Skip to main content

Main menu

  • Home
  • Articles
    • Current
    • Special Feature Articles - Most Recent
    • Special Features
    • Colloquia
    • Collected Articles
    • PNAS Classics
    • List of Issues
  • Front Matter
    • Front Matter Portal
    • Journal Club
  • News
    • For the Press
    • This Week In PNAS
    • PNAS in the News
  • Podcasts
  • Authors
    • Information for Authors
    • Editorial and Journal Policies
    • Submission Procedures
    • Fees and Licenses
  • Submit
  • Submit
  • About
    • Editorial Board
    • PNAS Staff
    • FAQ
    • Accessibility Statement
    • Rights and Permissions
    • Site Map
  • Contact
  • Journal Club
  • Subscribe
    • Subscription Rates
    • Subscriptions FAQ
    • Open Access
    • Recommend PNAS to Your Librarian

User menu

  • Log in
  • My Cart

Search

  • Advanced search
Home
Home
  • Log in
  • My Cart

Advanced Search

  • Home
  • Articles
    • Current
    • Special Feature Articles - Most Recent
    • Special Features
    • Colloquia
    • Collected Articles
    • PNAS Classics
    • List of Issues
  • Front Matter
    • Front Matter Portal
    • Journal Club
  • News
    • For the Press
    • This Week In PNAS
    • PNAS in the News
  • Podcasts
  • Authors
    • Information for Authors
    • Editorial and Journal Policies
    • Submission Procedures
    • Fees and Licenses
  • Submit
Research Article

Molecular mechanisms of cocaine reward: Combined dopamine and serotonin transporter knockouts eliminate cocaine place preference

Ichiro Sora, F. Scott Hall, Anne M. Andrews, Masanari Itokawa, Xiao-Fei Li, Hong-Bing Wei, Christine Wichems, Klaus-Peter Lesch, Dennis L. Murphy, and George R. Uhl
  1. *Molecular Neurobiology, National Institute on Drug Abuse-Intramural Research Program, National Institutes of Health, Baltimore, MD 21224; ‡Laboratory of Clinical Science, National Institute of Mental Health-Intramural Research Program, National Institutes of Health, Bethesda, MD 20892-1264; †Department of Chemistry, Pennsylvania State University, PA 16802; and §Department of Psychiatry, University of Würzburg, Würzburg 97080, Germany

See allHide authors and affiliations

PNAS April 24, 2001 98 (9) 5300-5305; https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.091039298
Ichiro Sora
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
F. Scott Hall
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Anne M. Andrews
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Masanari Itokawa
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Xiao-Fei Li
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Hong-Bing Wei
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Christine Wichems
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Klaus-Peter Lesch
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Dennis L. Murphy
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
George R. Uhl
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
  1. Edited by Solomon H. Snyder, Johns Hopkins University School of Medicine, Baltimore, MD, and approved February 28, 2001 (received for review January 24, 2001)

  • Article
  • Figures & SI
  • Info & Metrics
  • PDF
Loading

Abstract

Cocaine blocks uptake by neuronal plasma membrane transporters for dopamine (DAT), serotonin (SERT), and norepinephrine (NET). Cocaine reward/reinforcement has been linked to actions at DAT or to blockade of SERT. However, knockouts of neither DAT, SERT, or NET reduce cocaine reward/reinforcement, leaving substantial uncertainty about cocaine's molecular mechanisms for reward. Conceivably, the molecular bases of cocaine reward might display sufficient redundancy that either DAT or SERT might be able to mediate cocaine reward in the other's absence. To test this hypothesis, we examined double knockout mice with deletions of one or both copies of both the DAT and SERT genes. These mice display viability, weight gain, histologic features, neurochemical parameters, and baseline behavioral features that allow tests of cocaine influences. Mice with even a single wild-type DAT gene copy and no SERT copies retain cocaine reward/reinforcement, as measured by conditioned place-preference testing. However, mice with no DAT and either no or one SERT gene copy display no preference for places where they have previously received cocaine. The serotonin dependence of cocaine reward in DAT knockout mice is thus confirmed by the elimination of cocaine place preference in DAT/SERT double knockout mice. These results provide insights into the brain molecular targets necessary for cocaine reward in knockout mice that develop in their absence and suggest novel strategies for anticocaine medication development.

Cocaine is a reinforcing/rewarding abused drug that confers substantial morbidity and mortality (1). No present medication robustly blocks cocaine reward/reinforcement or substantially relieves cocaine dependence (2). Cocaine's potent actions in blocking uptake by neuronal plasma membrane transporters for dopamine (DAT), serotonin (SERT), and norepinephrine (NET) are well known (3). However, the relationships between these molecular actions and cocaine reward/reinforcement have remained obscure (4). Improved understanding of the molecular underpinnings of cocaine reward should increase understanding of brain reward mechanisms and provide better opportunities for rational design of effective anticocaine medications.

Most molecular explanations for cocaine reward/reinforcement have focused on DAT, whereas some implicate SERT. Support for DAT as a primary site for cocaine reward/reinforcement has come from structure–activity studies of DAT blockers (3, 5), effects of dopamine lesions on cocaine reward (6–8), and findings of enhanced dopamine release after cocaine administration (6). However, recent data demonstrate intact cocaine reward in each of two strains of DAT knockout mice (6, 9, 10). DAT is thus not absolutely required for cocaine reward in mice that develop in its absence. Several compounds that potently inhibit dopamine uptake, drugs that include mazindol, display only limited abuse liability in humans or animal models (11–13). Mazindol potently inhibits DAT and NET but only weakly inhibits SERT; these differences from cocaine could conceivably contribute to the distinct reward profile of mazindol (14–16). Contributions of SERT blockade to cocaine reward/reinforcement have also been suggested. Manipulations of serotonin systems that can modulate cocaine or amphetamine reward (17–21) include 5-HT1B serotonin receptor knockout (22), 5-HT1B agonists or antagonists (20, 22, 23), serotonin depletion (24), 5-HT2 antagonists (25), and 5-HT1A agonists (26). However, the SERT-selective blockers widely used for depression seldom either produce the rewarding responses characteristic of cocaine or enhance cocaine reward (27, 28). Cocaine reward is intact in SERT knockout mice (9). Indeed, the enhanced cocaine reward that can be found in SERT and NET knockout mice (9, 29) could suggest that NET- and SERT-expressing systems might contribute to the aversive features that can be associated with cocaine use (30).

