which the open reading frame was flanked by LoxP sites (Arc/Arg3.1f/f) (SI Appendix, Fig. S1A). Brain morphology, Arc/Arg3.1 expression, locomotor and exploratory behavior, pain sensitivity, and memory acquisition and persistence of the Arc/Arg3.1f/f mice were indistinguishable from those of WT littermates (SI Appendix, Fig. S1 B–G). We bred the Arc/Arg3.1f/f mice with two different CaMKIIα-Cre mice to generate either “Early-cKO” or “Late-cKO” mice, in which Arc/Arg3.1 was irreversibly removed after P7 or after P21, respectively (Fig. 1A and SI Appendix, Fig. S2A). We compared these to constitutive KO mice in which Arc/Arg3.1 was absent from the germline. Using immunostaining, immunoblotting, and rISH (SI Appendix, Fig. S2 B–D), we verified that near complete removal of Arc/Arg3.1 in adult forebrains was achieved in all mice.

Fig. 1.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Fig. 1.

Spatial learning depends on Arc/Arg3.1 expression during early postnatal development. (A) Spontaneous Arc/Arg3.1 up-regulation between P7 and P36 in WT mice; peak expression in the hippocampus is evident around P21. Generation of Arc/Arg3.1 KO (red), Early-cKO (blue), and Late-cKO (green) mice through terminal Cre-recombination in the germline (KO), starting after P7 and completed before P14 (Early-cKO), and after P21 and before P36 (Late-cKO). Coronal sections were subjected to rISH with an Arc/Arg3.1 probe. (B) Categories of search strategies in the MWM. Purple indicates nonspatial, yellow imprecise spatial, and magenta precise spatial strategies. (C, E, and G) Escape latencies to the hidden platform were scored for WT, KO, and cKO during the training days of the MWM. (D, F, and H) Search strategies during the last 4 d of training were analyzed in WT, KO, and cKO mice. (C) Significantly longer escape latencies in KO mice (genotype, F1,22 = 6.066, #P < 0.05; Bonferroni post hoc, *P < 0.05, **P < 0.01; n = 12, 12 mice). (D) KO mice employed more nonspatial and fewer spatial search strategies, χ2(2) = 14.435, P < 0.001. (E) Significantly longer escape latencies in Early-cKO mice (genotype, F1,29 = 5.507, #P < 0.05; genotype x day, F8,232 = 2.594, P = 0.010, Bonferroni post hoc, *P < 0.05, **P < 0.01; n = 17, 14 mice). (F) Early-cKO mice employed more imprecise nonspatial, and fewer precise spatial, searching strategies, χ2(2) = 17.219, P < 0.001. (G) Comparable escape latencies in Late-cKO mice (genotype and genotype x day, both P > 0.05; n = 12, 11 mice). (H) WT-control and Late-cKO mice adopted similar search strategies, χ2(2) = 1.115, P > 0.05. The color code of search categories is as in B. Data shown are mean ± SEM. Two-Way ANOVA with repeated measures was performed in C, E, and G. A χ2 independence test was applied in D, F, and H.

We next examined the capacity of adult Arc/Arg3.1-deficient mice for spatial learning in the Morris water maze (MWM) (6, 36). During daily interspaced training sessions, mice searched for a hidden escape platform in the pool indicated by surrounding visual cues. As training proceeded, mice learned to locate the platform efficiently by progressively adopting spatially precise navigation strategies (Fig. 1B), thereby achieving shorter escape latencies, a process known as “incremental space learning” (37). We assessed this learning progression by performing an algorithm-based analysis of search strategies (SI Appendix, Fig. S3). As expected, WT and WT-control mice gradually reduced their escape latencies (Fig. 1 C, E, and G) and progressively increased the use of precise spatial strategies (SI Appendix, Fig. S4 B–D). In contrast, KO mice exhibited longer escape latencies and improved to a lesser extent and at a slower rate compared with their WT littermates (Fig. 1C). Strikingly, KO mice employed significantly different search strategies and adhered to nonspatial strategies even at training completion (Fig. 1D and SI Appendix, Fig. S4B). Similarly, Early-cKO mice exhibited a retarded learning curve, indicated by significantly longer escape latencies (Fig. 1E) and predominant use of non-spatial search strategies (Fig. 1F and SI Appendix, Fig. S4C). By strong contrast, Late-cKO mice learned to locate the platform as accurately and efficiently as their WT littermates, as indicated by the similarity of the escape latency curves (Fig. 1G) and by their ability to adopt precise spatial navigation strategies that were indistinguishable from those used by their WT littermates (Fig. 1H and SI Appendix, Fig. S4D). All KO and cKO mice exhibited low levels of thigmotaxis by the end of training (SI Appendix, Fig. S4 B–D) and performed the cued version of the MWM as well as their WT littermates (SI Appendix, Fig. S5 A–D), excluding motivational or motoric differences between them. Taken together, these data demonstrate that the KO and Early-cKO mice were impaired in spatial learning but had no deficit in nonspatial cued navigation. In contrast, Late-cKO mice exhibited both intact spatial learning and cued navigation.

Deletions of Arc/Arg3.1 Before and After P21 Impair Long-Term Memory in Adult Mice.

