Skip to main content

Main menu

  • Home
  • Articles
    • Current
    • Special Feature Articles - Most Recent
    • Special Features
    • Colloquia
    • Collected Articles
    • PNAS Classics
    • List of Issues
  • Front Matter
    • Front Matter Portal
    • Journal Club
  • News
    • For the Press
    • This Week In PNAS
    • PNAS in the News
  • Podcasts
  • Authors
    • Information for Authors
    • Editorial and Journal Policies
    • Submission Procedures
    • Fees and Licenses
  • Submit
  • Submit
  • About
    • Editorial Board
    • PNAS Staff
    • FAQ
    • Accessibility Statement
    • Rights and Permissions
    • Site Map
  • Contact
  • Journal Club
  • Subscribe
    • Subscription Rates
    • Subscriptions FAQ
    • Open Access
    • Recommend PNAS to Your Librarian

User menu

  • Log in
  • My Cart

Search

  • Advanced search
Home
Home
  • Log in
  • My Cart

Advanced Search

  • Home
  • Articles
    • Current
    • Special Feature Articles - Most Recent
    • Special Features
    • Colloquia
    • Collected Articles
    • PNAS Classics
    • List of Issues
  • Front Matter
    • Front Matter Portal
    • Journal Club
  • News
    • For the Press
    • This Week In PNAS
    • PNAS in the News
  • Podcasts
  • Authors
    • Information for Authors
    • Editorial and Journal Policies
    • Submission Procedures
    • Fees and Licenses
  • Submit
Research Article

The habenula encodes negative motivational value associated with primary punishment in humans

Rebecca P. Lawson, Ben Seymour, Eleanor Loh, Antoine Lutti, Raymond J. Dolan, Peter Dayan, Nikolaus Weiskopf, and Jonathan P. Roiser
  1. aInstitute of Cognitive Neuroscience, University College London, London WC1N 3AR, United Kingdom;
  2. bWellcome Trust Centre for Neuroimaging, University College London, London WC1N 3BG, United Kingdom;
  3. cComputational and Biological Learning Laboratory, Department of Engineering, University of Cambridge, Cambridge CB2 1PZ, United Kingdom;
  4. dCenter for Information and Neural Networks, National Institute for Information and Communications Technology, Osaka 565-0871, Japan;
  5. eLaboratoire de Recherche en Neuroimagerie, Département des Neurosciences Cliniques, Centre Hospitalier Universitaire Vaudois, Université de Lausanne, 1011 Lausanne, Switzerland; and
  6. fGatsby Computational Neuroscience Unit, University College London, London WC1N 3AR, United Kingdom

See allHide authors and affiliations

PNAS first published July 28, 2014; https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1323586111
Rebecca P. Lawson
aInstitute of Cognitive Neuroscience, University College London, London WC1N 3AR, United Kingdom;
bWellcome Trust Centre for Neuroimaging, University College London, London WC1N 3BG, United Kingdom;
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
  • For correspondence: rebecca.lawson@ucl.ac.uk j.roiser@ucl.ac.uk
Ben Seymour
cComputational and Biological Learning Laboratory, Department of Engineering, University of Cambridge, Cambridge CB2 1PZ, United Kingdom;
dCenter for Information and Neural Networks, National Institute for Information and Communications Technology, Osaka 565-0871, Japan;
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Eleanor Loh
bWellcome Trust Centre for Neuroimaging, University College London, London WC1N 3BG, United Kingdom;
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Antoine Lutti
bWellcome Trust Centre for Neuroimaging, University College London, London WC1N 3BG, United Kingdom;
eLaboratoire de Recherche en Neuroimagerie, Département des Neurosciences Cliniques, Centre Hospitalier Universitaire Vaudois, Université de Lausanne, 1011 Lausanne, Switzerland; and
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Raymond J. Dolan
bWellcome Trust Centre for Neuroimaging, University College London, London WC1N 3BG, United Kingdom;
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Peter Dayan
fGatsby Computational Neuroscience Unit, University College London, London WC1N 3AR, United Kingdom
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Nikolaus Weiskopf
bWellcome Trust Centre for Neuroimaging, University College London, London WC1N 3BG, United Kingdom;
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Jonathan P. Roiser
aInstitute of Cognitive Neuroscience, University College London, London WC1N 3AR, United Kingdom;
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
  • For correspondence: rebecca.lawson@ucl.ac.uk j.roiser@ucl.ac.uk
  1. Edited by John P. O’Doherty, California Institute of Technology, Pasadena, CA, and accepted by the Editorial Board June 24, 2014 (received for review December 18, 2013)

  • Article
  • Figures & SI
  • Info & Metrics
  • PDF
Loading

Significance

Organisms must learn adaptively about environmental cue–outcome associations to survive. Studies in nonhuman primates suggest that a small phylogenetically conserved brain structure, the habenula, encodes the values of cues previously paired with aversive outcomes. However, such a role for the habenula has never been demonstrated in humans. We establish that the habenula encodes associations with aversive outcomes in humans, specifically that it tracks the dynamically changing negative values of cues paired with painful electric shocks, consistent with a role in learning. Importantly, habenula responses predicted the extent to which individuals withdrew from or approached negative and positive cues, respectively. These results suggest that the habenula plays a central role in driving aversively motivated learning and behavior in humans.

