New Research In
Physical Sciences
Social Sciences
Featured Portals
Articles by Topic
Biological Sciences
Featured Portals
Articles by Topic
- Agricultural Sciences
- Anthropology
- Applied Biological Sciences
- Biochemistry
- Biophysics and Computational Biology
- Cell Biology
- Developmental Biology
- Ecology
- Environmental Sciences
- Evolution
- Genetics
- Immunology and Inflammation
- Medical Sciences
- Microbiology
- Neuroscience
- Pharmacology
- Physiology
- Plant Biology
- Population Biology
- Psychological and Cognitive Sciences
- Sustainability Science
- Systems Biology
Massive increase in visual range preceded the origin of terrestrial vertebrates
Edited by Neil H. Shubin, The University of Chicago, Chicago, IL, and approved January 24, 2017 (received for review September 17, 2016)

Significance
Starting 385 million years ago, certain fish slowly evolved into legged animals living on land. We show that eyes tripled in size and shifted from the sides to the top of the head long before fish modified their fins into limbs for land. Before permanent life on land, these animals probably hunted like crocodiles, looking at prey from just above the water line, where the vastly higher transparency of air enabled long-distance vision and selected for larger eyes. The “buena vista” hypothesis that our study forwards is that seeing opportunities far away provided an informational zip line to the bounty of invertebrate prey on land, aiding selection for limbs—first for brief forays onto land and eventually, for life there.
Abstract
The evolution of terrestrial vertebrates, starting around 385 million years ago, is an iconic moment in evolution that brings to mind images of fish transforming into four-legged animals. Here, we show that this radical change in body shape was preceded by an equally dramatic change in sensory abilities akin to transitioning from seeing over short distances in a dense fog to seeing over long distances on a clear day. Measurements of eye sockets and simulations of their evolution show that eyes nearly tripled in size just before vertebrates began living on land. Computational simulations of these animal’s visual ecology show that for viewing objects through water, the increase in eye size provided a negligible increase in performance. However, when viewing objects through air, the increase in eye size provided a large increase in performance. The jump in eye size was, therefore, unlikely to have arisen for seeing through water and instead points to an unexpected hybrid of seeing through air while still primarily inhabiting water. Our results and several anatomical innovations arising at the same time suggest lifestyle similarity to crocodiles. The consequent combination of the increase in eye size and vision through air would have conferred a 1 million-fold increase in the amount of space within which objects could be seen. The “buena vista” hypothesis that our data suggest is that seeing opportunities from afar played a role in the subsequent evolution of fully terrestrial limbs as well as the emergence of elaborated action sequences through planning circuits in the nervous system.
Before terrestrial vertebrates arose, their ancestors inhabited underwater environments, where vision is highly compromised compared with vision above water. The visual difference between life in water and life above it is comparable with driving fast on a foggy road, where our responses must be rapid and simple, vs. driving in clear daylight conditions, where deliberation over more complex choices is enabled by the vast increase in sensory range. Nonetheless, although an immense quantity of work has been done on the emergence of limbs during the evolution of land vertebrates, how visual capability changed during the transition from water to land has not been explored. In part, this lack of exploration is because computational visual ecology—necessary to interpret the fossil data—has not been combined with early tetrapod paleontology. Through combining these disciplines, here we probe the evolutionary history of the switch in our visual sensory ecology from water to air. Surprisingly, our results show that eyes tripled in size just before full-time life on land evolved. Convergent lines of evidence, including our own, strongly support the hypothesis that a crocodilian ecotype—using the greatly enhanced visual capabilities conferred by vision through air to prey on the bounty of unexploited invertebrates that long preceded the vertebrates onto land—was the gateway between vertebrate life underwater and on land.
Large Eyes Appeared Before Terrestriality
Fig. 1 shows the sequence of steps of our study across its parent disciplines. We start with the assembly of a time-calibrated phylogeny of 59 tetrapodomorph taxa that bracket the water–land transition and have measurable skull and eye socket lengths (Materials and Methods). We generated a set of 1,000 evolutionary trees to account for uncertainties in phylogeny. Using this tree distribution, we then apply a phylogenetic comparative approach to estimate whether there were changes in the selective regime governing the evolution of relative eye socket length (socket length corrected for size of animal inferred from skull length by a regression). A statistical approach incorporating phylogenetic information is needed, because the evolutionary relationships of the animals in our sample result in our observations of their eye socket sizes losing statistical independence (1, 2), disqualifying conventional significance tests that depend on this property. Specifically, we follow an approach guided by the Ornstein–Uhlenbeck (OU) process as a model of trait evolution (3, 4). We use a Bayesian variant of this method (5) to study the adaptive landscape of the relative eye socket sizes of early tetrapods (Materials and Methods).