To investigate the ability of the mice to consolidate long-term spatial memory, we performed a probe trial 1 wk after training completion. WT and WT-controls retained a stable and precise spatial memory, evident from their targeted swimming toward the platform (Fig. 2 A, D, and G), persistent search within the target zone (Fig. 2 B, E, and H), and the significantly higher number of target-annulus crossings (Fig. 2 C, F, and I). In contrast, KO, Early-cKO, and Late-cKO mice failed to exhibit any preference for the platform zone (Fig. 2 B, E, and H) or target-annulus (Fig. 2 C, F, and I), disclosing impaired long-term spatial memory. KO and cKO mice remained at the release locus of the pool (in the opposite quadrant), thereby exhibiting an abnormal preference for this quadrant (O in Fig. 2 A–H). To examine the mnemonic capacity of the mice in a different hippocampus-dependent task, we subjected new groups of KO and cKO mice to contextual fear conditioning (CFC) (Fig. 2 J–L). Despite successful conditioning (SI Appendix, Fig. S5 E–H), KO, Early-cKO, and Late-cKO mice failed to remember the conditioning context, in contrast to their respective WT littermates, which maintained a strong and stable memory, as indicated by high levels of freezing (Fig. 2 J–L). Thus, consolidation of the long-term memory was dependent on expression of Arc/Arg3.1 in adulthood even when the learning ability was properly established during development.

Fig. 2.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Fig. 2.

Arc/Arg3.1 expression is required for the consolidation of long-term spatial and contextual memories throughout life. (A–I) Long-term memory in the MWM test, assessed during a probe trial 7 d after training completion. (A, D, and G) Mean occupancy density plots per group. (Scale bar in A applies to D and G.) (B–I) Percent time spent in the defined zones and number of annulus crossings were scored in WT, KO, and cKO. Axis labeling refers to zones as follows: L, left; O, opposite; R, right; T, target. (A) Off-target occupancy plot of KO mice. (B) KO mice demonstrated no preference for the target zone (genotype x quadrant, F3,66 = 5.573, P < 0.01; WT-T vs. KO-T, **P < 0.01; n = 12, 12). (C) Reduced target-annulus crossings in KO mice (genotype x quadrant, F3,66 = 4.368, P < 0.01; WT-T vs. KO-T, ***P < 0.001; n = 12, 12). (D) Off-target occupancy plot of Early-cKO mice. (E) Loss of preference for the target zone in Early-cKO mice (genotype x quadrant, F3,87 = 4.093, P < 0.01; WT-control-T vs. Early-cKO-T, *P < 0.05; n = 17, 14). (F) Significantly reduced target-annulus crossings in Early-cKO mice (genotype x quadrant, F3,87 = 2.939, P < 0.05; WT-control-T vs. Early-cKO-T, *P < 0.05; n = 17, 14). (G) Off-target occupancy plot in Late-cKO mice. (H) Late-cKO spent significantly less time in the target zone (genotype x quadrant, F3,63 = 3.055, P < 0.05; WT-control-T vs. Late-cKO-T, *P < 0.05; n = 12, 11). (I) Fewer target-annulus crossings in Late-cKO mice (genotype x quadrant, F3,63 = 4.834, P < 0.01; WT-control-T vs. Late-cKO-T, **P < 0.01; n = 12, 11). (J–L) Long-term (7 d) contextual fear memory was scored as percent of time freezing in the conditioning context. (J) Freezing levels in WT and KO (t21 = 4.551, ***P < 0.001; n = 12, 11). (K) Freezing levels in WT-control and Early-cKO (t29 = 2.355, *P < 0.05; n = 15, 16). (L) Freezing levels in WT-control and Late-cKO mice (t20 = 2.343, *P < 0.05; n = 9, 13). All data are mean ± SEM. Open gray circles indicate data of individual mice. Two-Way ANOVA with Bonferroni post hoc test was performed in B, C, E, F, H, and I. One-Way ANOVA with Dunnett post hoc test was also performed in B, C, E, F, H, and I to test the preference for the target quadrant within groups (SI Appendix, Table S1). Two-tailed two-sample t test was applied in J–L.

Deletion of Arc/Arg3.1 Before but Not After P21 Alters in Vivo Hippocampal Oscillatory Activity in Adult Mice.

Hippocampal oscillations in the theta and gamma frequency bands and sharp wave ripples in CA1 are believed to be important for encoding spatial information and for consolidating memory (8, 11⇓⇓⇓⇓–16). We hypothesized that these activity patterns would be modified in KO and cKO mice. To test this hypothesis, we performed multichannel depth recordings from the hippocampi of urethane-anesthetized mice and measured local field potentials (LFPs) (Fig. 3 and SI Appendix, Fig. S6). During rapid eye movement (REM)-like phases, WT mice exhibited clear theta (4 to 6 Hz) accompanied by gamma (20 to 50 Hz) oscillations in the CA1 pyramidal layer (Fig. 3 A and C). Slow wave sleep (SWS)-like phases were characterized by large amplitude LFPs in the stratum radiatum [sharp waves (SPWs)], accompanied by high frequency ripples in the pyramidal layer of CA1 [CA1 stratum pyramidale (CA1 str. pyr.)] (Fig. 3 B and C). Strikingly, KO mice exhibited significantly reduced theta and gamma power in the CA1 str. pyr. (Fig. 3D) and significantly fewer ripples (Fig. 3E). Remaining ripples that were still detected in the KO CA1 str. pyr., as well as co-occurring SPWs in the stratum radiatum, were similar in amplitude to those of their WT counterparts (Fig. 3E). Notably, the frequency of the remaining ripples was higher in KO mice (Fig. 3E). Early-cKO mice showed a reduction in theta but not in gamma power in the CA1 str. pyr (Fig. 3F). Ripple numbers in CA1 of Early-cKO mice were WT-like, but their frequency and amplitudes were significantly lower, as was the amplitude of SPWs (Fig. 3G). In stark contrast, Late-cKO mice were WT-like in all measures of network activity in the hippocampus (Fig. 3 H and I). Thus, genetic ablation of Arc/Arg3.1 in the germline or before P14 (KO and Early-cKO) impairs adult oscillatory network activity in the hippocampus whereas removal of Arc/Arg3.1 after P21 (Late-cKO) leaves network activity intact. Our LFP measurements reflect sleep-like activity and are complemented by a previous report of altered hippocampal oscillations and a desynchronization of place cell activity in the Arc/Arg3.1 KO mice during running (38).

