Disclosing information about the self is intrinsically rewarding
Edited by Michael S. Gazzaniga, University of California, Santa Barbara, CA, and approved March 27, 2012 (received for review February 7, 2012)
Abstract
Humans devote 30–40% of speech output solely to informing others of their own subjective experiences. What drives this propensity for disclosure? Here, we test recent theories that individuals place high subjective value on opportunities to communicate their thoughts and feelings to others and that doing so engages neural and cognitive mechanisms associated with reward. Five studies provided support for this hypothesis. Self-disclosure was strongly associated with increased activation in brain regions that form the mesolimbic dopamine system, including the nucleus accumbens and ventral tegmental area. Moreover, individuals were willing to forgo money to disclose about the self. Two additional studies demonstrated that these effects stemmed from the independent value that individuals placed on self-referential thought and on simply sharing information with others. Together, these findings suggest that the human tendency to convey information about personal experience may arise from the intrinsic value associated with self-disclosure.
Sign up for PNAS alerts.
Get alerts for new articles, or get an alert when an article is cited.
Studies of human conversation have documented that 30–40% of everyday speech is used to relay information to others about one’s private experiences or personal relationships (1–4), and recent surveys of Internet use indicate that upwards of 80% of posts to social media sites (such as Twitter) consist simply of announcements about one’s own immediate experiences (5). Although other primates do not generally attempt to communicate to others what they know—for example, by pointing out interesting things or modeling behaviors for others to imitate—by 9 mo of age, human children begin trying to draw others’ attention to aspects of the environment that they find important (6), and adults in all societies make consistent attempts to impart their knowledge to others (7). Recently, a number of commentators have argued that such unusually high rates of disclosure derive from a species-specific motivation to share one’s beliefs and knowledge about the world (6, 7), suggesting that our species may have an intrinsic drive to disclose thoughts to others. This account suggests the following hypothesis: To the extent that humans are motivated to propagate the products of their minds, opportunities to disclose one’s thoughts should be experienced as a powerful form of subjective reward. Here, across five studies, we used a combination of neuroimaging and cognitive methods to demonstrate empirical support for this possibility.
Over the past two decades, researchers have characterized a system of neural regions that responds to the anticipation and receipt of reward (8–11). In both humans and animals, the mesolimbic dopamine system—which includes the nucleus accumbens (NAcc) and the ventral tegmental area (VTA)—responds robustly to primary rewards such as food (12–15); secondary rewards such as money or other tokens that can be exchanged for primary rewards (16–22); and even social rewards such as learning that others share one’s opinion, experiencing humor, or catching a brief glimpse of an attractive member of the opposite sex (23–27).
In the current studies, we capitalized on the consistency of such findings to examine whether these same brain regions are engaged by opportunities to disclose information about one’s self. In these studies, participants underwent functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) scanning while alternately disclosing their own beliefs and opinions or speculating about the beliefs and opinions of another person. To the extent that revealing one’s own thoughts is intrinsically rewarding, we expected greater activity in the NAcc and VTA to accompany trials in which participants answered such questions about themselves than when they answered questions about others.
In addition, we assessed the value associated with opportunities for self-disclosure behaviorally. To do so, we developed a modified version of the “pay-per-view” task introduced by Deaner, Platt, and colleagues (28, 29). In this task, participants make a series of choices between stimuli with fixed value (such as money) and a second category of stimuli with unknown subjective value. For example, Deaner et al. (28) had macaques choose between variable amounts of a primary reinforcer (juice) and brief visual displays of a high-status groupmate. Monkeys waived significant amounts of juice for the opportunity to view a dominant male, suggesting that such displays have high subjective value. In much the same way, Hayden et al. (29) quantified the reward associated with beauty and sexual attraction by measuring the amount of money that university students would forgo to view brief visual displays of attractive members of the opposite sex. Here, we used similar logic to assess the value associated with self-disclosure. To the extent that revealing one’s own thoughts is intrinsically rewarding, we expected participants to forgo more money to answer questions about themselves than about others.
Results
In study 1a, participants (n = 78) alternately disclosed their own opinions or judged the opinions of others (30, 31). In study 1b, participants (n = 117) alternately disclosed their beliefs about their own personality traits or judged the traits of another person (Fig. S1). For both sets of data, we conducted two parallel analyses to examine whether neural regions associated with reward—specifically, the NAcc and the VTA—were engaged by the opportunity to disclose one’s own beliefs or opinions more than by considering those of another person. First, we conducted a whole-brain random-effects contrast to identify regions that were more active for self > other trials (P < 0.05, corrected; see Tables S1 and S2 for full results). Replicating earlier work, this contrast revealed an extensive region of medial prefrontal cortex (MPFC), a region that has consistently been associated with self-referential thought (32–34). More importantly for the current hypothesis, this analysis also revealed robust activity in those neural regions implicated in reward processing. In study 1a, significantly greater response was observed in the NAcc bilaterally when disclosing one’s own opinions and attitudes than when judging those of another person (Fig. 1A). In study 1b, both the NAcc bilaterally and the VTA (cluster extent = 22 voxels) responded more strongly when disclosing beliefs about one’s own personality traits than when judging the traits of others (Fig. 1B).