Although no single monoamine transporter is absolutely necessary for cocaine reward in mice that develop in its absence, several possible roles for these transporters in cocaine reward in wild-type mice nevertheless remain (31–33). Cocaine may normally work as a “dirty drug” that produces reward through simultaneous actions at more than one transporter site. For example, DAT/SERT selectivity ratios can provide better correlations with cocaine analog reward/reinforcement than DAT affinity alone (34). Multiple molecular sites for cocaine's rewarding/reinforcing actions could provide such redundancies that no one site was absolutely necessary for cocaine reward. Such redundancies could be enhanced by compensatory mechanisms active in mice that develop with transporter absence. Thus, if cocaine altered activities in several parallel or interactive brain systems with substantial redundancy, the systems expressing the remaining transporter(s) might compensate for loss of cocaine-modulated activities in the systems that normally express the absent transporters, maintaining cocaine reward. To assess whether DAT- and SERT-expressing systems could provide such redundancy in the long-term absence of the other transporter, we have constructed double knockout mice with deletions of one or two copies of both the DAT and SERT genes. We have examined baseline features and then tested cocaine responses, including assessments of cocaine reward/reinforcement. We use cocaine-conditioned place preference as a primary measure of cocaine reward/reinforcement.

Materials and Methods

Subjects.

Animals were bred from the single knockout lines previously reported under American Association of Laboratory Animal Care and National Institutes of Health guidelines (9).

Brain Neurochemistry.

Mice were killed by cervical dislocation; brains were rapidly removed and dissected on ice. Levels of monoamines, receptors, transporters, and mRNAs were assessed in regional brain samples stored at −70°C. Frontal cortex, hippocampus, striatum, brainstem, and hypothalamic samples were analyzed for monoamine neurotransmitters and their metabolites by HPLC using electrochemical detection at +0.3 V with minor modifications, as described (35), by using 10 × 4.6-mm Spherisorb 3-mm ODS-2 reversed-phase chromatography columns (Thomson Instruments, Springfield, VA) and elution with 0.1 M monochloroacetic acid/8% acetonitrile/0.5 g/liter octanesulfonic acid/0.3% triethylamine/10 mM EDTA. Monoamines were quantitated relative to the internal standard, 5-hydroxy-N-methyltryptamine, and protein determined by Lowry's method.

Receptor Binding.

To measure receptor and transporter densities and affinities, washed membranes were prepared from brain regions and incubated with [3H] ligands, and membrane-associated ligands were estimated after rapid filtration over Whatman GF/B filters using a Brandel (Bethesda, MD) apparatus. DAT, SERT, NET, dopamine D2 receptor, and serotonin 5HT1A receptor densities were analyzed by saturation binding of [3H] WIN35428 (36), [3H] paroxetine (37), [3H] nisoxetine (38), [3H] YM-09151–2 (39), and [3H] [8-hydroxy-2-(di-n-propylamino)tetralin] (8-OH DPAT) (40), respectively, using results from parallel incubations with 1 mM unlabeled 0.1 mM cocaine/0.1 mM citalopram/0.1 mM desipramine/10 μM unlabeled sulpiride/10 μM unlabeled serotonin to estimate nonspecific binding, respectively.

5-HT1B Receptor Autoradiography.

Mice were decapitated, the brains rapidly removed and frozen, and the left hemispheres sectioned sagittally (20 μm) at −20°C. Sections were thaw-mounted on gelatin-coated slides and stored at −70°C. Sections were incubated twice for 15 min in 170 mM Tris⋅HCl (pH 7.4) with 150 mM NaCl, dried, incubated for 2 h at room temperature in incubation buffer with 20 pM [125I]-cyanopindolol (CYP) (NEN Life Sciences)/20 μM isoproterenol (Research Biochemicals) to block β-adrenergic receptors, and 10 nM DPAT (Research Biochemicals) to block 5-HT1A receptors, dipped in 4°C 170 mM Tris⋅HCl (pH 7.4), washed twice in 4°C fresh wash buffer, dipped in 4°C distilled water, dried under a stream of air, and apposed to Hyperfilm-3H for 48 h, as described (41). Films were developed, and [125I]-CYP binding was quantified by using nih image Software (National Institutes of Health).

Behavioral Testing.

Behavioral tests were performed as described (9). Locomotor activity was assessed as total distance traveled, calculated from measurement of beam breaks in Optovarimex activity monitors (Columbus Instruments, Columbus, OH), to which the mice had not been previously exposed, under dim light sound-attenuated conditions (9). After 3 h of habituation, mice were injected with cocaine HCl (10 mg/kg s.c.). Reward/reinforcement was assessed by conditioned place preference testing using a two-compartment Plexiglas chamber, one with a wire mesh floor and one with corncob bedding on a smooth Plexiglas floor, as described (9). During conditioning sessions, mice were restricted to single compartments for 20 min after injection with cocaine (10 or 20 mg/kg s.c.) or saline, returned to their home cages for 4 h, and then subjected to another 20-min conditioning trial. Injections were counterbalanced. Cocaine withdrawal has not produced aversive effects in this same apparatus at 4 h after these cocaine doses. A single conditioned place preference assessment session followed the last conditioning session by 24 h. In these sessions, mice had access to both compartments, and the proportion of the 20-min session spent on each side was recorded. Results were compared with the proportion of time spent on that side in preconditioning sessions (9). Statistical comparisons used the Statistical Package for Social Science, t tests, and ANOVAs, followed by Scheffe post hoc analyses. Data are presented as mean ± SEM. Conditioning studies were conducted blind to genotype, often resulting in uneven numbers of mice in each group.