The sequence of steps within early tetrapod paleontology as well as computational visual ecology used for generating the results of this study.
Analysis of the selective regime shifts within the tree distribution reveals a surprising finding: a change favoring larger eye sockets is most likely to have happened before the origin of the vertebrates with complete limbs including fingers and toes (hereafter “digited tetrapods”), in animals understood to be primarily aquatic (Fig. 2). This major shift in eye socket evolution occurred in a phase of fundamental reorganization of the tetrapod body plan. The transitional phase begins with the lineage leading to the last ancestor of the elpistostegalians (the finned transitional group in Fig. 2) and ends with the lineage connecting to the last ancestor of the very first digited tetrapods, Ventastega (inferred to have digits) and Acanthostega. During this phase of evolution, tetrapods entered a new adaptive zone that selected relative eye socket sizes that were a factor of 1.42–1.53 larger than the ancestral adaptive peak for fish, such as Eusthenopteron (Fig. 2). Fig. 3 shows the absolute and relative socket sizes for our dataset grouped according to the adaptive zones identified by the analysis of selective regime shifts. We refer to the group before the increase in eye socket size as “finned tetrapods” (6). As shown in Fig. 3, there was a near tripling in the mean absolute socket size between the finned and digited tetrapods. We will show below that the enlargement of eye sockets supports the inference that eyes also approximately tripled in size.
Evolution and adaptive landscape of relative eye socket size in early tetrapods summarizing our phylogenetic comparative study performed over a sample of 1,000 time-calibrated trees. The circles (red for finned, yellow for finned transitional, blue for digited, and brown for digited aquatic tetrapods) represent body size-corrected relative eye socket sizes, the residuals from phylogenetically generalized least squares regression (PGLS) of log10-transformed variables (Materials and Methods and SI Appendix, Scaling of Orbit and Skull Length in Early Tetrapods). The thick branches indicate positions of well-supported selective regime shifts, with associated factors signifying the change in eye socket size compared with the ancestral regime (before the green dot) after body size effects are accounted for.
Eye socket lengths across the taxa in Fig. 2 grouped by the regime shift analysis. (A) The mean (horizontal bars) absolute eye socket length of digited tetrapods was three times larger than that of their finned relatives, with the elpistostegalians (finned-transitional) midway. The digited tetrapods that returned to life underwater (adelospondyl-colosteids, digited-aquatic) reverted to a size similar to that of their finned relatives. (B) Relative eye socket size was calculated as residuals from a phylogenetically informed regression of log10-transformed variables (Materials and Methods) averaged over the full set of 1,000 trees. Positive residuals indicate eye sockets larger than expected based on skull length, whereas negative residuals indicate eye sockets smaller than expected. The Bayesian OU results in Fig. 2 show the presence of an adaptive evolutionary process and provide estimates of the adaptive peak for each group (horizontal bars). The elpistostegalians entered a new selective regime but are lagging behind, because time was insufficient to accrue enough increases in eye socket size to reach the peak.However, the digited tetrapods are centered around their adaptive peak, except for the adelospondyl-colosteids. As expected for adiverse group, not all tetrapods are at their respective peak, reflecting a normal evolutionary pattern in which trait values are dispersed around the optimal value. The Bayesian OU findings show that there must have been a selective benefit from larger eye sockets in finned transitional and digited tetrapods, but uncovering its basis requires modeling visual performance across likely environments.
Interestingly, there was a reversion to small eye sockets in a group of animals that subsequently specialized for life underwater. This group is termed the “adelospondyl-colosteids” (7) (Figs. 2 and 3). They have elongated snake-like bodies with tiny limbs (7, 8) and shrunken eye sockets similar to those of the finned tetrapods. Although there are other animals that are clearly semiaquatic within the digited tetrapods, the adelospondyl-colosteids are unique in the extent of aquatic specialization across the entire group and considered fully aquatic.