Fig. 1.
To confirm that these regions overlapped with those responsive to rewarding stimuli, we independently defined neural regions from a task in which a separate sample of participants (n = 14) received monetary rewards [the Monetary Incentive Delay (MID) task] (18). Replicating earlier research, we observed greater activity in bilateral NAcc in trials during which participants anticipated and received a small monetary reward ($2) than in those on which no reward was available. These regions-of-interest (ROIs) were then interrogated for responses during self-disclosure. In both study 1a and study 1b, both left and right aspects of this independently defined NAcc cluster revealed significantly greater activity during trials in which participants disclosed their own beliefs or opinions than when they judged those of others [study 1a: t(77) = 3.59, P = 0.0006, Cohen’s d = 0.41; study 1b: t(116) = 3.73 P = 0.0003, d = 0.35; Fig. 1D]. Interestingly, a number of other fMRI studies have reported—but not discussed—similar striatal activation associated with the contrast of self > nonself (35–39), supporting the hypothesis that disclosing about the self is experienced as intrinsically rewarding.
Although activation of the NAcc and VTA often reflects the presence of rewarding stimuli or positive prediction errors (40), these regions have also been shown to respond to nonreward signals (cf. 41, 42–44). To obviate this potential ambiguity in the interpretation of neural activation, study 2 deployed a completely different measure of the value associated with opportunities for self-disclosure. This behavioral assay examined the amount of money that participants were willing to forgo to disclose information about themselves.
Participants (n = 37) in study 2 made a series of choices to perform one of three tasks: (i) a self trial on which they reported their own opinions and attitudes (e.g., “how much do you enjoy winter sports such as skiing?”); (ii) an other trial on which they judged the attitudes of another person (e.g., “how much does Barack Obama enjoy winter sports such as skiing?”); or (iii) a fact trial on which they answered “true” or “false” to a trivia item (e.g., “Leonardo Da Vinci painted the Mona Lisa”). On each of 195 trials, participants chose between two question types: that is, between self and other, other and fact, or self and fact. Each choice was associated with a small monetary payoff ($0.01–$0.04) that the participants received at the completion of the experiment. Payoff amounts for each choice varied across trials (and were occasionally equal), as did the type of question for which participants received the larger amount. If participants experience self-disclosure to be intrinsically rewarding, they should be willing to forgo money to answer questions about themselves. On the other hand, participants were free to maximize their financial payoff by consistently choosing whichever task paid the greater amount, irrespective of the opportunity for self-disclosure. The relative value of each task was modeled by calculating the point of subjective equivalence (PSE) between the question types. This value was derived by fitting a cumulative normal distribution curve to each participant’s choices (Fig. 2A) and finding the monetary value at which a participant effectively chooses arbitrarily between two question types (28). Thus, the PSE represents the relative monetary value of one question type over another.
Fig. 2.
Consistent with the hypothesis that self-disclosure is inherently valuable, participants showed a significant preference for answering questions about the self over both of the nonself question types. When payoff amounts were equal between question types, participants chose to answer self questions 69 and 66% of the time over other questions and fact questions, respectively. Moreover, calculations of the PSE showed that answering questions about the self was worth significantly more than either of the other two types of questions. Participants gave up an average of 0.63¢ per trial to perform the self task instead of judging another person [t(36) = 3.32, P = . 002, d = 1.11] and were willing to forgo an average of 0.54¢ per trial to perform the self task instead of answering a fact question [t(36) = 2.12, P = 0.04, d = 0.71; Fig. 2B]. This translated into an average loss of 17% of potential earnings, owing to participants’ choices to receive the lower payoff amount to answer questions about the self. Just as monkeys are willing to forgo juice rewards to view dominant groupmates and college students are willing to give up money to view attractive members of the opposite sex, our participants were willing to forgo money to think and talk about themselves.
Together, studies 1 and 2 provided both behavioral and neural evidence that self-disclosure is intrinsically rewarding. However, the results from answering questions about the self in studies 1 and 2 confound two possible sources of reward from self-disclosure. One possibility is that participants may simply place a high value on introspecting about the self. Indeed, a good deal of research supports the idea that the self is a fundamentally positive stimulus: people implicitly associate positive affect with the self (45–47), overvalue their personal possessions (48, 49), and even tend to adopt professions and live in cities that resemble the sound of their name (47). Thus, perhaps just the act of thinking about the self is sufficiently rewarding to account for the results of studies 1 and 2. However, a more interesting possibility is that self-referential thought might be even more valuable if those introspections are communicated to another person. If humans are indeed motivated by the opportunity to propagate knowledge, we should expect individuals to place especially high value on those situations during which they not only have occasion to reflect on their thoughts and opinions, but also are allowed to share their insights (both self-referential and otherwise) with another person.