Results

Features of Combined DAT and SERT Knockout Mice.

(i) Viability.

Matings between DAT and SERT knockout mice produce mice with all possible genotypes at the DAT and SERT loci in ratios close to those expected (Fig. 1A) (9, 42, 43). Gross and histologic examinations reveal that both DAT and DAT/SERT knockout mice display reduced pituitary sizes and abnormal central anterior pituitaries with fewer acidophilic cells, as previously reported in DAT knockout mice (44). DAT−/− SERT+/+ mice demonstrate slower and smaller postweaning weight gains, as previously reported (9, 42). These are partially complemented by SERT knockout (Fig. 1B). Eight-week-old DAT−/− homozygous knockouts are larger in the absence of SERT than in the presence of one or two copies of the wild-type SERT gene. No other gross or microscopic pathology in the double knockouts not seen in single knockouts has been identified (D. Huso, I.S., and G.R.U., unpublished data).

Figure 1
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 1

Disruption of the DAT and SERT genes and effects on weights at 2 months. (A) Southern analysis of hybridization of the 3′ SERT genomic probe to KpnI-digested DNA extracted from tail tips of seven mice displaying wild-type (+/+, lane 3), heterozygote (+/−, lanes 4, 5, 7), and homozygote (−/−, lanes 1, 2, 6) patterns. The presence of an 8.1-kb fragment indicates a homozygous mutant genotype, whereas wild-type fragments are 9.6 kb32. (B) Southern analysis of hybridization of the 5′ DAT genomic probe to EcoRI-digested DNA extracted from tail tips of seven mice displaying wild-type (+/+, lane 7), heterozygote (+/−, lanes 2, 3, and 6), and homozygote (−/−, lanes 1, 4, and 5) patterns. The presence of a 5.3-kb fragment indicates the homologous recombinant genotype, whereas wild-type fragments are 6.0 kb9. (C) Weights of mice of each genotype at 8 weeks of age. Mean (± SEM; n, 5–30) values for weights were compared by using ANOVA followed by Scheffe post hoc analyses; *, significantly different from wild type; #, significantly different from DAT−/− SERT+/+; P < 0.05 for each.

(ii) Monoamine transporter and receptor binding.

Densities of DAT and SERT expression are reduced in gene-dose-dependent fashion. DAT+/− mice show similar reductions in DAT expression on either wild-type or knockout SERT−/− backgrounds (Table 1). SERT expression also follows the number of intact copies of the SERT gene on DAT+/+ and on DAT−/− backgrounds. There is thus little evidence for any substantial crosstalk in determining striatal or frontal cortical levels of expression of these two transporters. Receptor autoradiographic analyses also fail to document any striking regional differences in knockout effects on transporter expression (data not shown). Each region assessed follows similar genotype-dependent patterns of expression. We have characterized several of the many dopamine and serotonin receptors that could be assessed in these mice. We have characterized striatal expression of the DRD2 dopamine receptor subtype (45–48) because of its high level of expression in this region and previous reports of reduced striatal expression in DAT−/− mice (42). DRD2 expression was not further reduced in the DAT−/− SERT−/− double knockout mice. Binding to one of the most abundant serotonin receptors, the 5-HT1A, was most reproducible and previously documented in the hippocampus (Table 1). Hippocampal 5-HT1A binding, assessed by using 8-OH DPAT, was reduced in SERT+/− and −/− mice. DAT knockout returned these binding levels toward wild-type values. Half-wild-type levels of DAT exacerbated the hippocampal 5-HT1A-binding changes induced by SERT deletions (Table 1). Autoradiographic analyses of 5HT1B receptor densities revealed statistically significant differences from wild-type values only in ventral medial midbrain in double knockout DAT−/− SERT−/− mice. SERT−/− mice with either a single copy of DAT or DAT−/− mice with a single SERT gene copy displayed smaller 15% reductions in ventral midbrain that did not reach statistical significance (Table 2). No other region sampled displayed significant alterations in 5HT1B-binding levels. NET levels, assessed by using [3H] nisoxetine binding, also provided only a trend toward down-regulation in DAT−/− and SERT−/− mice. Mice with the combined DAT−/− SERT−/− knockouts displayed more than 60% reductions of nisoxetine Bmax values. Indeed, this difference and a trend in ventral midbrain 5HT1B receptor densities are the only changes in the combined DAT−/− SERT−/− knockouts that convincingly distinguish them from the mice that retain a single wild-type copy of one of these genes.

View this table:
  • View inline
  • View popup
Table 1

Dopamine and serotonin transporter and receptor alterations in single- and multiple-knockout mice

View this table:
  • View inline
  • View popup
Table 2

Regional 5-HT1B receptor densities in DAT/SERT knockout mice

(iii) Monoamine neurochemistry.