Abstract

Learning what to approach, and what to avoid, involves assigning value to environmental cues that predict positive and negative events. Studies in animals indicate that the lateral habenula encodes the previously learned negative motivational value of stimuli. However, involvement of the habenula in dynamic trial-by-trial aversive learning has not been assessed, and the functional role of this structure in humans remains poorly characterized, in part, due to its small size. Using high-resolution functional neuroimaging and computational modeling of reinforcement learning, we demonstrate positive habenula responses to the dynamically changing values of cues signaling painful electric shocks, which predict behavioral suppression of responses to those cues across individuals. By contrast, negative habenula responses to monetary reward cue values predict behavioral invigoration. Our findings show that the habenula plays a key role in an online aversive learning system and in generating associated motivated behavior in humans.

  • high-resolution fMRI
  • conditioned behavior
  • pallidum

Footnotes

  • ↵1To whom correspondence may be addressed. Email: rebecca.lawson{at}ucl.ac.uk or j.roiser{at}ucl.ac.uk.
  • Author contributions: R.P.L., B.S., R.J.D., P.D., and J.P.R. designed research; R.P.L. and E.L. performed research; R.P.L. and B.S. analyzed data; R.P.L., B.S., R.J.D., P.D., N.W., and J.P.R. wrote the paper; A.L. and N.W. developed imaging methods; and J.P.R. conceived the study.

  • The authors declare no conflict of interest.

  • This article is a PNAS Direct Submission. J.P.O. is a guest editor invited by the Editorial Board.

  • This article contains supporting information online at www.pnas.org/lookup/suppl/doi:10.1073/pnas.1323586111/-/DCSupplemental.

Freely available online through the PNAS open access option.

Next
Back to top
Article Alerts
Email Article

Thank you for your interest in spreading the word on PNAS.

NOTE: We only request your email address so that the person you are recommending the page to knows that you wanted them to see it, and that it is not junk mail. We do not capture any email address.

Enter multiple addresses on separate lines or separate them with commas.
The habenula encodes negative motivational value associated with primary punishment in humans
(Your Name) has sent you a message from PNAS
(Your Name) thought you would like to see the PNAS web site.
CAPTCHA
This question is for testing whether or not you are a human visitor and to prevent automated spam submissions.
Citation Tools
Habenula encodes negative motivational value
Rebecca P. Lawson, Ben Seymour, Eleanor Loh, Antoine Lutti, Raymond J. Dolan, Peter Dayan, Nikolaus Weiskopf, Jonathan P. Roiser
Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences Jul 2014, 201323586; DOI: 10.1073/pnas.1323586111

Citation Manager Formats

  • BibTeX
  • Bookends
  • EasyBib
  • EndNote (tagged)
  • EndNote 8 (xml)
  • Medlars
  • Mendeley
  • Papers
  • RefWorks Tagged
  • Ref Manager
  • RIS
  • Zotero
Request Permissions
Share
Habenula encodes negative motivational value
Rebecca P. Lawson, Ben Seymour, Eleanor Loh, Antoine Lutti, Raymond J. Dolan, Peter Dayan, Nikolaus Weiskopf, Jonathan P. Roiser
Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences Jul 2014, 201323586; DOI: 10.1073/pnas.1323586111
del.icio.us logo Digg logo Reddit logo Twitter logo CiteULike logo Facebook logo Google logo Mendeley logo
  • Tweet Widget
  • Facebook Like
  • Mendeley logo Mendeley
Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences: 118 (15)
Current Issue

Submit

Sign up for Article Alerts

Jump to section

  • Article
  • Figures & SI
  • Info & Metrics
  • PDF

You May Also be Interested in

Smoke emanates from Japan’s Fukushima nuclear power plant a few days after tsunami damage
Core Concept: Muography offers a new way to see inside a multitude of objects
Muons penetrate much further than X-rays, they do essentially zero damage, and they are provided for free by the cosmos.
Image credit: Science Source/Digital Globe.
Water from a faucet fills a glass.
News Feature: How “forever chemicals” might impair the immune system
Researchers are exploring whether these ubiquitous fluorinated molecules might worsen infections or hamper vaccine effectiveness.
Image credit: Shutterstock/Dmitry Naumov.
Venus flytrap captures a fly.
Journal Club: Venus flytrap mechanism could shed light on how plants sense touch
One protein seems to play a key role in touch sensitivity for flytraps and other meat-eating plants.
Image credit: Shutterstock/Kuttelvaserova Stuchelova.
Illustration of groups of people chatting
Exploring the length of human conversations
Adam Mastroianni and Daniel Gilbert explore why conversations almost never end when people want them to.
Listen
Past PodcastsSubscribe
Panda bear hanging in a tree
How horse manure helps giant pandas tolerate cold
A study finds that giant pandas roll in horse manure to increase their cold tolerance.
Image credit: Fuwen Wei.

Similar Articles

Site Logo
Powered by HighWire
  • Submit Manuscript
  • Twitter
  • Facebook
  • RSS Feeds
  • Email Alerts

Articles

  • Current Issue
  • Special Feature Articles – Most Recent
  • List of Issues

PNAS Portals

  • Anthropology
  • Chemistry
  • Classics
  • Front Matter
  • Physics
  • Sustainability Science
  • Teaching Resources

Information

  • Authors
  • Editorial Board
  • Reviewers
  • Subscribers
  • Librarians
  • Press
  • Cozzarelli Prize
  • Site Map
  • PNAS Updates
  • FAQs
  • Accessibility Statement
  • Rights & Permissions
  • About
  • Contact

Feedback    Privacy/Legal

Copyright © 2021 National Academy of Sciences. Online ISSN 1091-6490