To examine these possibilities, in study 3 we measured responses of the mesolimbic dopamine system while participants considered their own opinions and then either shared them with another person or kept their answer private (Fig. S2). As in study 1, participants (n = 17) underwent fMRI while alternately disclosing their own opinions and attitudes (self trials) or speculating about the opinions of another person, Barack Obama (other trials). Importantly, participants arrived at the laboratory in the company of a friend or relative, and their answers to half the questions were displayed to this person via computer (shared trials), whereas answers to the other half remained private (private trials). Thus, in total, there were four possible trial types: self shared, self private, other shared, and other private. Participants were informed that no one, including the experimenters, would ever see their responses to the private trials. As such, these instructions rendered the private condition in this study even more private than in studies 1 and 2, where participants may reasonably have assumed that the experimenters would view their responses.
We conducted three parallel analyses to test the separate contributions of introspecting about the self and disclosing those introspections to another person to the reward of self-disclosure. First, we conducted two whole-brain random-effects contrasts (P < 0.05, corrected) to examine the main effects of self > other trials (Fig. 3A) and shared > private trials (Fig. 3B). The contrast of self > other revealed robust response in the MPFC, replicating both study 1 as well as a large extant literature linking this region with self-referential tasks (32–34). However, this same region was not revealed by the share > private contrast, suggesting that the MPFC response to the self > other contrast does not reflect a response to self-disclosure per se, but rather to the self-referential nature of self-disclosure more specifically. In contrast, both the NAcc and the VTA responded consistently to both the self and disclosure aspects of self-disclosure: both contrasts of self > other and share > private revealed significantly greater activity in the bilateral NAcc and VTA, suggesting that merely introspecting about the self is sufficient to engage the mesolimbic reward system but that the opportunity to share such introspections is likewise rewarding (see Tables S1 and S2 for full results).
Fig. 3.
In a second analysis, we functionally localized the NAcc in the same set of participants by examining neural responses to the anticipation and receipt of monetary rewards during the MID task (18) (Fig. 3C). Consistent with the results of the whole-brain analysis, a 2 × 2 repeated-measures ANOVA over activity in bilateral NAcc revealed two main effects. First, we observed a significant main effect of self > other [F(1,16) = 7.73, P = 0.013, d = 0.70], again suggesting that introspecting about the self is more rewarding than considering the opinions of others. Second, we observed a significant main effect of sharing > private [F(1,16) = 17.02, P = 0.001, d = 1.03], suggesting that disclosing information to other people is more rewarding than merely introspecting about one’s opinions privately. No interaction was observed between the self and shared factors [F(1,16) = 0.56, P = 0.47, d = 0.19], suggesting that the value of disclosing information with others is not contingent upon that information being specifically about the self, nor is the value of introspection contingent upon having an audience for those thoughts (Fig. 3D). Rather, both self-referential thought and sharing with others provide two independent motivators for self-disclosure.
The design of study 3 was highly similar to studies 1a and 1b, with one important exception. Because participants in study 1a and 1b were never informed about who, if anyone, would see their responses, it is unclear whether participants in these studies regarded the situation as one in which their answers were being shared (i.e., with the research staff) or remained private. In contrast, the presence or absence of an audience was made highly salient in study 3, thus raising the possibility that participants would more consistently regard the private trials as confidential. To examine the potential effect of these differences, we calculated the effect sizes of the simple effect of self >other within both the shared condition (d = 0.56) and the private condition (d = 0.35) from the functionally localized ROI in study 3. These effect sizes were very similar to the self > other effects from both study 1a (d = 0.41) and study 1b (d = 0.35). In other words, although the private condition of study 3 appears numerically smaller than the effects observed in studies 1a and 1b, we observed nearly identical effects across studies, suggesting that individuals value self-referential thought even in the absence of either a real or an implied audience.
In a third analysis, we anatomically localized both the NAcc and the VTA in each participant in native space, using a high-resolution proton-density-weighted anatomical image. In addition to providing an independent way of localizing reward regions of interest, such an analysis also allows for interrogation of activity in the VTA, the small size of which (∼60 mm3) makes it difficult to localize functionally. Results of this analysis were consistent with the previous two analyses: A 2 × 2 repeated-measures ANOVA of activity in the NAcc revealed greater activity during self than during other trials [F(1,16) = 4.05, P = 0.061, d = 0.50], significantly greater activity during shared than during private trials [F(1,16) = 21.74, P = 0.0003, d = 1.17], but no interaction between the two factors [F(1,16) = 0.21, P = 0.65, d = 0.11]. Likewise, a 2 × 2 repeated-measures ANOVA of activity in the VTA revealed significantly greater response during self trials than other trials [F(1,16) = 7.38, P = 0.015, d = 0.68] and during shared than during private trials [F(1,16) = 5.94, P = 0.027, d = 0.61], but no interaction between the two factors [F(1,16) = 0.40, P = 0.53, d = 0.16]. Together, these results suggest that the value of self-disclosure may derive from two independent sources: both introspecting about the self and communicating information to other people. Because both factors robustly activate neural regions associated with reward and do not interact, each of these factors appears to contribute independently to the motivation for self-disclosure.