Our discovery that the evolution of larger eye sockets occurred in animals that were primarily aquatic is in line with other critical conclusions of the past several decades of early tetrapod paleontology, which has found that robust limbs evolved in primarily aquatic animals (9, 10) and that fingers and toes evolved in primarily aquatic animals (11). Notably, the increase in eye size starting in the elpistostegalians coincided with a distinct change in the placement of eyes in this group. Although placed laterally in the early phase of tetrapod evolution, similar to other fish, the eyes moved into a position on the top of the head in this group (Eusthenopteron compared with Tiktaalik in Fig. 2). They are on raised “eye brows,” low bony prominences on the top of the skull (12⇓–14).
Computational Visual Ecology
The adaptive landscape analysis in Fig. 2 shows the location and magnitude of changes in relative eye socket size but is insufficient for understanding their possible bases. Larger sockets strongly correlate with larger eyes as shown by data on fish (SI Appendix, Estimating Eye and Pupil Size in Early Tetrapods and Fig. S4), reptiles (15), birds (16), and primates (17). Evidence spanning such a broad bracket of vertebrates shows that eye socket size in our group of ancient animals reliably captures what their eye size would have been. We can, therefore, estimate that the nearly threefold increase of absolute eye socket size (Fig. 3) corresponds to an almost threefold increase in eye size. However, what are these large eyes good for? To better understand the significance of the increase in eye size, estimates of the functional consequences of these changes across environments that bracket the most likely possibilities (computational visual ecology) (Fig. 1) can be helpful.
Larger eyes are strongly correlated with larger pupils (SI Appendix, Estimating Eye and Pupil Size in Early Tetrapods and Fig. S4), a key variable in estimating visual capability. We selected four measures of visual function that we calculated as a function of pupil size: the distance at which a standard object, a 10-cm black disk, could be seen; the volume of space within which that same object could be seen given an estimate of the field of view; and the gains in both range and volume for a change in eye size (the derivatives of range and volume measures with respect to eye size). These measures were computed for the mean pupil size of the finned and digited groups
To make estimates of visual range and volume, we adapted a model of aquatic visual capability for pelagic fish from Nilsson et al. (18), which focused on first-order optical physics, and incorporated contrast threshold into this framework. With this change, visual ranges in aquatic and above-water environments can be calculated using the same model. The visual system is modeled with two channels: one that only sees an empty background and one that only sees a given target (18) (Materials and Methods and SI Appendix, Computational Visual Sensory Ecology Estimates). For these reasons, our visual range estimates represent a best case scenario for visual detection in the absence of clutter.
To compute the amount of light in the background and the light from the target, we must model the likely visual environments of the early tetrapods. Visual range for a given object differs greatly between aquatic and aerial environments, a fact related to their dramatically different attenuation lengths. The attenuation length of a medium is defined as 1/
Our results (Fig. 4) show that visual performance underwent a massive increase with the shift from vision through water to vision through air. For the case of daytime viewing horizontally in water through finned tetrapod eyes vs. air through digited tetrapod eyes, the range increased by well over a factor of 100. A conservative estimate of the total volume that our standard object could be sensed within (Materials and Methods)—a more ethologically relevant measure (29, 30)—increases by just under 2 million times (Fig. 4 A1, A2, C1, and C2 and SI Appendix, row 2 in Table S4).
Visual performance in and out of water. Aquatic vision is estimated using the Baseline River water type defined in SI Appendix, Table S3 at a depth of 8 m. The object is a black 10-cm-diameter disk. (A1 and A2) Maximum distance that the object can be seen in water and air, respectively, under various lighting conditions. Note that visual range scales proportionately to target size. For a 1-cm disk in daylight, the aerial range decreases to 139.3 m assuming mean pupil size for digited tetrapods and 54.7 m for the mean pupil size of finned tetrapods (data not shown). (B1 and B2) How much range is gained for an increase in pupil size? Note that the y-axis multiplier is 100 times larger in B2. (C1 and C2) Total volume within which the standard object is visible. Note that the y-axis multiplier is 1 million times larger in C2. (D1 and D2) How much volume is gained for an increase in pupil size? Note that the y-axis multiplier is 1 million times larger in D2. For the aerial plots, the aquatic values are shown but imperceptible. Uncertainty for the daylight condition (green fill) was calculated by using alternative values for the vision model (Materials and Methods) and is not shown for other conditions for clarity. SD is here only for showing the distribution of estimated pupil sizes; it cannot be used in this context for ascertaining significance because of shared ancestry (details are in the text). *Red horizontal bars show
We have performed a large number of sensitivity analyses to determine how robust this conclusion is to our assumptions (Materials and Methods, Sensitivity Analyses). These analyses divide into perturbations of our baseline visual environment and perturbations to our baseline visual physiology. In terms of sensitivity to environment, we note that a large increase in our measures of visual performance occurs regardless of the diel activity patterns of the early tetrapods. Our results are consequently agnostic to whether the increase in eye size was for the gain of sensitivity that this causes for the dim light vision models (leading to larger range) or because larger eyes in full light lead to an increase in acuity (also leading to larger range) (SI Appendix, Vision Model Limitations and Sensitivity Analysis and Table S1). Our results are also unaffected by variations in water clarity. In terms of perturbations to our baseline visual physiology, changes to a host of factors, including contrast threshold, photoreceptor size, and dark noise level among others, have effects that are well outside of the range where our conclusions are affected. For the daylight vision case, these variations are shown in Fig. 4, solid green fill.