We focused neurochemical analyses on the heavily monoamine innervated striatum and frontal cortex. DAT−/− mice display marked alterations in striatal dopamine and its metabolites homovanillic acid (HVA) and 3,4-dihydroxyphenylacetic acid (DOPAC) [Table 3, previously noted (42)]. SERT−/− mice display reduced levels of serotonin and its metabolite 5-HIAA in frontal cortex [Table 3, previously noted (43)]. DAT−/− mice display modest 15–35% reductions in frontal cortical norepinephrine content (Table 3). Adding SERT deletion to DAT knockouts enhances DOPAC alterations (Bonferroni post hoc comparisons; Table 3) but fails to further alter serotonin or norepinephrine in any dramatic fashion.

View this table:
  • View inline
  • View popup
Table 3

Monoamine tissue levels in DAT/SERT knockout mice

Behavior of Combined DAT and SERT Knockout Mice.

(i) Baseline behavioral assessments.

Combined DAT/SERT knockout mice lack gross behavioral deficits not already described for single knockouts. Locomotor activity in a novel environment is elevated in DAT−/− SERT+/+ mice, as previously described (9, 42). DAT−/− SERT−/− mice are even more active (Fig. 2). Both single and combined knockout mice habituate to the novel environment over at least the first 4 of the 5 hours of habituation testing (Fig. 2; see also refs. 9 and 42). Mice of each of these genotypes do not differ significantly from wild-type animals in tests of muscle tone, screen hang time, or ability to stay on a rotating rod. Although SERT−/− mice display enhanced anxiety responses as previously reported, SERT−/− DAT−/− animals display no further enhancements. Double knockout mice can also learn a passive avoidance task as readily as wild-type littermates.

Figure 2
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 2

Locomotion in DAT/SERT double knockout mice during a habituation period and after 10 mg/kg of cocaine. Locomotor activities were recorded for 3-h periods in mice of several genotypes (n, 11–33). Mice were placed into an activity monitor cage, to which they had not been previously exposed, 3 h before injection of cocaine (10 mg/kg s.c.) (time 0, arrow). DAT homozygous knockout mice of any SERT genotype show greater locomotor activity when placed in a novel environment, but no incremental locomotor response to cocaine. *, P < 0.05 vs. wild-type littermate. +, P < 0.05 vs. DAT−/− SERT+/+ littermate; #, P < 0.05 vs. predrug value.

(ii) Cocaine: Locomotor and rewarding effects.

Because screening tests failed to identify double knockout features that would obviously invalidate results of further behavioral tests, mice of each genotype were tested for cocaine influences on locomotion and reward/reinforcement. Cocaine did not enhance locomotor activity in DAT−/− knockout mice habituated to a novel environment for 3 h, as previously reported (Fig. 2) (9, 42). Although evaluation of drug-induced locomotor increases is likely to be less sensitive in mice that exhibit altered baseline locomotion, deletions of either one or two copies of the SERT gene in mice that lacked DAT expression failed to complement or reverse this DAT knockout effect. DAT−/− mice that received cocaine continued to display habituation to the test apparatus and displayed no enhanced locomotor activity whether two, one, or no SERT gene copies were present.

To assess reward/reinforcement, we tested cocaine-conditioned place preference (9). This test provides a technically tractable and robust measure of drug reward in mice (49). It has been validated by its ability to detect the rewarding properties of virtually every class of substance abused by humans. It allows mice to express their drug preference 24 h after the last drug administration, when acute cocaine effects on motor performance are less likely to confound results (7, 8, 50). The failures of DAT−/− or SERT−/− single gene knockouts to decrease this measure of cocaine reward/reinforcement have been previously documented (9).

Pairing 10 mg/kg of cocaine with one side of a test apparatus reliably increased the time spent in this drug-paired environment in DAT−/− and in SERT−/− single gene knockout mice. Both of these strains thus show strong cocaine place preferences, as previously reported (9) (Fig. 3A). Even DAT+/− SERT−/− mice that express no SERT and only half of wild-type levels of DAT retain near-wild-type levels preference for cocaine-paired environments. However, neither DAT−/− SERT+/− nor DAT−/− SERT−/− mice exhibited any significant preference for the cocaine-paired environments. Removing all of one cocaine target, DAT, and either 50% or all of another cocaine target, SERT, eliminates cocaine reward. Dose-effect studies provide no clearcut evidence for leftward shifts in cocaine dose–response relationships in double knockouts compared with the DAT−/− mice (Fig. 3B).

Figure 3
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 3

Lack of cocaine-conditioned place preference in DAT knockout mice with no or one copy of the SERT gene. (Upper) Conditioned place preference induced by 10 mg/kg of cocaine in mice of several genotypes (n, 8–56). Time scores shown represent differences between post- and preconditioning time spent in the cocaine-paired environment. DAT knockout mice with no or one copy of SERT displayed no preference for the places associated with 10 mg/kg of cocaine. *, P < 0.05 vs. saline. Average initial preference for the nonpreferred side for all groups was between 37 and 45%. (Lower) Conditioned place preference induced by 10- and 20-mg/kg of cocaine doses in mice of several genotypes (n, 8–56). Time scores shown represent differences between post- and preconditioning time spent in the cocaine-paired environment. Wild-type mice and SERT knockout mice with one copy of DAT displayed significant preferences for the place associated with either 10 or 20 mg/kg of cocaine, but double knockout mice did not have a significant preference for places associated with either dose. *, P < 0.05 vs. saline.

Discussion

The simplest hypotheses that explain the current data are that: (i) cocaine may normally work to provide rewarding actions at both DAT and SERT, and/or (ii) either DAT or SERT can mediate cocaine reward in the lifelong absence of the other transporter. Neurochemical rearrangements in single and double knockout mice demonstrate compensations for lifelong transporter deletion and might even follow long-term DAT or SERT blockade by drugs. However, double knockout mice that lack cocaine reward show few knockout influences on brain neurochemistry not found in the single knockout mice that retain cocaine reward. Retained cocaine reward in mice with as few as one DAT copy in the absence of SERT contrasts with the requirement for two SERT copies in the absence of DAT appears to indicate that fewer intact dopaminergic than serotonergic mechanisms are required for cocaine place preference.