In study 4, we replicated the findings from study 3 using the same behavioral assay used in study 2. On each of the 240 trials, participants (n = 41) chose between a passive 5-s delay and answering one of four types of question: (i) a self shared trial in which participants disclosed their opinions to another person; (ii) a self private trial in which participants privately introspected about their opinions; (iii) an other shared trial in which participants judged the attitudes of another person and disclosed their answer to another person; or (iv) an other private trial in which participants judged another person’s attitudes privately. This design diverged slightly from the previous studies in that comparisons were made to a passive delay rather than an active task (i.e., questions about others or facts). We were concerned about the possibility that participants might have valued self-referential thought simply because thinking about the self is relatively easier or entails more certainty than answering questions about other people or about trivia facts (although this concern does not likely apply to the share > private effects of study 3). To examine this possibility, we included a maximally easy condition: passive delay. If participants were choosing to answer questions merely on the basis of relative ease, then they should place the greatest value on the opportunity to simply rest passively for several seconds. However, if the act of self-disclosure itself represents an intrinsically rewarding option, participants should still be willing to forgo money to self-disclose, despite it being the more difficult of two options.
Payoff amounts varied (between $0.01 and $0.04) across trials, as did the option for which participants received the larger payoff. As in study 2, we quantified the relative monetary value of each question type by calculating the PSE between that question type and the delay option by fitting a cumulative normal distribution curve to each participant’s choices and finding the monetary value at which the participant was indifferent to the two options (Fig. 2C).
Consistent with the results of study 3, participants showed a consistent preference for answering questions about the self, and this preference was augmented when participants knew they were communicating their answers to another person. When payoff amounts were equal, participants chose to answer a self shared question 69% of the time, an other shared question 67% of the time, and a self private question 62% of the time. Only on other private questions did participant choice behavior not differ significantly from chance (57%). Analysis of PSE scores revealed both a main effect of self over other [F(1,40) = 5.08, P = 0.03, d = 0.36] and a main effect of sharing over private [F(1,40) = 8.71, P = 0.005, d = 0.47] but no interaction between the two factors [F(1,40) = 0.37, P = 0.55, d = 0.10]. As displayed in Fig. 2D, this pattern of results was strikingly similar to that obtained using neural data in study 3: participants were willing to forgo money to introspect about the self, and even more money when they were able to disclose the results of such introspection to another person. Indeed, this drive for self-disclosure led to an average loss of ∼25% of potential earnings on self shared trials. As it happened, participants valued sharing a response about the self at just under one cent (0.97¢), putting a new twist on the old phrase “a penny for your thoughts.”
Discussion
Despite the frequency with which humans disclose the contents of their own thoughts, little has been known about the proximate mechanisms that motivate this behavior. Here, we suggest that humans so willingly self-disclose because doing so represents an event with intrinsic value, in the same way as with primary rewards such as food and sex. Intriguingly, findings also suggested that both parts of “self-disclosure” have reward value. Although participants were willing to forgo money merely to introspect about the self and doing so was sufficient to engage brain regions associated with the rewarding outcomes, these effects were magnified by knowledge that one’s thoughts would be communicated to another person, suggesting that individuals find opportunities to disclose their own thoughts to others to be especially rewarding.
The present research follows from a rich history of investigations into self-disclosure and serves to both validate and extend these previous findings. For example, cognitive research into self-disclosure has identified factors that affect the likelihood that individuals will self-disclose, such as one’s feeling toward the audience (50) or the intimate nature of the disclosure (2). Other studies have suggested a number of instrumental reasons that people might choose to self-disclose, including expectations for reciprocation (51, 52), benefits to personal well-being (53, 54), and increased liking between relationship partners (50). Interestingly, a number of earlier researchers have put forward the hypothesis explicitly tested here—that self-disclosure will act as an intrinsic reward (51, 55, 56); however, despite calls to do so (56), this notion has not previously been tested empirically. As such, the current study validates a long-standing hypothesis that self-disclosure arises—at least in part—from the subjective value associated with it.