Fig. 3.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Fig. 3.

Genetic deletion of Arc/Arg3.1 before P21, but not afterward, impairs oscillatory network activity in the hippocampus. (A and B) Exemplary raw LFP traces were recorded in hippocampus CA1 (three electrodes shown: located above, within, and below str. pyr., respectively) during (A) paradoxical/REM-like sleep and (B) SWS-like states. Genotype is color coded throughout the figure: black, WT/WT-control; red, constitutive KO; blue, Early-cKO; green, Late-cKO. (C) Exemplary spectrograms from CA1 str. pyr of WT, KO, and cKO mice indicated by power per dB. Frequency bands >80 Hz (above the dashed line) were displayed at a lower resolution. (D, F, and H) Power spectra and box plots display the mean power (in dB) in the theta (ϑ) and gamma (γ) frequency bands recorded during paradoxical/REM-like states. A significant reduction in ϑ power is observed in KO mice (D, U = 25, *P < 0.05; n = 14, 8) and Early-cKO (F, U = 50, *P < 0.05; n = 6, 9) but not in Late-cKO mice (H, U = 60, P > 0.05; n = 10, 11). Reduced γ power was significant only in the KO mice (D, U = 26, *P < 0.05; n = 14, 8). (E, G, and I) SPW-ripple complexes were analyzed from SWS-like states. Ripple occurrence (occ.) (s−1), amplitude (amp.) (V−4), frequency (freq.) (Hz), and SPW amplitude (A/m3*104) were significantly impacted in KO (E, Ripple occ.: U = 44, *P < 0.05; Ripple freq.: U = 145, ***P < 0.001; n = 14, 12) and Early-cKO mice (G, Ripple amp.: U = 55, **P < 0.01; Ripple freq.: U = 53, *P < 0.05; SPW amp.: U = 44, *P < 0.05; n = 7, 9). Indistinguishable SPW-ripple activity in WT-control and Late-cKO mice (I, all parameters, P > 0.05; n = 9, 10). Hour glass markers represent the median and 25th and 75th percentiles. Crosses mark outliers. Mann–Whitney–Wilcoxon U test was applied in D–I.

Deletion of Arc/Arg3.1 in Adult Mice Impairs Spatial Memory but Not Learning.

The behavior and LFP analyses suggest that the maturation of spatial learning requires Arc/Arg3.1 expression only during a limited time window of postnatal development. To verify this, we genetically removed Arc/Arg3.1 from the hippocampi of adult mice (Adult-cKO) through stereotactic delivery of rAAV-Cre (Fig. 4A and SI Appendix, Fig. S7). Adult-cKO mice performed in the MWM as well as their control littermates, exhibiting rapidly decreasing escape latencies (Fig. 4B) and indistinguishable utilization of spatial strategies (Fig. 4 C and D). Thus, adult removal of Arc/Arg3.1 from the hippocampus does not affect spatial learning. Similar to KO, Early-cKO, and Late-cKO mice, Adult-cKO also exhibited a profound loss of long-term spatial memory (Fig. 4 E–G).

Fig. 4.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Fig. 4.

Deletion of Arc/Arg3.1 in adult mice impairs spatial memory but not learning. (A) Illustration of bilateral rAAV-Cre injection in the hippocampus to achieve Arc/Arg3.1 ablation in the adult mice. (B) Escape latencies to the hidden platform during nine training days of the MWM were scored for control and Adult-cKO mice (genotype and genotype x day, both P > 0.05; n = 9, 10). (C) Analysis of search strategies in WT-control and Adult-cKO mice scored as percentage of trials for each defined strategy following 9 d of training. Similar progression of navigational strategy was observed in both groups [statistics for day 6 to 9: χ2 test of independence over individual strategies, χ2(6) = 9.835, P > 0.05; n = 9, 10]. Color-coded individual strategy is as in SI Appendix, Fig. S4. C, chaining; DS1, directional search; DS2, directed search; DS3, direct swim; FS, focal search; R, random; S, scanning; T, thigmotaxis. (D) Strategies grouped into precise-spatial (magenta), imprecise-spatial (yellow), and nonspatial (purple) were analyzed for days 6 to 9 in WT-control and Adult-cKO mice [χ2 test of independence for grouped strategies as color coded, χ2(2) = 1.826, P > 0.05; n = 9, 10]. Color code for grouped search categories is as in Fig. 1B. (E–G) Analysis of a probe trial performed 7 d after training completion. Occupancy plot, the percent time spent in the defined zones, and number of annulus crossings were scored in WT-control and Adult-cKO mice. Axis labeling refers to zones as follows: L, left; O, opposite; R, right; T, target. (E) A diffuse occupancy plot of Adult-cKO in contrast to a focal plot of WT-control littermates. (F) Significantly reduced percent time spent in target zone in Adult-cKO mice (genotype x quadrant, F3,51 = 5.916, P < 0.01; WT-control-T vs. Adult-cKO-T, **P < 0.01; n = 9, 10). (G) Decreased number of target-annulus crossings in Adult-cKO mice (genotype x quadrant, F3,51 = 6.957, P < 0.01; WT-control-T vs. Adult-cKO-T, **P < 0.01; n = 9, 10). Data are presented as mean ± SEM in F and G. Open gray circles indicate measurements of individual mice. Two-way ANOVA with Bonferroni post hoc test was performed in B, F, and G. One-way ANOVA with Dunnett post hoc test was also performed in F and G, to test the preference for the target quadrant within groups (SI Appendix, Table S1). WT-controls showed a preference for the target quadrant, but not Adult-cKO.