One finding that is informative given the movement of the eyes from their standard fish-like location along the sides of the head to the top (compare Eusthenopteron with Tiktaalik in Fig. 2) has to do with extended range for looking up toward the water surface vs. looking horizontally within water. Because sunlight is predominantly down-welling at shallower depths (ref. 31, figure 2.20 and ref. 32), upward viewing in these environments provides a considerable increment in range—a factor of
Based on our results on trait evolution and computational visual ecology, we conclude that the observed increase in eye size is the result of adaptive evolution, where the derivative of the range and volume with respect to the pupil size is a proxy for the selective benefit of mutations that increase eye size. This measure shows a very clear pattern: in the transition from underwater to aerial viewing, there is a gain of
Discussion
Our results show that the inferred tripling of eye size achieves very little additional performance for eyes that are underwater (Fig. 4A1). In the most likely aquatic environments of early tetrapods, such as the lobe-finned fish Eusthenopteron, vision was on the order of a body length—as also estimated for contemporary coastal fish (35)—before and after the tripling of eye size. In contrast, were these eyes looking out over the waterline, a conservative estimate of the field of view gives a total visually surveyed volume increase of over 1 million times the aquatic volume (Fig. 4 C1 vs. C2). Although the bulk of this increase is because of the change in environment, just examining the effect of eye size changes alone discloses that aerial performance increases a factor of 10 times more over the performance increase that happens in the aquatic case. Furthermore, for aerial vision, there is a 5 million-fold increase in the amount of space that our standard object can be seen within for a given increase in eye size compared with aquatic vision. Finally, there is a switch from diminishing returns with larger eyes in the aquatic case to increasing returns for larger eyes in the aerial case (Fig. 4 D1 vs. D2).
These performance gains may explain the evolutionary increase of eye size, despite the high metabolic cost of sensory acquisition (30, 36⇓⇓–39), but additional insights can be gained by combining the metabolic cost of sensing with the analysis of the energetics of movement (30). This synthesis suggests that reducing the metabolic cost of predation is spread across the motor and sensory systems involved. To scan a given volume of space for prey or predators, increased energy expenditure on longer-range vision can have savings over having to translate the entire body more when sensory range is short. Similarly, swiveling a large visual sensory volume using the neck that evolved first in Tiktaalik is less costly than body reorientation for acquiring the same visual information (30).
The 1 million-fold gain in visually monitored space through aerial vision could, therefore, have had a favorable impact on energetics. If aerial vision and its beneficial energetics are the basis of the increase in eye size, it coincides with another change likely to improve energetics. In the transitional elpistostegalians, there is evidence for another major bodily function moving from below to above water: respiration. In this group and later taxa, there was an enlargement of breathing spiracles (also called otic notches) located behind the eyes (green in Fig. 5) (9, 13, 26, 40). The enlargement of spiracles occurred during a time of Earth’s history when oxygen levels trended downward (26, 41). For aquatic animals, the Devonian decline of oxygen was exacerbated by the fact that water has 1/30th of the oxygen of air, while being 800 times denser (42, 43). Therefore, respiration with water requires
A possible evolutionary scenario consistent with our results. Having invaded shallow waters, where the down-welling component of sunlight is significant, better visual range is obtained with eye sockets moved to the top of the skull, providing upward vision (Fig. 4A1) as shown here for Panderichthys. Possibly driven by low oxygen, animals surfaced near shore to breathe through the spiracles that had also dorsalized to just behind the eyes in the elpistostegalians, as shown here for Tiktaalik. Without correction for the differing refractive index of air, they initially saw blurry outlines of invertebrate fauna (33) that had already been living on land for 50 My. With continued surfacing and selection of the slight changes to lens and cornea to enable a focused image of their quarry, in a small fraction (34) of the 12-My transition from finned to digited tetrapod eye sizes, the full power of long-range vision would have emerged. The strong derivative of visual volume with respect to eye size would have facilitated the observed selection for larger eye size. Simultaneously, selective advantages of limbs with digits over limbs with fins made animals like Acanthostega better suited for longer forays onto land, culminating in more terrestrial forms, such as Pederpes, 30 My after Tiktaalik. The colored portion of the simplified tree marks an evolutionary phase with substantial body plan modifications. Shown in green in Left are the spiracles (what becomes the Eustachian tube) likely used for breathing at the water surface while using aerial vision. Total animal lengths are between 50 cm and 1.5 m and are not drawn to scale. Age spans from 385 My for Eusthenopteron to 355 My for Pederpes.