Could knockout mouse data apply to cocaine reinforcement and reward in wild-type mice? The failure of single transporter gene knockouts to eliminate cocaine reinforcement and reward left open several possible roles for DAT and SERT in cocaine reward/reinforcement in wild-type mice (9), including the small possibility that previously known or novel nontransporter effects might mediate cocaine reward/reinforcement. The present results weigh strongly, to our knowledge for the first time, against this idea, and renew focus on transporter-based cocaine mechanisms. The most parsimonious explanation for the current data is that normal redundancy of transporter-related brain reward/reinforcement systems and/or adaptations to chronic deletions of single transporters render chronic ablation of both DAT and SERT necessary to completely block the cocaine reward/reinforcement that can be assessed by conditioned place preferences.

Adaptations to chronic DAT loss could alter cocaine's impact on serotonin systems important for rewarding or aversive properties. We have recently found that the SERT-specific blocker fluoxetine gains rewarding/reinforcing properties in DAT−/− SERT+/+ mice that are never found in wild-type animals (30). Previously available evidence has also fit the idea that removing both DAT and SERT might be required to block cocaine reward/reinforcement. Pharmacological studies support the idea that serotonin could normally interact with dopamine systems in ways important for psychostimulant reward/reinforcement. Agonists and antagonists selective for the serotonin 5-HT1B receptor modulate the reward/reinforcement induced by acute administration of DAT blockers (23, 51–54). Cocaine reinforcement differs between 5-HT1B knockout and wild-type mice (22). Serotonin can potently modulate dopamine levels in brain circuits associated with reward/reinforcement, such as the mesocortical/mesolimbic circuits projecting from the ventral tegmental area to the prefrontal cortex and nucleus accumbens (55–57). Serotonin systems could thus interact with dopamine systems in contributing to cocaine reward/reinforcement.

Cocaine has been reported to lose its ability to elevate dopamine efflux in DAT knockout mice (10). However, analyses of previous data and preliminary microdialysis studies in our mice demonstrate attenuated rather than absent effects (I.S., F.S.H., and G.R.U., unpublished observations). Our mouse strains that display cocaine reward/reinforcement, and those that do not, fail to display clearcut evidence that cocaine actions at SERT sustain cocaine reward/reinforcement in DAT knockouts simply by mimicking the increased dopamine overflow normally found after cocaine administration to wild-type mice.

How could deletion of SERT alone produce trends toward greater cocaine place preference, whereas double deletions of SERT and DAT ablate cocaine reward/reinforcement? These observations could fit with hypotheses that cocaine-elevated serotonin levels stimulate serotonin receptor subtypes in circuits that produce both reinforcing and aversive affects. Differential adaptations in these serotonin circuits could alter the overall balance of cocaine reward and aversive features in different knockout combinations. If blockade of NET and/or SERT in wild-type animals contributes to aversive cocaine effects, the enhanced cocaine reinforcement reported in NET knockout or SERT knockout mice, and even greater enhancements in NET/SERT double knockouts, makes sense (F.S.H., I.S., and G.R.U., unpublished work). Rearrangements in these systems in DAT knockout mice, clearly supported by their dramatically different responses to fluoxetine (30), could help preserve cocaine reward/reinforcement by altering the net balance of aversive and reinforcing effects of cocaine on serotonin circuits.

The effects of transporter gene copy numbers on cocaine place preference appear to indicate a greater role for DAT than SERT in cocaine reward/reinforcement in wild-type mice, consistent with previous pharmacological suggestions. Deleting two DAT copies and one SERT gene copy ablates cocaine preference just as effectively as deletion of both copies of both genes. By contrast, DAT+/− SERT−/− knockout mice that have a single wild-type DAT gene copy retain near-maximal cocaine reward/reinforcement and display no evidence for different cocaine dose-effect relationships. We are currently examining the drug-dose relationships in DAT−/− SERT+/− mice. If similar gene-dose relationships obtain in pharmacological interventions for human cocaine therapy, near-complete actions at DAT, accompanied by even subtotal actions at SERT, might provide an efficacious transporter-based strategy for reducing cocaine reward/reinforcement.

The present results suggest that medication strategies using drugs acting at both dopamine and serotonin brain systems might provide one minimal set of activities necessary to effectively combat cocaine addiction. Such drugs might be found. The sequence homologies between DAT and SERT (16) and work from structure/function studies that identify DAT domains selectively involved in cocaine recognition (58, 59) both support the idea that a single compound might provide uptake-sparing cocaine blockade at both transporters and a transporter-based anticocaine therapeutic. A second strategy could be based on involvement of selected dopamine and serotonin receptor subtypes. Drugs acting at both serotonin and dopamine receptor subtypes might represent a receptor-based medications development strategy targeting cocaine reward/reinforcement.

Acknowledgments

We are grateful to Kaori Itokawa, Nobue Kitanaka, Naraja Karmacharya, Nancy Goodman, Ira Baum, Qing-Rong Liu, David Huso, and the Charles River/Triad animal care staff. We also thank the National Institute on Drug Abuse, the National Institute of Mental Health, the Bundesministerium für Bildung, Wissenschaft, Forschung und Technologie, the Ministry of Education, Science, Sports, and Culture (Japan), and the Ministry of Health and Welfare (Japan). G.R.U. is grateful for his affiliation with the Departments of Neurology and Neuroscience, Johns Hopkins School of Medicine.

Footnotes

    • ↵¶ To whom reprint requests should be addressed at: Molecular Neurobiology, Box 5180, Baltimore, MD 21224. E-mail: guhl{at}intra.nida.nih.gov.