Discussion

Oscillatory network activity in the rodent hippocampus develops postnatally, starting with the generation of SPWs at P2 to P4, followed by increased gamma (17) and theta power (18), and finally the emergence of ripples (39) in CA1 at P10 to P12. In agreement with this timeline, the presence of Arc/Arg3.1 until the second postnatal week in the Early-cKO mice may have supported the development of CA3 and the emergence of theta/gamma oscillations and sharp waves in CA1, but not their subsequent maturation. This interpretation is in line with an activity-dependent sequential maturation of the hippocampal regions (40). Impaired spatial encoding and loss of theta/gamma entrainment may be mechanisms contributing to the profound behavioral deficits in both KO and Early-cKO mice. Our findings extend previous reports linking theta/gamma oscillations and ripples to spatial learning (11⇓⇓⇓⇓–16) and indicate that their developmental maturation is a prerequisite for the emergence of reference-based navigation. During hippocampus-dependent spatial learning, Arc/Arg3.1 expression is induced in more than 30% of CA1 neurons (34, 41, 42). Navigation-relevant oscillations engage a similarly large network of CA3-CA1 neurons (43, 44). We therefore think it is unlikely that the very low amount of residual Arc/Arg3.1 in the hippocampus of Late-cKO mice (<7% in dentate gyrus (DG), undetectable in CA1) can be responsible for the intact spatial learning and oscillations observed in these animals. The absence of long-term memory in the Late-cKO mice, despite intact oscillatory network activity and ripples, suggests that Arc/Arg3.1 is continuously required for linking these activity patterns to memory consolidation. The loss of contextual fear memory and spatial memory in all KO and cKO mice after a hiatus of 1 wk confirms previous findings (35, 45) that Arc/Arg3.1 is essential for the maintenance of explicit memories. However, in contrast to KO and Early-cKO, Late-cKO mice exhibited normal incremental spatial learning where daily-interspaced training in the same environment incrementally enhances space learning and improves reference-based navigation (6, 46). We hypothesize that the intact oscillatory activity in the Late-cKO enables precise encoding of spatial representations by the hippocampal–entorhinal cortical network (8, 47) and may recruit short-term plasticity, sufficient to carry the spatial representations from day to day. The induced expression of Arc/Arg3.1 observed during MWM training (41, 48) is then required not for incremental learning itself but for the consolidation of memory at later stages.

During development, Arc/Arg3.1 might modify hippocampal network architecture via its known effects on homeostatic synaptic scaling (49⇓–51) and possibly through hitherto unexplored mechanisms. Potential candidate mechanisms might include changes in the microarchitecture of excitation/inhibition connectivity, which affect ripple oscillations and memory (52⇓–54) and mark the opening and closure of critical periods (55). In adult animals, Arc/Arg3.1 could regulate memory consolidation through mechanisms of long-term plasticity (35, 56).

We propose that our findings reflect a critical period for spatial learning mediated by Arc/Arg3.1 expression between P7 and P21. The existence of critical periods for human learning and memory has been debated (57) but has gained strong support by recent rodent research (5, 58). A critical period (P17 to P24) for the acquisition of latent episodic memories (reflected by infantile amnesia) was identified, which is mediated by BDNF and the NMDA receptor (NMDAR) subunit switch, two molecular mechanisms involved in sensory cortex critical periods (1). Of note, since Arc/Arg3.1 is part of the NMDAR complex (59), is up-regulated by BDNF (60, 61), and mediates consolidation of BDNF-dependent synaptic plasticity (56), it potentially may also be involved in sculpting this critical period.

Arc/Arg3.1 has also been implicated in the critical period of ocular dominance in the visual cortex (62), by mediating potentiation of the preserved-eye visual response and depression of the deprived-eye visual response. Recently, Jenks et al. (63) showed that artificially enhancing Arc/Arg3.1 expression can extend this critical period and reintroducing Arc/Arg3.1 into KO visual cortex can restore it. These reports and our findings demonstrate a role for Arc/Arg3.1 in cortical/sensory and hippocampal/memory critical periods. They also raise the intriguing possibility that local differences in Arc/Arg3.1 regulation could generate different time windows and forms of plasticity unique to each region. We speculate that misregulation of Arc/Arg3.1 during critical periods might result in neurodevelopmental disorders, such as schizophrenia (64), fragile X mental retardation syndrome (65), and autism spectrum disorders (66), in which Arc/Arg3.1 has been implicated.

Materials and Methods

Animals.

Mice aged 3 to 6 mo were used in all experiments. All of the experiments were conducted in accordance with the German and European Community laws on protection of experimental animals and approved by the local authorities of the City of Hamburg. Experimenters were blind to the mice genotype until conclusion of experiments and analysis.

Floxed Arc/Arg3.1 mice were generated together with the conventional KO line previously described (35). A type II recombination clone was injected into C57BL/6J blastocysts. Male chimeras were backcrossed into C57BL/6J. Arc/Arg3.1 loxP flanked mice (Arc/Arg3.1f/f) were normal and fertile. The F1 generation of Arc/Arg3.1f/f was backcrossed into C57BL/6J for at least 10 generations. Conditional Arc/Arg3.1 KO (cKO) mice were obtained by breeding the Arc/Arg3.1f/f mice with the following Cre recombinase transgenic mice: (i) Tg(CaMKIIα-cre)1Gsc (67) to obtain Early-cKO mice; (ii) Tg(CaMKIIα-cre)T29-1Stl (68) to obtain Late-cKO mice; and (iii) Tg(CMV-cre) (69) to rederive germline KO mice [these were not different from the conventional KO line (35), and their results were pooled together]. Additional information can be found in SI Appendix, SI Materials and Methods. Detection and quantification of Arc/Arg3.1 were performed with immunoblotting, immunohistochemistry, and radioactive in situ hybridization. Further details are available in SI Appendix, SI Materials and Methods.