It has been suggested that the anatomical features of the elpistostegalians—enlarged breathing spiracles at the top of the skull and eyes on top of the skull on bony prominences—enabled stealthy crocodilian-like predatory behavior (8, 13, 40), in which animals are at the surface with their eyes and spiracles just out of the water, looking at the water–land interface for potential prey to attack from the water (Tiktaalik in Fig. 5). Possible prey include large terrestrial invertebrates that arrived approximately 50 My before vertebrates (33). Interestingly, one group of these invertebrates, the millipedes, developed chemical defense systems in the lower Devonian (44). In millipedes that exist today, these systems deter vertebrate predators (45). Hunting in a crocodile-like manner resolves a certain logical tension between the specializations for fully aquatic life seen in the elpistostegalians and adaptations that seem better suited for brief forays onto land: larger eyes and limbs (9, 10) significantly supplemented by tail movement (46).
If air breathing and aerial vision-guided predation of invertebrates on land were early tetrapod adaptations to unfavorable energetics, including low dissolved oxygen within water, it is interesting that the early Carboniferous is also the time when the adelospondyl-colosteid group of digited aquatic tetrapods evolved. Not only did the eyes of this purely aquatic group revert to the mean size of the finned tetrapods (Greererpeton in Fig. 2), they also lost their breathing spiracles (8, 47⇓–49), with the possible exemption of some adelospondyls (8, 50). Notably, by this time in the Carboniferous, oxygen levels had more than rebounded from their low in the Devonian (26, 41).
Implications of Long-Range Vision for Reactive Neural Circuits and Planning
Long-range vision has large effects on animal behavior, because it allows more complex decision making over extended sensory ranges. Stimulus-evoked behavior (51) with respect to the most crucial decisions that an organism needs to make, such as an escape response to a looming predator or a lunge at prey, was the normal situation in the aquatic habitats from which land vertebrates originated, where viewing distances were on the order of a body length (SI Appendix, Reaction Time with Respect to Visual Range in Finned Tetrapods). With short-range visual stimuli arriving in a just-in-time-to-act fashion, internally driven behavior (52) with serial decision making (53) requiring time proportional to the length of the sequence of actions being deliberated (54) was challenging for actions stemming from perception of dynamic stimuli—such as mobile predators and prey (ref. 55, p. 496). This limitation changed with the evolution of the first long-range (more than 100 body lengths) imaging sensory modality: aerial vision. The only other long-range imaging system in animals is echolocation, which only emerged much later in mammals.
In quite simple instances, an increase in visual range can result in a completely different behavioral control strategy, with large savings in mechanical effort (SI Appendix, How Temporal/Spatial Range Affects Optimal Decision Making). After the emergence of vision above the water line, the total volume of space monitored by vision in daylight conditions increased 1 million-fold over that of water in full sunlight, enabling (although not necessitating) complex “deliberative mode” strategies (29, 53, 55) with respect to the most unpredictable features of our environment: other animals. Emergence onto land, with its complex environmental geometry (56) featuring multiple paths toward prey or away from predators, would have furthered the selective benefit of more complex control strategies that take more time to compute than the simplest reactive strategies.