In an ultimate sense, the tendency to broadcast one’s thoughts and beliefs may confer an adaptive advantage in individuals in a number of ways: by engendering social bonds and social alliances between people (50, 57, 58); by eliciting feedback from others to attain self-knowledge (59); by taking advantage of performance advantages that result from sharing one’s sensory experience (60); or by obviating the need to discover firsthand what others already know, thus expanding the amount of know-how any single person can acquire in a lifetime. As such, the proximate motivation to disclose our internal thoughts and knowledge to others around us may serve to sustain the behaviors that underlie the extreme sociality of our species.
Materials and Methods
Studies 1a and 1b.
Seventy-eight individuals participated in study 1a and 117 individuals participated in study 1b. Informed consent was obtained from all participants in a manner approved by the Human Studies Committee of the Massachusetts General Hospital or by the Committee on the Use of Human Subjects at Harvard University. Studies 1a and 1b collectively comprise 14 separate data collections, including 4 published previously (Table S3). In study 1a, participants alternately disclosed their own opinions or judged the opinions of others (30, 31). On each trial, participants first saw a cue for 500 ms that indicated the target of the judgment: either a chalk outline of a head used to indicate self or a photograph of another person. The identity of the other varied across data collections, but was always a fictional, unfamiliar target. The visual cue was presented above a brief phrase that interrogated opinions and dispositions (e.g., “prefer coffee over tea” or “get frustrated sitting in traffic”). Participants used a 4- or 5-point Likert scale to indicate how well the statement described their own opinion or that of the other person within a response window of 3,500 ms.
In study 1b, participants alternately disclosed their beliefs about their own personality traits or judged the traits of another person. The other person was the President of the United States at the time of data collection (George W. Bush or Barack Obama), a choice guided by earlier behavioral (61, 62) and neuroimaging (63) studies of self-referential thought (for review, see ref. 34). On each trial, participants saw a cue for 500 ms that indicated the target of the judgment (“self,” “Bush,” “Obama”) above a personality trait (e.g., “curious” or “ambitious”), and used a Likert scale to indicate how well the trait described the target within a response window of 3,500 ms.
In both studies, trials were separated by a variable intertrial interval (ITI) of 0–10 s (64). Functional data were acquired using a gradient-echo echo-planar pulse sequence [repetition time (TR) = 2,000 ms; echo time (TE) = 35 ms; 1.5T Siemens Sonata or 3T Siemens Trio scanners; 26 or 31 axial, interleaved slices, 5 mm thick; 3.75 × 3.75 or 3 × 3 in-plane resolution; see Table S3 for design features of all data acquisitions]. Functional images were preprocessed and analyzed using Statistical Parametric Mapping (SPM) (Wellcome Department of Cognitive Neurology). Data were preprocessed to correct for slice time acquisition differences in each whole-brain volume and spatially realigned to correct for head movement. Images were normalized to a standard anatomical space (2-mm isotropic voxels) on the basis of the ICBM 152 brain template (Montreal Neurological Institute) and then spatially smoothed using an 8-mm FWHM Gaussian kernel. Preprocessed images were analyzed using a general linear model in which trials were modeled as an event with no duration that onset at the presentation of the cue. Trials were conditionalized on the basis of the target: self or other. Events were modeled using a canonical hemodynamic response function and its temporal derivative. Analyses included covariates of no interest (session mean and linear trend, no response trials, and their temporal derivative). Analyses were performed individually for each participant, and contrast images were subsequently entered into a second-level analysis treating participants as a random effect. At the group level, analyses were conducted using a statistical criterion of 57 or more contiguous voxels at a voxel-wise threshold of P < 0.001, providing an experiment-wise threshold of P < 0.05, corrected for multiple comparisons per Slotnick and Schacter’s (65) specifications.
Study 3.
Seventeen individuals participated in study 3. Informed consent was obtained from all participants in a manner approved by the Committee on the Use of Human Subjects at Harvard University. Participants in this study alternately disclosed their own opinions and attitudes (self trials) or judged the opinions of another person, Barack Obama (other trials). Participants arrived at the laboratory in the company of a friend or relative, and their answers to half the questions were shared with that other person (shared trials), whereas answers to the other half remained private (private trials). On each trial, participants first saw a two-word cue for 1,500 ms that indicated both the target of the judgment (self or other) and whether the answer would be shared or would remain private (share or private), followed by a 3,500-ms response window. Participants answered 48 of each of the four possible trial types: self shared, self private, other shared, and other private. Questions generally asked about fairly mundane opinions, such as those involving food preferences or everyday activities, and avoided issues that were controversial or potentially embarrassing to participants.