Virus Injection.

rAAV1/2-CaMKIIα-CreERT2-2A-Venus were injected into the dorsal and ventral hippocampus of adult mice (3 to 6 mo old), bilaterally. Experiments began 7 d after injections. At the end of experiments, brains of all mice were collected, sectioned, and immunostained to evaluate the extent of Cre expression and Arc/Arg3.1 ablation. Only animals with >80% ablation were included in the results. Details are available in SI Appendix, SI Materials and Methods.

Behavioral Analysis.

Open field, Zero maze, and flinch-jump tests were used to characterize behavior and sensory sensitivity of Arc/Arg3.1f/f mice. The Morris water maze was used to assess spatial learning and memory. Contextual fear conditioning was used to test long-term memory. A custom-made algorithm was written in MATLAB to analyze spatial navigation strategies of mice, based on their swim tracks. Further details are available in SI Appendix, SI Materials and Methods and Fig. S3.

In Vivo Electrophysiology.

Adult mice were recorded under urethane anesthesia using linear 16-site silicon probes inserted into the dorsal hippocampus, as previously described (70, 71). SWS- and REM-like epochs were identified from the multitaper spectrograms calculated from the LFP. Ripples during SWS-like epochs were automatically detected and analyzed, together with their co-occurring sharp waves (SPWs). Power spectra were computed from the current source density (CSD) (72), and theta and gamma power were calculated for the selected REM-like epochs by integrating the area below their respective frequency bands. Details are available in SI Appendix, SI Materials and Methods.

Statistics.

Various statistical tests were used for analysis of different datasets, as indicated in the figure legends. These are also described in detail in SI Appendix, SI Materials and Methods and Table S1.

Acknowledgments

We thank Günther Schütz for providing Tg(CaMKIIα-cre)1Gsc mice, Rolf Sprengel for sharing a Syn-iCre-2A-Venus plasmid, Sabine Hoffmeister-Ulrich for performing copy number variation analysis, Ingke Braren and the Vector Facility, University Medical Center Hamburg–Eppendorf, Hamburg, Germany for virus production, Irm Hermans-Borgmeyer for advice on breeding strategies, and Eva Kronberg for animal breeding and care. This work was supported by DFG Grant SFB 936 to Project B4 (to D.K.) in the years 2011–2015, by Grant “Molekulare Mechanismen der Netzwerkmodifizierung” (to D.K. and O.O.) from the Federal State of Hamburg, and by a scholarship from the Chinese government awarded by the China Scholarship Council (to X.G.).

Footnotes

  • ↵1X.G., S.C.-G., and J.G. contributed equally to this work.

  • ↵2Present address: Department of Neurodegenerative Diseases & Geropsychiatry, University Hospital Bonn, 53127 Bonn, Germany.

  • ↵3To whom correspondence may be addressed. Email: kuhl{at}zmnh.uni-hamburg.de or ora.ohana{at}zmnh.uni-hamburg.de.
  • Author contributions: D.K. and O.O. designed research; X.G., S.C.-G., J.G., S.G., U.S., L.B., D.M., and D.I. performed research; X.G., S.C.-G., J.G., D.I., D.K., and O.O. analyzed data; and X.G., S.C.-G., J.G., D.K., and O.O. wrote the paper.

  • The authors declare no conflict of interest.

  • This article is a PNAS Direct Submission.

  • This article contains supporting information online at www.pnas.org/lookup/suppl/doi:10.1073/pnas.1810125115/-/DCSupplemental.

Published under the PNAS license.