As more behavior became regulated by long-range vision (even with nocturnality) (SI Appendix, Table S4), there would have been reduced selective benefit for that portion of the neural infrastructure of the “reactive mode” (29, 55) that aids predator evasion after detection at short range in water. In fish and amphibians, the delay between predator detection and escape is reduced by
With the evolution of suitable brain circuitry, certain animals were able to consider multiple options for pursuit or evasion that are likely to enhance fitness, such as by vicarious trial and error behavior in rodents, in which future possibilities are imagined (53, 64). Vicarious trial and error, like other forms of prospective cognition or “mental time travel,” are dependent on the hippocampus. This structure has the same developmental origin in birds and mammals (65), which had their most recent common ancestor in the Late Carboniferous not long after fully terrestrial animals arose. With this affordance of long-range vision, therefore, we hypothesize that the core neuronal components of planning (53, 65)—now understood to occur in both birds (66) and mammals but less well-studied in reptiles (67)—evolved within a common ancestor (65).
Conclusion
Although the emergence of complete limbs with fingers and toes is central in our imagination of what happened when we transitioned from fish, our results (summarized in Fig. 5) highlight how there was a dramatic change in the information environment of early tetrapods before these anatomical changes occurred. This change in sensory landscape raises the interesting possibility that seeing fitness-enhancing opportunities along the water’s edge from afar facilitated the evolution of full terrestriality. We show that eyes nearly tripled in size between when early lobe-finned fish lived and when tetrapods with digited limbs evolved. This big increase in eye size was likely driven by aquatic tetrapods surfacing their eyes above the water line and hunting like crocodiles. This lifestyle caused “primarily aquatic” features to be retained (9, 11, 68), while more and more robust limbs gradually evolved, enabling forays onto land (69). Because small adjustments to optical mechanics evolved to account for the change in the refractive index of air from water, there was an ever-expanding domain of visual awareness, leading these animals to long-range viewing and hunting of the bounty of invertebrate food on the shores. These resources would favor animals evolving morphological adaptations—such as weight-bearing limbs—and neurobehavioral adaptations—such as executing an extended sequence of goal-directed activity—that would enable the exploitation of the new resource. According to the buena vista hypothesis (ref. 55, p. 482), the greatly extended and inexpensive channel of information that long-range vision provided to the opportunities on land may have been central to the evolution of terrestriality and eventually, forms of prospective cognition that this habitat advantages.
Materials and Methods
Selection of Taxa, Phylogenetic Hypothesis, and Time Calibration.
Our goal was to select fossils that would cover the initial water to land transition in the Tetrapodomorpha. The habitat preferences of many of the early tetrapods are not exactly known yet, but an increase of terrestriality is generally assumed to have occurred in the Upper Devonian and Early Carboniferous tetrapods. For example, the watcheerid Pederpes (Fig. 5) shows foot anatomy that is well-suited for terrestrial locomotion (ref. 8, p. 273), whereas more basal members, such as Ichthyostega and Acanthostega, are still largely aquatic (8). Hence, we sampled across the stem tetrapod group (sensu Clack). Our selection of taxa was largely guided by prior phylogenies (48, 70⇓⇓⇓–74) to account for phylogenetic covariance. For digited tetrapods, the Early Tetrapod Database (47) was used to identify well-preserved material. To increase the number of well-preserved species, phylogenetic scope was extended crownward to include basal members of the total group Amphibia [Edopoidea and Dendrerpetontidae (74)] and the total group Amniota [Anthracosauria and Gephyrostegidae (72)], which covers most measurable taxa that can be placed in existing phylogenetic hypotheses. Our stemward coverage is less complete, focusing on better known taxa. Caerorhachis and Neopteroplax were excluded, because their phylogenetic position is unclear. We model the uncertainty caused by poorly understood phylogenetic relationships for the baphetids, colosteids, and tristichopterids by forming polytomies. We used the stratigraphic ranges of the fossils to time-calibrate the phylogeny with the paleotree package in R (75) (SI Appendix, Figs. S1, S2). For the basal tetrapodomorphs, stratigraphic range was based on the dating of the corresponding geological formation extracted from recent papers; for digited tetrapods, the Early Tetrapod Database (47) was used. We used the International Chronostratigraphic Chart (76) to translate to absolute time. Time data were treated as minimum and maximum bounds on single point dates, which were pulled from a uniform distribution. To account for the uncertainty involved in the exact stratigraphic ranges, we repeated this process 1,000 times, resulting in a set of 1,000 time-calibrated phylogenies. The time scaling was performed with the “equal” method, which equally allocates time available on deeper branches to resolve the zero branch length problem (75, 77, 78). Polytomies were resolved randomly for each of the 1,000 trees.