Functional data were acquired using a gradient-echo echo-planar pulse sequence (TR = 2,500 ms; TE = 30 ms; 3T Siemens Trio scanners; 42 axial, interleaved slices, 0 skip; 2-mm isotropic voxels). Data acquisition was optimized for signal extraction from the brain regions of interest: the NAcc and the VTA. These acquisition parameters allowed for partial brain coverage; the volumes acquired excluded only the most dorsal/posterior extent of the occipital and parietal cortex. Trials were separated by a variable ITI of 0–12.5 s. Functional images were preprocessed and analyzed using SPM (Wellcome Department of Cognitive Neurology) as for studies 1a and 1b. At the group level, an experiment-wise threshold of P < 0.05 corrected for multiple comparisons per Slotnick and Schacter’s (65) specifications; because of the limited field of view and smaller voxel size, Monte Carlo simulations indicated use of a statistical criterion of 15 or more contiguous voxels at a voxel-wise threshold of P < 0.01.
To enable an analysis of native space anatomical regions of interest, a proton-density-weighted image (TR 6,000 ms, TE 8.6 ms, flip angle 180°, echo spacing 8.62 ms) was acquired with the same orientation and slice thickness as the functional images (42 slices, 2 mm thick, 0.9 mm in-plane spatial resolution) immediately following acquisition of functional data. These images were used to localize the VTA and bilateral NAcc individually for each subject using the manual segmentation tool in the ITK-SNAP segmentation software. Anatomical regions of interest were then aligned with the functional data by applying parameters from the coregistration of the anatomical image to the first image of the functional task. To functionally localize brain regions associated with the processing of rewarding simuli, participants completed a MID task (66). We defined ROIs by comparing trials on which participants won money to trials in which participants could not earn any reward (See SI Materials and Methods for further details). We extracted parameter estimates (βs) from the anatomically defined reward regions for the self shared, self private, other shared, and other private trials in study 3, and from the functionally defined NAcc regions for the self and other trials in studies 1a and 1b, and for the self shared, self private, other shared, and other private trials in study 3.
Studies 2 and 4.
Thirty-seven individuals participated in study 2 and 41 individuals participated in study 4. Informed consent was obtained from all participants in a manner approved by the Committee on the Use of Human Subjects at Harvard University. The PSE for each question type was calculated individually for each participant by fitting a cumulative normal distribution to the participant’s choice behavior. In study 2, at each value for the relative payoff between two question types, we first calculated the percentage of trials in which participants chose (i) to answer a question about the self over a question about a fact, (ii) to answer a question about the self over a question about another person, and (iii) to answer a question about another person over a question about a fact. The other person in studies 2 and 4 was always the current head of state at the time of data collection, Barack Obama. In study 4, at each value for the relative payoff between waiting and answering one of the four question types (self share, self private, other share, and other private), we calculated the percentage of trials in which a participant chose to answer that question type over waiting. This resulted in a total of 7 data points per subject, one at each of the relative payoffs between the two options, as relative payoffs ranged between −$0.03 and +$0.03. Cumulative normal curves were fit to these values by implementing a Nelder-Mead simplex search algorithm in Matlab. Cumulative normal distributions are defined on the basis of the following probability density function:
Starting values for this estimation were set at a mean of 0 and an SD of 1, and the search continued for 10,000 iterations, or until a solution was achieved. Final solutions of PSE were limited to a range of −$0.03 and +$0.03.
Acknowledgments
The authors thank Zach Alexander, Kimberlee D’Ardenne, Juan Manuel Contreras, Richard Hackman, Joe Moran, Kenneth Parreno, Emma Templeton, Adam Waytz, and Jamil Zaki for helpful advice and assistance. D.I.T. was supported by a graduate research fellowship from the National Science Foundation.
Supporting Information
Supporting Information (PDF)
Supporting Information
- Download
- 221.65 KB
References
1
RIM Dunbar, A Marriott, NDC Duncan, Human conversational behavior. Hum Nat 8, 231–246 (1997).
2
N Emler, A social psychology of reputation. Eur Rev Soc Psychol 1, 171–193 (1990).
3
N Emler, Gossip, reputation, and social adaptation. Good Gossip, eds R Goodman, A Ben Ze’ev (Kansas University Press, Lawrence, KS), pp. 117–133 (1994).
4
MH Landis, HE Burtt, A study of conversations. J Comp Psychol 4, 81–89 (1924).
5
M Naaman, J Boase, CH Lai, Is it really about me?: Message content in social awareness streams. Proceedings of the 2010 ACM Conference on Computer Supported Cooperative Work (Association for Computing Machinery, Savannah, GA), pp. 189–192 (2010).
6
M Tomasello The Cultural Origins of Human Cognition (Harvard University Press, Cambridge, MA, 1999).
7
G Csibra, G Gergely, Natural pedagogy as evolutionary adaptation. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci 366, 1149–1157 (2011).
8
W Schultz, Getting formal with dopamine and reward. Neuron 36, 241–263 (2002).
9
PR Montague, GS Berns, Neural economics and the biological substrates of valuation. Neuron 36, 265–284 (2002).