References

  1. ↵
    1. Hensch TK
    (2005) Critical period plasticity in local cortical circuits. Nat Rev Neurosci 6:877–888.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  2. ↵
    1. Knudsen EI
    (2004) Sensitive periods in the development of the brain and behavior. J Cogn Neurosci 16:1412–1425.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  3. ↵
    1. Kral A
    (2013) Auditory critical periods: A review from system’s perspective. Neuroscience 247:117–133.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  4. ↵
    1. Travaglia A,
    2. Bisaz R,
    3. Cruz E,
    4. Alberini CM
    (2016) Developmental changes in plasticity, synaptic, glia and connectivity protein levels in rat dorsal hippocampus. Neurobiol Learn Mem 135:125–138.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  5. ↵
    1. Travaglia A,
    2. Bisaz R,
    3. Sweet ES,
    4. Blitzer RD,
    5. Alberini CM
    (2016) Infantile amnesia reflects a developmental critical period for hippocampal learning. Nat Neurosci 19:1225–1233.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  6. ↵
    1. Morris RG,
    2. Garrud P,
    3. Rawlins JN,
    4. O’Keefe J
    (1982) Place navigation impaired in rats with hippocampal lesions. Nature 297:681–683.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  7. ↵
    1. Scoville WB,
    2. Milner B
    (1957) Loss of recent memory after bilateral hippocampal lesions. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 20:11–21.
    OpenUrlFREE Full Text
  8. ↵
    1. Buzsáki G,
    2. Moser EI
    (2013) Memory, navigation and theta rhythm in the hippocampal-entorhinal system. Nat Neurosci 16:130–138.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  9. ↵
    1. Eichenbaum H
    (2017) On the integration of space, time, and memory. Neuron 95:1007–1018.
    OpenUrl
  10. ↵
    1. Lisman J, et al.
    (2017) Viewpoints: How the hippocampus contributes to memory, navigation and cognition. Nat Neurosci 20:1434–1447.
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  11. ↵
    1. Ego-Stengel V,
    2. Wilson MA
    (2010) Disruption of ripple-associated hippocampal activity during rest impairs spatial learning in the rat. Hippocampus 20:1–10.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  12. ↵
    1. Girardeau G,
    2. Benchenane K,
    3. Wiener SI,
    4. Buzsáki G,
    5. Zugaro MB
    (2009) Selective suppression of hippocampal ripples impairs spatial memory. Nat Neurosci 12:1222–1223.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  13. ↵
    1. Jadhav SP,
    2. Kemere C,
    3. German PW,
    4. Frank LM
    (2012) Awake hippocampal sharp-wave ripples support spatial memory. Science 336:1454–1458.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  14. ↵
    1. Montgomery SM,
    2. Buzsáki G
    (2007) Gamma oscillations dynamically couple hippocampal CA3 and CA1 regions during memory task performance. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 104:14495–14500.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  15. ↵
    1. Nyhus E,
    2. Curran T
    (2010) Functional role of gamma and theta oscillations in episodic memory. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 34:1023–1035.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  16. ↵
    1. Winson J
    (1978) Loss of hippocampal theta rhythm results in spatial memory deficit in the rat. Science 201:160–163.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  17. ↵
    1. Lahtinen H, et al.
    (2002) Postnatal development of rat hippocampal gamma rhythm in vivo. J Neurophysiol 88:1469–1474.
    OpenUrlPubMed
  18. ↵
    1. Leblanc MO,
    2. Bland BH
    (1979) Developmental aspects of hippocampal electrical activity and motor behavior in the rat. Exp Neurol 66:220–237.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  19. ↵
    1. Leinekugel X, et al.
    (2002) Correlated bursts of activity in the neonatal hippocampus in vivo. Science 296:2049–2052.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  20. ↵
    1. Langston RF, et al.
    (2010) Development of the spatial representation system in the rat. Science 328:1576–1580.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  21. ↵
    1. Ainge JA,
    2. Langston RF
    (2012) Ontogeny of neural circuits underlying spatial memory in the rat. Front Neural Circuits 6:8.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  22. ↵
    1. Akers KG,
    2. Arruda-Carvalho M,
    3. Josselyn SA,
    4. Frankland PW
    (2012) Ontogeny of contextual fear memory formation, specificity, and persistence in mice. Learn Mem 19:598–604.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  23. ↵
    1. Albani SH,
    2. McHail DG,
    3. Dumas TC
    (2014) Developmental studies of the hippocampus and hippocampal-dependent behaviors: Insights from interdisciplinary studies and tips for new investigators. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 43:183–190.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  24. ↵
    1. Guskjolen A,
    2. Josselyn SA,
    3. Frankland PW
    (2017) Age-dependent changes in spatial memory retention and flexibility in mice. Neurobiol Learn Mem 143:59–66.
    OpenUrl
  25. ↵
    1. Rudy JW,
    2. Stadler-Morris S,
    3. Albert P
    (1987) Ontogeny of spatial navigation behaviors in the rat: Dissociation of “proximal”- and “distal”-cue-based behaviors. Behav Neurosci 101:62–73.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  26. ↵
    1. Wills TJ,
    2. Muessig L,
    3. Cacucci F
    (2013) The development of spatial behaviour and the hippocampal neural representation of space. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci 369:20130409.
    OpenUrl
  27. ↵
    1. Tan HM,
    2. Wills TJ,
    3. Cacucci F
    (2017) The development of spatial and memory circuits in the rat. Wiley Interdiscip Rev Cogn Sci 8:e1424.
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  28. ↵
    1. Blumberg MS,
    2. Freeman JH,
    3. Robinson SR
    1. Dumas TC,
    2. Rudy JW
    (2010) Development of the hippocampal memory system: Creating networks and modifiable synapses. Oxford Handbook of Developmental Behavioral Neuroscience, eds Blumberg MS, Freeman JH, Robinson SR (Oxford Univ Press, New York), pp 587–606.
  29. ↵
    1. Egorov AV,
    2. Draguhn A
    (2013) Development of coherent neuronal activity patterns in mammalian cortical networks: Common principles and local hetereogeneity. Mech Dev 130:412–423.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  30. ↵
    1. Link W, et al.
    (1995) Somatodendritic expression of an immediate early gene is regulated by synaptic activity. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 92:5734–5738.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  31. ↵
    1. Lyford GL, et al.
    (1995) Arc, a growth factor and activity-regulated gene, encodes a novel cytoskeleton-associated protein that is enriched in neuronal dendrites. Neuron 14:433–445.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  32. ↵
    1. Steward O,
    2. Wallace CS,
    3. Lyford GL,
    4. Worley PF
    (1998) Synaptic activation causes the mRNA for the IEG Arc to localize selectively near activated postsynaptic sites on dendrites. Neuron 21:741–751.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  33. ↵
    1. Waltereit R, et al.
    (2001) Arg3.1/Arc mRNA induction by Ca2+ and cAMP requires protein kinase A and mitogen-activated protein kinase/extracellular regulated kinase activation. J Neurosci 21:5484–5493.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  34. ↵
    1. Guzowski JF,
    2. McNaughton BL,
    3. Barnes CA,
    4. Worley PF
    (1999) Environment-specific expression of the immediate-early gene Arc in hippocampal neuronal ensembles. Nat Neurosci 2:1120–1124.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  35. ↵
    1. Plath N, et al.
    (2006) Arc/Arg3.1 is essential for the consolidation of synaptic plasticity and memories. Neuron 52:437–444.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  36. ↵
    1. Morris RGM
    (1981) Spatial localization does not require the presence of local cues. Learn Motiv 12:239–260.
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  37. ↵
    1. Chersi F,
    2. Burgess N
    (2015) The cognitive architecture of spatial navigation: Hippocampal and striatal contributions. Neuron 88:64–77.
    OpenUrl
  38. ↵