Skull Measurements.
Measurements were taken from published drawings and images produced by experts in the field. Eye socket length is defined as the maximum length of the socket, except for in taxa in the digited tetrapod group featuring antorbital vacuities, for which the major axis of an ellipse fit to the orbit alone was used. We define skull length as the distance from the tip of the snout to the caudal margin of the postparietal bones at their medial suture. The extracted measurements are shown in SI Appendix, Table S5. Skull source data (images and drawings) used for measurement are in SI Appendix.
Defining Eye Size in a Phylogenetic Comparative Framework.
We chose the Bayesian implementation of the OU method (5) to assess the adaptive significance of eye socket size differences in early tetrapods. Specifically, we analyzed whether the evolution of relative eye socket size includes changes in the selective regime that may be congruent with periods of change in the water to land transition. This approach does not require prior classification of samples into categories, which is advantageous given that it is currently difficult to assign early tetrapods into discrete habitat categories. Bayesian OU instead agnostically infers whether changes in the selective regime of a trait occurred through evolutionary time and if so, along which branches in the phylogeny these changes likely happened. It is also less prone to error for small phylogenetic comparative datasets (
Computational Visual Sensory Ecology Estimates.
The numerical model for both aquatic and aerial vision contains two components: calculation of the optical stimulus, which can overestimate visual range (particularly in the aerial case), and calculation of the effect of contrast threshold. The framework for calculation of the optical stimulus is adapted from Nilsson et al. (18), which calculates visual range based on the assumption that there are two separate channels that view the background and the target, where channel size is determined by the angular size of the target on the retina (given a particular photoreceptor arrangement) for optimal viewing. A target is said to be visible at a distance if and only if the difference between the photons that arrive from the target and the background are greater than their combined Poisson noise with some reliability coefficient. This relationship can be summarized by the equation:
The 10-cm black disk size was chosen to be ethologically relevant for the 1- to 2-m body lengths typical of the early tetrapods, but the differences between aquatic and aerial performance reported here are insensitive to size chosen. We estimated the visual sensory volume (18, 29, 82) for this object by a spherical sector of specified radius (from visual range calculations), azimuth, and elevation. For aquatic viewing, we chose an azimuth of 305∘ [170∘ per eye minus 35∘ binocular overlap typical of fish (83)] and an elevation of 60∘. For aerial vision, we chose an azimuth of 287∘ [156∘ for monocular vision and 25∘ binocular overlap typical of crocodile (ref. 84, pp. 293–294)] and an elevation of 30∘. The aerial vertical field of view of 30∘ is similar to the vertically compressed field of view provided by the horizontal foveal streak in contemporary crocodile eyes (
Optical Properties of Water for Aquatic Vision Estimates.
Because early tetrapods predominantly inhabited freshwater rivers, streams, or estuaries (8, 9, 24⇓–26), light-field simulations were done in waters with higher turbidity and absorption than the clearest ocean water. The Baseline River model (parameter values; SI Appendix, Table S3) used in Fig. 4 A1–D1 was selected, such that the attenuation length at 575 nm was between 0.02 and 2.7 m, the span of values for New Zealand and Alaskan rivers and lakes (22, 23). For example, the Baseline River model’s attenuation length of 0.46 m is around the first quartile of lake values and above the median of river values. The water model parameter values were used as input to the radiative transfer program HydroLight (version 5.3; Sequoia Scientific, Inc.) to generate spectral radiance values [
Sensitivity Analyses.
Our selection of three light environments across
Acknowledgments
We thank Scott Schaper and Olivia Carmo for assistance with data collection and Ian Abraham for coding the ball control example (SI Appendix, Fig. S8). We also thank Josef Uyeda for assistance with the bayou package. The image of Eusthenopteron is courtesy of John Merck (University of Maryland), and reconstruction of Acanthostega is courtesy of Michael Coates (University of Chicago). We thank Sönke Johnsen (Duke University) for providing the starlight spectra used in this study. Marco Gallio, Michael Paulin, and Peter C. Wainwright gave valuable feedback on an earlier draft. This work was partially funded by National Science Foundation Grants IOS-ORG 1456830 (to M.A.M.) and PECASE IOB-0846032 (to M.A.M.), and the support of Northwestern University during M.A.M.’s 3-mo academic leave.
Footnotes
- ↵1To whom correspondence may be addressed. Email: maciver{at}northwestern.edu or lschmitz{at}kecksci.claremont.edu.