    • This paper was submitted directly (Track II) to the PNAS office.

    Abbreviations

    DAT,
    dopamine transporter;
    SERT,
    serotonin transporter;
    NET,
    norepinephrine transporter;
    HVA,
    homovanillic acid;
    DOPAC,
    3,4-dihydroxyphenylacetic acid
    • Received January 24, 2001.
    • Copyright © 2001, The National Academy of Sciences

    References

    1. ↵
      1. Substance Abuse and Mental Health Services Administration
      (1999) National Household Survey on Drug Abuse: Main Findings 1999 (U.S. Department of Health and Human Services, Rockville, MD).
    2. ↵
      1. Caroll F I,
      2. Lewis A H
      (1994) Pharmaceutical News 1:11–17.
      OpenUrl
    3. ↵
      1. Kuhar M J,
      2. Ritz M C,
      3. Boja J W
      (1991) Trends Neurosci 14:299–302, pmid:1719677.
      OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
    4. ↵
      1. Woolverton W L,
      2. Johnson K M
      (1992) Trends Pharmacol Sci 13:193–200, pmid:1604712.
      OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
    5. ↵
      1. Spealman R D,
      2. Madras B K,
      3. Bergman J
      (1989) J Pharmacol Exp Ther 251:142–149, pmid:2795456.
      OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
    6. ↵
      1. Di Chiara G,
      2. Imperato A
      (1988) Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 85:5274–5278, pmid:2899326.
      OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
    7. ↵
      1. Pettit H O,
      2. Ettenberg A,
      3. Bloom F E,
      4. Koob G F
      (1984) Psychopharmacology 84:167–173, pmid:6438676.
      OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
    8. ↵
      1. Roberts D C,
      2. Corcoran M E,
      3. Fibiger H C
      (1977) Pharmacol Biochem Behav 6:615–620, pmid:122445.
      OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
    9. ↵
      1. Sora I,
      2. Wichems C,
      3. Takahashi N,
      4. Li X F,
      5. Zeng Z,
      6. Revay R,
      7. Lesch K P,
      8. Murphy D L,
      9. Uhl G R
      (1998) Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 95:7699–7704, pmid:9636213.
      OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
    10. ↵
      1. Rocha B A,
      2. Fumagalli F,
      3. Gainetdinov R R,
      4. Jones S R,
      5. Ator R,
      6. Giros B,
      7. Miller G W,
      8. Caron M G
      (1998) Nat Neurosci 1:132–137, pmid:10195128.
      OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
    11. ↵
      1. Chait L D,
      2. Uhlenhuth E H,
      3. Johanson C E
      (1987) J Pharmacol Exp Ther 242:777–783, pmid:3656112.
      OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
      1. Wilson A W,
      2. Neill J C,
      3. Costall B
      (1998) Alcohol 16:249–270, pmid:9744857.
      OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
    12. ↵
      1. Yanagita T,
      2. Katoh S,
      3. Wakasa Y,
      4. Oinuma N
      (1982) National Institute on Drug Abuse Res Monogr 41:208–214.
      OpenUrl
    13. ↵
      1. Javitch J A,
      2. Blaustein R O,
      3. Snyder S H
      (1984) Mol Pharmacol 26:35–44, pmid:6087116.
      OpenUrlAbstract
      1. Cooper J R,
      2. Bloom F E,
      3. Roth R H
      (1991) The Biochemical Basis of Neuropharmacology (Oxford Univ. Press, New York).
    14. ↵
      1. Uhl G R,
      2. Hartig P R
      (1992) Trends Pharmacol Sci 13:421–425, pmid:1293865.
      OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
    15. ↵
      1. Miliaressis E
      (1977) Pharmacol Biochem Behav 7:177–180, pmid:144276.
      OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
      1. Poschel B P
      (1974) Psychopharmacol Bull 10:46–47, pmid:4419439.
      OpenUrlPubMed
      1. Redgrave P
      (1978) Brain Res 155:277–295, pmid:688018.
      OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
    16. ↵
      1. Harrison A A,
      2. Parsons L H,
      3. Koob G F,
      4. Markou A
      (1999) Psychopharmacology (Berlin) 141:242–250, pmid:10027505.
      OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
    17. ↵
      1. Fletcher P J,
      2. Tampakeras M,
      3. Yeomans J S
      (1995) Pharmacol Biochem Behav 52:65–71, pmid:7501680.
      OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
    18. ↵
      1. Rocha B A,
      2. Scearce-Levie K,
      3. Lucas J J,
      4. Hiroi N,
      5. Castanon N,
      6. Crabbe J C,
      7. Nestler E J,
      8. Hen R
      (1998) Nature (London) 393:175–178, pmid:9603521.
      OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
    19. ↵
      1. Fletcher P J,
      2. Korth K M
      (1999) Psychopharmacology (Berlin) 142:165–174, pmid:10102769.
      OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
    20. ↵
      1. Tran-Nguyen L T,
      2. Baker D A,
      3. Grote K A,
      4. Solano J,
      5. Neisewander J L
      (1999) Psychopharmacology (Berlin) 146:60–66, pmid:10485965.
      OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
    21. ↵
      1. Nomikos G G,
      2. Spyraki C
      (1988) Pharmacol Biochem Behav 30:853–858, pmid:3147460.
      OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
    22. ↵
      1. Papp M,
      2. Willner P
      (1991) Psychopharmacology 103:99–102, pmid:1826060.
      OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
    23. ↵
      1. Frank R A,
      2. Zubrycki E
      (1989) Pharmacol Biochem Behav 33:725–727, pmid:2587615.
      OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
    24. ↵
      1. Tella S R
      (1995) Pharmacol Biochem Behav 51:687–692, pmid:7675844.
      OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
    25. ↵
      1. Xu F,
      2. Gainetdinov R R,
      3. Wetsel W C,
      4. Jones S R,
      5. Bohn L M,
      6. Miller G W,
      7. Wang Y M,
      8. Caron M G
      (2000) Nat Neurosci 3:465–471, pmid:10769386.
      OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
    26. ↵