    1. Malkki HA, et al.
    (2016) Effects of Arc/Arg3.1 gene deletion on rhythmic synchronization of hippocampal CA1 neurons during locomotor activity and sleep. Neurobiol Learn Mem 131:155–165.
    OpenUrl
  39. ↵
    1. Buhl DL,
    2. Buzsáki G
    (2005) Developmental emergence of hippocampal fast-field “ripple” oscillations in the behaving rat pups. Neuroscience 134:1423–1430.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  40. ↵
    1. Donato F,
    2. Jacobsen RI,
    3. Moser MB,
    4. Moser EI
    (2017) Stellate cells drive maturation of the entorhinal-hippocampal circuit. Science 355:eaai8178.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  41. ↵
    1. Guzowski JF, et al.
    (2006) Recent behavioral history modifies coupling between cell activity and Arc gene transcription in hippocampal CA1 neurons. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 103:1077–1082.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  42. ↵
    1. Vazdarjanova A, et al.
    (2006) Spatial exploration induces ARC, a plasticity-related immediate-early gene, only in calcium/calmodulin-dependent protein kinase II-positive principal excitatory and inhibitory neurons of the rat forebrain. J Comp Neurol 498:317–329.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  43. ↵
    1. Csicsvari J,
    2. Hirase H,
    3. Mamiya A,
    4. Buzsáki G
    (2000) Ensemble patterns of hippocampal CA3-CA1 neurons during sharp wave-associated population events. Neuron 28:585–594.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  44. ↵
    1. Wilson MA,
    2. McNaughton BL
    (1993) Dynamics of the hippocampal ensemble code for space. Science 261:1055–1058.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  45. ↵
    1. Guzowski JF, et al.
    (2000) Inhibition of activity-dependent arc protein expression in the rat hippocampus impairs the maintenance of long-term potentiation and the consolidation of long-term memory. J Neurosci 20:3993–4001.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  46. ↵
    1. Commins S,
    2. Cunningham L,
    3. Harvey D,
    4. Walsh D
    (2003) Massed but not spaced training impairs spatial memory. Behav Brain Res 139:215–223.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  47. ↵
    1. McNaughton BL,
    2. Battaglia FP,
    3. Jensen O,
    4. Moser EI,
    5. Moser MB
    (2006) Path integration and the neural basis of the ‘cognitive map’ Nat Rev Neurosci 7:663–678.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  48. ↵
    1. Gardner RS, et al.
    (2016) Differential Arc expression in the hippocampus and striatum during the transition from attentive to automatic navigation on a plus maze. Neurobiol Learn Mem 131:36–45.
    OpenUrl
  49. ↵
    1. Okuno H, et al.
    (2012) Inverse synaptic tagging of inactive synapses via dynamic interaction of Arc/Arg3.1 with CaMKIIβ. Cell 149:886–898.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  50. ↵
    1. Ranson A,
    2. Cheetham CE,
    3. Fox K,
    4. Sengpiel F
    (2012) Homeostatic plasticity mechanisms are required for juvenile, but not adult, ocular dominance plasticity. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 109:1311–1316.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  51. ↵
    1. Shepherd JD, et al.
    (2006) Arc/Arg3.1 mediates homeostatic synaptic scaling of AMPA receptors. Neuron 52:475–484.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  52. ↵
    1. Valero M, et al.
    (2017) Mechanisms for selective single-cell reactivation during offline sharp-wave ripples and their distortion by fast ripples. Neuron 94:1234–1247.e7.
    OpenUrl
  53. ↵
    1. Valero M, et al.
    (2015) Determinants of different deep and superficial CA1 pyramidal cell dynamics during sharp-wave ripples. Nat Neurosci 18:1281–1290.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  54. ↵
    1. Pelkey KA, et al.
    (2015) Pentraxins coordinate excitatory synapse maturation and circuit integration of parvalbumin interneurons. Neuron 85:1257–1272.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  55. ↵
    1. Hensch TK, et al.
    (1998) Local GABA circuit control of experience-dependent plasticity in developing visual cortex. Science 282:1504–1508.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  56. ↵
    1. Messaoudi E, et al.
    (2007) Sustained Arc/Arg3.1 synthesis controls long-term potentiation consolidation through regulation of local actin polymerization in the dentate gyrus in vivo. J Neurosci 27:10445–10455.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  57. ↵
    1. Nelson CA
    (2000) Neural plasticity and human development: The role of early experience in sculpting memory systems. Dev Sci 3:115–136.
    OpenUrl
  58. ↵
    1. Alberini CM,
    2. Travaglia A
    (2017) Infantile amnesia: A critical period of learning to learn and remember. J Neurosci 37:5783–5795.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  59. ↵
    1. Fernández E, et al.
    (2017) Arc requires PSD95 for assembly into postsynaptic complexes involved with neural dysfunction and intelligence. Cell Rep 21:679–691.
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  60. ↵
    1. Messaoudi E,
    2. Ying SW,
    3. Kanhema T,
    4. Croll SD,
    5. Bramham CR
    (2002) Brain-derived neurotrophic factor triggers transcription-dependent, late phase long-term potentiation in vivo. J Neurosci 22:7453–7461.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  61. ↵
    1. Yin Y,
    2. Edelman GM,
    3. Vanderklish PW
    (2002) The brain-derived neurotrophic factor enhances synthesis of Arc in synaptoneurosomes. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 99:2368–2373.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  62. ↵
    1. McCurry CL, et al.
    (2010) Loss of Arc renders the visual cortex impervious to the effects of sensory experience or deprivation. Nat Neurosci 13:450–457.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  63. ↵
    1. Jenks KR, et al.
    (2017) Arc restores juvenile plasticity in adult mouse visual cortex. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 114:9182–9187.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  64. ↵
    1. Fromer M, et al.
    (2014) De novo mutations in schizophrenia implicate synaptic networks. Nature 506:179–184.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  65. ↵
    1. Niere F,
    2. Wilkerson JR,
    3. Huber KM
    (2012) Evidence for a fragile X mental retardation protein-mediated translational switch in metabotropic glutamate receptor-triggered Arc translation and long-term depression. J Neurosci 32:5924–5936.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  66. ↵
    1. Greer PL, et al.
    (2010) The Angelman syndrome protein Ube3A regulates synapse development by ubiquitinating arc. Cell 140:704–716.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  67. ↵
    1. Casanova E, et al.
    (2001) A CamKIIalpha iCre BAC allows brain-specific gene inactivation. Genesis 31:37–42.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  68. ↵
    1. Tsien JZ, et al.
    (1996) Subregion- and cell type-restricted gene knockout in mouse brain. Cell 87:1317–1326.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  69. ↵
    1. Schwenk F,
    2. Baron U,
    3. Rajewsky K
    (1995) A cre-transgenic mouse strain for the ubiquitous deletion of loxP-flanked gene segments including deletion in germ cells. Nucleic Acids Res 23:5080–5081.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  70. ↵
    1. Fazeli W, et al.
    (2017) Early-life exposure to caffeine affects the construction and activity of cortical networks in mice. Exp Neurol 295:88–103.
    OpenUrl
  71. ↵
    1. Prox J, et al.
    (2013) Postnatal disruption of the disintegrin/metalloproteinase ADAM10 in brain causes epileptic seizures, learning deficits, altered spine morphology, and defective synaptic functions. J Neurosci 33:12915–12928.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  72. ↵
    1. Pettersen KH,
    2. Devor A,
    3. Ulbert I,
    4. Dale AM,
    5. Einevoll GT
    (2006) Current-source density estimation based on inversion of electrostatic forward solution: Effects of finite extent of neuronal activity and conductivity discontinuities. J Neurosci Methods 154:116–133.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
PreviousNext
Back to top
Article Alerts
Email Article