Author contributions: M.A.M. and L.S. designed research; M.A.M. and L.S. performed research; T.D.M. contributed new reagents/analytic tools; M.A.M., L.S., U.M., and C.D.M. analyzed data; and M.A.M. and L.S. wrote the paper.
The authors declare no conflict of interest.
This article is a PNAS Direct Submission.
Data deposition: Code and data to reproduce these results is available at https://doi.org/10.5281/zenodo.321923.
This article contains supporting information online at www.pnas.org/lookup/suppl/doi:10.1073/pnas.1615563114/-/DCSupplemental.
References
- ↵
- ↵
- ↵
- ↵
- ↵.
- Uyeda JC,
- Harmon LJ
- ↵
- ↵
- ↵.
- Clack JA
- ↵
- ↵.
- Shubin NH,
- Daeschler EB,
- Jenkins FA
- ↵
- ↵.
- Vorobyeva E,
- Schultze HP
- ↵
- ↵.
- Schultze H,
- Arsenault M
- ↵
- ↵
- ↵.
- Angielczyk KD,
- Schmitz L
- ↵.
- Nilsson DE,
- Warrant E,
- Johnsen S
- ↵.
- Kirk JTO
- ↵.
- Mobley CD
- ↵.
- Davies-Colley RJ,
- Nagels JW
- ↵.
- Davies-Colley RJ
- ↵.
- Koenings J,
- Edmundson J
- ↵.
- Narkiewicz M, et al.
- ↵.
- Retallack GJ
- ↵.
- Clack JA
- ↵.
- Killinger D,
- Churnside J,
- Rothman L
- ↵.
- Middleton WEK
- ↵.
- Snyder JB,
- Nelson ME,
- Burdick JW,
- MacIver MA
- ↵
- ↵.
- Cronin TW,
- Johnsen S,
- Marshall NJ,
- Warrant EJ
- ↵.
- Mobley C,
- Boss E,
- Roesier C
- ↵
- ↵.
- Nilsson DE,
- Pelger S
- ↵
- ↵
- ↵.
- Niven JE,
- Laughlin SB
- ↵.
- Moran D,
- Softley R,
- Warrant EJ
- ↵.
- Lewis JE,
- Gilmour KM,
- Moorhead MJ,
- Perry SF,
- Markham MR
- ↵.
- Graham JB, et al.
- ↵.
- Royer DL,
- Donnadieu Y,
- Park J,
- Kowalczyk J,
- Goddéris Y
- ↵.
- Glass ML,
- Rantin FT
- ↵.
- Glass ML
- ↵.
- Wilson HM
- ↵.
- Shear WA
- ↵.
- McInroe B, et al.
- ↵
- ↵.
- Bolt JR,
- Lombard RE
- ↵.
- Carroll R
- ↵
- ↵.
- Jun JJ,
- Longtin A,
- Maler L
- ↵
- ↵
- ↵
- ↵.
- MacIver MA
- ↵
- ↵.
- Kohashi T,
- Oda Y
- ↵.
- Zottoli SJ,
- Faber DS
- ↵.
- Preuss T,
- Osei-Bonsu PE,
- Weiss SA,
- Wang C,
- Faber DS
- ↵.
- Catania KC
- ↵.
- Bhattacharyya KD,
- McLean D,
- MacIver MA
- ↵
- ↵
- ↵
- ↵.
- Allen TA,
- Fortin NJ
- ↵
- ↵.
- Wilkinson A,
- Huber L
- ↵
- ↵.
- Gunter G
- ↵
- ↵.
- Anderson PSL,
- Friedman M,
- Ruta M
- ↵
- ↵.
- Milner AC,
- Milner AR,
- Walsh SA
- ↵.
- Schoch RR
- ↵
- ↵.
- Cohen KM,
- Finney SC,
- Gibbard PL,
- Fan JX
- ↵.
- Brusatte SL,
- Benton MJ,
- Ruta M,
- Lloyd GT
- ↵
- ↵.
- Cooper N,
- Thomas GH,
- Venditti C,
- Meade A,
- Freckleton RP
- ↵
- ↵
- ↵
- ↵.
- Land MF,
- Nilsson DE
- ↵.
- Walls GL
- ↵.
- Nagloo N,
- Collin SP,
- Hemmi JM,
- Hart NS
- ↵.
- Steen M
- ↵
- ↵