      1. Hall F S,
      2. Sora I,
      3. Li X F,
      4. Uhl G
      (2000) Soc. Neurosci. Abstr..
    27. ↵
      1. Uhl G R,
      2. Vandenbergh D J,
      3. Miner L L
      (1996) Curr Biol 6:935–936, pmid:8805320.
      OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
      1. Lakoski J M,
      2. Golloway M P,
      3. White F J
      1. Reith M E A
      (1991) in Cocaine: Pharmacology, Physiology, and Clinical Strategies, eds Lakoski J M, Golloway M P, White F J(CRC Press, Boca Raton), pp 203–227.
    28. ↵
      1. Ritz M C,
      2. Cone E J,
      3. Kuhar M J
      (1990) Life Sci 46:635–645, pmid:2308472.
      OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
    29. ↵
      1. Roberts D C,
      2. Phelan R,
      3. Hodges L M,
      4. Hodges M M,
      5. Bennett B,
      6. Childers S,
      7. Davies H
      (1999) Psychopharmacology (Berlin) 144:389–397, pmid:10435412.
      OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
    30. ↵
      1. Andrews A M,
      2. Murphy D L
      (1993) J Neurochem 60:1167–1170, pmid:8094744.
      OpenUrlPubMed
    31. ↵
      1. Boja J W,
      2. Carroll F I,
      3. Rahman M A,
      4. Philip A,
      5. Lewin A H,
      6. Kuhar M J
      (1990) Eur J Pharmacol 184:329–332, pmid:2079102.
      OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
    32. ↵
      1. Marcusson J O,
      2. Bergstrom M,
      3. Eriksson K,
      4. Ross S B
      (1988) J Neurochem 50:1783–1790, pmid:2967349.
      OpenUrlPubMed
    33. ↵
      1. Tejani-Butt S M,
      2. Brunswick D J,
      3. Frazer A
      (1990) Eur J Pharmacol 191:239–243, pmid:2086242.
      OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
    34. ↵
      1. Terai M,
      2. Hidaka K,
      3. Nakamura Y
      (1989) Eur J Pharmacol 173:177–182, pmid:2576228.
      OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
    35. ↵
      1. Boujrad F,
      2. Dauphin F,
      3. de Beaurepaire R
      (1998) Brain Res 812:279–282, pmid:9813368.
      OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
    36. ↵
      1. Bolanos-Jimenez F,
      2. Manhaes de Castro R M,
      3. Seguin L,
      4. Cloez-Tayarani I,
      5. Monneret V,
      6. Drieu K,
      7. Fillion G
      (1995) Eur J Pharmacol 294:531–540, pmid:8750715.
      OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
    37. ↵
      1. Giros B,
      2. Jaber M,
      3. Jones S R,
      4. Wightman R M,
      5. Caron M G
      (1996) Nature (London) 379:606–612, pmid:8628395.
      OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
    38. ↵
      1. Bengel D,
      2. Murphy D L,
      3. Andrews A M,
      4. Wichems C H,
      5. Feltner D,
      6. Heils A,
      7. Mossner R,
      8. Westphal H,
      9. Lesch K P
      (1998) Mol Pharmacol 53:649–655, pmid:9547354.
      OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
    39. ↵
      1. Bosse R,
      2. Fumagalli F,
      3. Jaber M,
      4. Giros B,
      5. Gainetdinov R R,
      6. Wetsel W C,
      7. Missale C,
      8. Caron M G
      (1997) Neuron 19:127–138, pmid:9247269.
      OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
    40. ↵
      1. Maldonado R,
      2. Saiardi A,
      3. Valverde O,
      4. Samad T A,
      5. Roques B P,
      6. Borrelli E
      (1997) Nature (London) 388:586–589, pmid:9252189.
      OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
      1. Pert A
      (1998) Adv Pharmacol 42:991–995, pmid:9328064.
      OpenUrlPubMed
      1. Phillips T J,
      2. Brown K J,
      3. Burkhart-Kasch S,
      4. Wenger C D,
      5. Kelly M A,
      6. Rubinstein M,
      7. Grandy D K,
      8. Low M J
      (1998) Nat Neurosci 1:610–615, pmid:10196569.
      OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
    41. ↵
      1. Noble E P
      (2000) Eur J Psychiatry 15:79–89.
      OpenUrl
    42. ↵
      1. Tzschentke T M
      (1998) Prog Neurobiol 56:613–672, pmid:9871940.
      OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
    43. ↵
      1. Roberts D C,
      2. Koob G F
      (1982) Pharmacol Biochem Behav 17:901–904, pmid:6817350.
      OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
    44. ↵
      1. Parsons L H,
      2. Koob G F,
      3. Weiss F
      (1999) Synapse 32:132–135, pmid:10231132.
      OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
      1. Parsons L H,
      2. Weiss F,
      3. Koob G F
      (1996) Psychopharmacology (Berlin) 128:150–160, pmid:8956376.
      OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
      1. Parsons L H,
      2. Weiss F,
      3. Koob G F
      (1998) J Neurosci 18:10078–10089, pmid:9822762.
      OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
    45. ↵
      1. Calcagnetti D J,
      2. Keck B J,
      3. Quatrella L A,
      4. Schechter M D
      (1995) Life Sci 56:475–483, pmid:7869827.
      OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
    46. ↵
      1. Ichikawa J,
      2. Meltzer H Y
      (1999) Brain Res 842:445–451, pmid:10526141.
      OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
      1. De Deurwaerdere P,
      2. Spampinato U
      (1999) J Neurochem 73:1033–1042, pmid:10461892.
      OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
    47. ↵
      1. Lorrain D S,
      2. Riolo J V,
      3. Matuszewich L,
      4. Hull E M
      (1999) J Neurosci 19:7648–7652, pmid:10460270.
      OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
    48. ↵
      1. Kitayama S,
      2. Wang J B,
      3. Uhl G R
      (1993) Synapse 15:58–62, pmid:8310426.
      OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
    49. ↵
      1. Lin Z,
      2. Wang W,
      3. Kopajtic T,
      4. Revay R S,
      5. Uhl G R
      (1999) Mol Pharmacol 56:434–447, pmid:10419565.
      OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
    PreviousNext
    Back to top
    Article Alerts
    Email Article