Thank you for your interest in spreading the word on PNAS.

NOTE: We only request your email address so that the person you are recommending the page to knows that you wanted them to see it, and that it is not junk mail. We do not capture any email address.

Enter multiple addresses on separate lines or separate them with commas.
Arc/Arg3.1 mediates a critical period for spatial learning and hippocampal networks
(Your Name) has sent you a message from PNAS
(Your Name) thought you would like to see the PNAS web site.
CAPTCHA
This question is for testing whether or not you are a human visitor and to prevent automated spam submissions.
Citation Tools
Arc/Arg3.1 mediates a critical period for spatial learning and hippocampal networks
Xiaoyan Gao, Sergio Castro-Gomez, Jasper Grendel, Sabine Graf, Ute Süsens, Lars Binkle, Daniel Mensching, Dirk Isbrandt, Dietmar Kuhl, Ora Ohana
Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences Dec 2018, 115 (49) 12531-12536; DOI: 10.1073/pnas.1810125115

Citation Manager Formats

  • BibTeX
  • Bookends
  • EasyBib
  • EndNote (tagged)
  • EndNote 8 (xml)
  • Medlars
  • Mendeley
  • Papers
  • RefWorks Tagged
  • Ref Manager
  • RIS
  • Zotero
Request Permissions
Share
Arc/Arg3.1 mediates a critical period for spatial learning and hippocampal networks
Xiaoyan Gao, Sergio Castro-Gomez, Jasper Grendel, Sabine Graf, Ute Süsens, Lars Binkle, Daniel Mensching, Dirk Isbrandt, Dietmar Kuhl, Ora Ohana
Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences Dec 2018, 115 (49) 12531-12536; DOI: 10.1073/pnas.1810125115
del.icio.us logo Digg logo Reddit logo Twitter logo CiteULike logo Facebook logo Google logo Mendeley logo
  • Tweet Widget
  • Facebook Like
  • Mendeley logo Mendeley

Article Classifications

  • Biological Sciences
  • Neuroscience
Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences: 115 (49)
Table of Contents

Submit

Sign up for Article Alerts

Jump to section

  • Article
    • Abstract
    • Results
    • Discussion
    • Materials and Methods
    • Acknowledgments
    • Footnotes
    • References
  • Figures & SI
  • Info & Metrics
  • PDF

You May Also be Interested in

Water from a faucet fills a glass.
News Feature: How “forever chemicals” might impair the immune system
Researchers are exploring whether these ubiquitous fluorinated molecules might worsen infections or hamper vaccine effectiveness.
Image credit: Shutterstock/Dmitry Naumov.
Reflection of clouds in the still waters of Mono Lake in California.
Inner Workings: Making headway with the mysteries of life’s origins
Recent experiments and simulations are starting to answer some fundamental questions about how life came to be.
Image credit: Shutterstock/Radoslaw Lecyk.
Cave in coastal Kenya with tree growing in the middle.
Journal Club: Small, sharp blades mark shift from Middle to Later Stone Age in coastal Kenya
Archaeologists have long tried to define the transition between the two time periods.
Image credit: Ceri Shipton.
Illustration of groups of people chatting
Exploring the length of human conversations
Adam Mastroianni and Daniel Gilbert explore why conversations almost never end when people want them to.
Listen
Past PodcastsSubscribe
Panda bear hanging in a tree
How horse manure helps giant pandas tolerate cold
A study finds that giant pandas roll in horse manure to increase their cold tolerance.
Image credit: Fuwen Wei.

Similar Articles

Site Logo
Powered by HighWire
  • Submit Manuscript
  • Twitter
  • Facebook
  • RSS Feeds
  • Email Alerts

Articles

  • Current Issue
  • Special Feature Articles – Most Recent
  • List of Issues

PNAS Portals

  • Anthropology
  • Chemistry
  • Classics
  • Front Matter
  • Physics
  • Sustainability Science
  • Teaching Resources

Information

  • Authors
  • Editorial Board
  • Reviewers
  • Subscribers
  • Librarians
  • Press
  • Cozzarelli Prize
  • Site Map
  • PNAS Updates
  • FAQs
  • Accessibility Statement
  • Rights & Permissions
  • About
  • Contact

Feedback    Privacy/Legal

Copyright © 2021 National Academy of Sciences. Online ISSN 1091-6490