10
SM McClure, MK York, PR Montague, The neural substrates of reward processing in humans: The modern role of FMRI. Neuroscientist 10, 260–268 (2004).
11
SN Haber, B Knutson, The reward circuit: Linking primate anatomy and human imaging. Neuropsychopharmacology 35, 4–26 (2010).
12
K D’Ardenne, SM McClure, LE Nystrom, JD Cohen, BOLD responses reflecting dopaminergic signals in the human ventral tegmental area. Science 319, 1264–1267 (2008).
13
DM Small, RJ Zatorre, A Dagher, AC Evans, M Jones-Gotman, Changes in brain activity related to eating chocolate: From pleasure to aversion. Brain 124, 1720–1733 (2001).
14
JP O’Doherty, R Deichmann, HD Critchley, RJ Dolan, Neural responses during anticipation of a primary taste reward. Neuron 33, 815–826 (2002).
15
L Hernandez, BG Hoebel, Food reward and cocaine increase extracellular dopamine in the nucleus accumbens as measured by microdialysis. Life Sci 42, 1705–1712 (1988).
16
BH Schott, et al., Mesolimbic functional magnetic resonance imaging activations during reward anticipation correlate with reward-related ventral striatal dopamine release. J Neurosci 28, 14311–14319 (2008).
17
B Knutson, J Taylor, M Kaufman, R Peterson, G Glover, Distributed neural representation of expected value. J Neurosci 25, 4806–4812 (2005).
18
B Knutson, CM Adams, GW Fong, D Hommer, Anticipation of increasing monetary reward selectively recruits nucleus accumbens. J Neurosci 21, 159–159 (2001).
19
SM McClure, DI Laibson, G Loewenstein, JD Cohen, Separate neural systems value immediate and delayed monetary rewards. Science 306, 503–507 (2004).
20
MJ Koepp, et al., Evidence for striatal dopamine release during a video game. Nature 393, 266–268 (1998).
21
MX Cohen, et al., Neuroelectric signatures of reward learning and decision-making in the human nucleus accumbens. Neuropsychopharmacology 34, 1649–1658 (2009).
22
HC Breiter, JD Berke, WA Kennedy, BR Rosen, SE Hyman, Activation of striatum and amygdala during reward conditioning: An FMRI study. Neuroimage 3, S220 (1996).
23
I Aharon, et al., Beautiful faces have variable reward value: fMRI and behavioral evidence. Neuron 32, 537–551 (2001).
24
D Mobbs, MD Greicius, E Abdel-Azim, V Menon, AL Reiss, Humor modulates the mesolimbic reward centers. Neuron 40, 1041–1048 (2003).
25
E Fehr, CF Camerer, Social neuroeconomics: The neural circuitry of social preferences. Trends Cogn Sci 11, 419–427 (2007).
26
V Klucharev, K Hytönen, M Rijpkema, A Smidts, G Fernández, Reinforcement learning signal predicts social conformity. Neuron 61, 140–151 (2009).
27
D Sabatinelli, MM Bradley, PJ Lang, VD Costa, F Versace, Pleasure rather than salience activates human nucleus accumbens and medial prefrontal cortex. J Neurophysiol 98, 1374–1379 (2007).
28
RO Deaner, AV Khera, ML Platt, Monkeys pay per view: Adaptive valuation of social images by rhesus macaques. Curr Biol 15, 543–548 (2005).
29
BY Hayden, PC Parikh, RO Deaner, ML Platt, Economic principles motivating social attention in humans. Proc Biol Sci 274, 1751–1756 (2007).
30
JP Mitchell, CN Macrae, MR Banaji, Dissociable medial prefrontal contributions to judgments of similar and dissimilar others. Neuron 50, 655–663 (2006).
31
DI Tamir, JP Mitchell, Neural correlates of anchoring-and-adjustment during mentalizing. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 107, 10827–10832 (2010).
32
G Northoff, F Bermpohl, Cortical midline structures and the self. Trends Cogn Sci 8, 102–107 (2004).
33
G Northoff, et al., Self-referential processing in our brain: A meta-analysis of imaging studies on the self. Neuroimage 31, 440–457 (2006).
34
JP Mitchell, Social psychology as a natural kind. Trends Cogn Sci 13, 246–251 (2009).
35
TW Schmitz, HA Rowley, TN Kawahara, SC Johnson, Neural correlates of self-evaluative accuracy after traumatic brain injury. Neuropsychologia 44, 762–773 (2006).
36
JH Pfeifer, MD Lieberman, M Dapretto, “I know you are but what am I?!”: Neural bases of self- and social knowledge retrieval in children and adults. J Cogn Neurosci 19, 1323–1337 (2007).
37
AH Gutchess, EA Kensinger, DL Schacter, Aging, self-referencing, and medial prefrontal cortex. Soc Neurosci 2, 117–133 (2007).