    Thank you for your interest in spreading the word on PNAS.

    NOTE: We only request your email address so that the person you are recommending the page to knows that you wanted them to see it, and that it is not junk mail. We do not capture any email address.

    Enter multiple addresses on separate lines or separate them with commas.
    Molecular mechanisms of cocaine reward: Combined dopamine and serotonin transporter knockouts eliminate cocaine place preference
    (Your Name) has sent you a message from PNAS
    (Your Name) thought you would like to see the PNAS web site.
    CAPTCHA
    This question is for testing whether or not you are a human visitor and to prevent automated spam submissions.
    Citation Tools
    Molecular mechanisms of cocaine reward: Combined dopamine and serotonin transporter knockouts eliminate cocaine place preference
    Ichiro Sora, F. Scott Hall, Anne M. Andrews, Masanari Itokawa, Xiao-Fei Li, Hong-Bing Wei, Christine Wichems, Klaus-Peter Lesch, Dennis L. Murphy, George R. Uhl
    Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences Apr 2001, 98 (9) 5300-5305; DOI: 10.1073/pnas.091039298

    Citation Manager Formats

    • BibTeX
    • Bookends
    • EasyBib
    • EndNote (tagged)
    • EndNote 8 (xml)
    • Medlars
    • Mendeley
    • Papers
    • RefWorks Tagged
    • Ref Manager
    • RIS
    • Zotero
    Request Permissions
    Share
    Molecular mechanisms of cocaine reward: Combined dopamine and serotonin transporter knockouts eliminate cocaine place preference
    Ichiro Sora, F. Scott Hall, Anne M. Andrews, Masanari Itokawa, Xiao-Fei Li, Hong-Bing Wei, Christine Wichems, Klaus-Peter Lesch, Dennis L. Murphy, George R. Uhl
    Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences Apr 2001, 98 (9) 5300-5305; DOI: 10.1073/pnas.091039298
    del.icio.us logo Digg logo Reddit logo Twitter logo CiteULike logo Facebook logo Google logo Mendeley logo
    • Tweet Widget
    • Facebook Like
    • Mendeley logo Mendeley
    Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences: 98 (9)
    Table of Contents

    Submit

    Sign up for Article Alerts

    Jump to section

    • Article
      • Abstract
      • Materials and Methods
      • Results
      • Discussion
      • Acknowledgments
      • Footnotes
      • Abbreviations
      • References
    • Figures & SI
    • Info & Metrics
    • PDF

    You May Also be Interested in

    Setting sun over a sun-baked dirt landscape
    Core Concept: Popular integrated assessment climate policy models have key caveats
    Better explicating the strengths and shortcomings of these models will help refine projections and improve transparency in the years ahead.
    Image credit: Witsawat.S.
    Model of the Amazon forest
    News Feature: A sea in the Amazon
    Did the Caribbean sweep into the western Amazon millions of years ago, shaping the region’s rich biodiversity?
    Image credit: Tacio Cordeiro Bicudo (University of São Paulo, São Paulo, Brazil), Victor Sacek (University of São Paulo, São Paulo, Brazil), and Lucy Reading-Ikkanda (artist).
    Syrian archaeological site
    Journal Club: In Mesopotamia, early cities may have faltered before climate-driven collapse
    Settlements 4,200 years ago may have suffered from overpopulation before drought and lower temperatures ultimately made them unsustainable.
    Image credit: Andrea Ricci.
    Steamboat Geyser eruption.
    Eruption of Steamboat Geyser
    Mara Reed and Michael Manga explore why Yellowstone's Steamboat Geyser resumed erupting in 2018.
    Listen
    Past PodcastsSubscribe
    Birds nestling on tree branches
    Parent–offspring conflict in songbird fledging
    Some songbird parents might improve their own fitness by manipulating their offspring into leaving the nest early, at the cost of fledgling survival, a study finds.
    Image credit: Gil Eckrich (photographer).

    Similar Articles

    Site Logo
    Powered by HighWire
    • Submit Manuscript
    • Twitter
    • Facebook
    • RSS Feeds
    • Email Alerts

    Articles

    • Current Issue
    • Special Feature Articles – Most Recent
    • List of Issues

    PNAS Portals

    • Anthropology
    • Chemistry
    • Classics
    • Front Matter
    • Physics
    • Sustainability Science
    • Teaching Resources

    Information

    • Authors
    • Editorial Board
    • Reviewers
    • Subscribers
    • Librarians
    • Press
    • Site Map
    • PNAS Updates
    • FAQs
    • Accessibility Statement
    • Rights & Permissions
    • About
    • Contact

    Feedback    Privacy/Legal

    Copyright © 2021 National Academy of Sciences. Online ISSN 1091-6490