38
CN Macrae, JM Moran, TF Heatherton, JF Banfield, WM Kelley, Medial prefrontal activity predicts memory for self. Cereb Cortex 14, 647–654 (2004).
39
AC Jenkins, JP Mitchell, Medial prefrontal cortex subserves diverse forms of self-reflection. Soc Neurosci 12, 1–8 (2011).
40
JP O’Doherty, P Dayan, K Friston, H Critchley, RJ Dolan, Temporal difference models and reward-related learning in the human brain. Neuron 38, 329–337 (2003).
41
A Litt, H Plassmann, B Shiv, A Rangel, Dissociating valuation and saliency signals during decision-making. Cereb Cortex 21, 95–102 (2011).
42
SM Nicola, The flexible approach hypothesis: Unification of effort and cue-responding hypotheses for the role of nucleus accumbens dopamine in the activation of reward-seeking behavior. J Neurosci 30, 16585–16600 (2010).
43
B Seymour, N Daw, P Dayan, T Singer, R Dolan, Differential encoding of losses and gains in the human striatum. J Neurosci 27, 4826–4831 (2007).
44
CF Zink, G Pagnoni, ME Martin, M Dhamala, GS Berns, Human striatal response to salient nonrewarding stimuli. J Neurosci 23, 8092–8097 (2003).
45
N Epley, E Whitchurch, Mirror, mirror on the wall: Enhancement in self-recognition. Pers Soc Psychol Bull 34, 1159–1170 (2008).
46
AG Greenwald, et al., A unified theory of implicit attitudes, stereotypes, self-esteem, and self-concept. Psychol Rev 109, 3–25 (2002).
47
BW Pelham, MC Mirenberg, JT Jones, Why Susie sells seashells by the seashore: Implicit egotism and major life decisions. J Pers Soc Psychol 82, 469–487 (2002).
48
D Kahneman, JL Knetsch, RH Thaler, Anomalies: The endowment effect, loss aversion, and status quo bias. J Econ Perspect 5, 193–206 (1991).
49
CK Morewedge, LL Shu, DT Gilbert, TD Wilson, Bad riddance or good rubbish? Ownership and not loss aversion causes the endowment effect. J Exp Soc Psychol 45, 947–951 (2009).
50
NL Collins, LC Miller, Self-disclosure and liking: A meta-analytic review. Psychol Bull 116, 457–475 (1994).
51
PC Cozby, Self-disclosure, reciprocity and liking. Sociometry 35, 151–160 (1972).
52
HA McAllister, Self-disclosure and liking: Effects for senders and receivers. J Pers 48, 409–418 (1980).
53
HC Ko, TK Chen, Understanding the continuous self-disclosure of bloggers from the cost-benefit perspective. Proceedings of the Second Conference on Human Systems Interactions (Institute of Electrical and Electronics Engineers, Cantania, Italy), pp. 520–527 (2009).
54
KG Niederhoffer, JW Pennebaker, Sharing one’s story: On the benefits of writing or talking about emotional experience. Handbook of Positive Psychology, eds CR Snyder, SJ Lopez (Oxford University Press, New York, 2002).
55
PC Cozby, Self-disclosure: A literature review. Psychol Bull 79, 73–91 (1973).
56
J Omarzu, A disclosure decision model: Determining how and when individuals will self-disclose. Pers Soc Psychol Rev 4, 174–185 (2000).
57
RIM Dunbar Grooming, Gossip, and the Evolution of Language (Harvard University Press, Cambridge, MA, 1996).
58
K Dindia, Sex differences in self-disclosure, reciprocity of self-disclosure, and self-disclosure and liking: Three meta-analyses reviewed. Balancing the Secrets of Private Disclosures, ed S Petronio (Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Mahwah, NJ), pp. 21–36 (2000).
59
CH Cooley Human Nature and the Social Order (Charles Scribner’s, New York), pp. 136–178 (1902).
60
B Bahrami, et al., Optimally interacting minds. Science 329, 1081–1085 (2010).
61
GH Bowers, SG Gilligan, Remembering information related to one’s self. J Res Pers 13, 420–432 (1979).
62
TB Rogers, NA Kuiper, WS Kirker, Self-reference and the encoding of personal information. J Pers Soc Psychol 35, 677–688 (1977).
63
WM Kelley, et al., Finding the self? An event-related fMRI study. J Cogn Neurosci 14, 785–794 (2002).
64
AM Dale, Optimal experimental design for event-related fMRI. Hum Brain Mapp 8, 109–114 (1999).
65
SD Slotnick, DL Schacter, A sensory signature that distinguishes true from false memories. Nat Neurosci 7, 664–672 (2004).
66
B Knutson, A Westdorp, E Kaiser, D Hommer, FMRI visualization of brain activity during a monetary incentive delay task. Neuroimage 12, 20–27 (2000).
Information & Authors
Information
Published in
Classifications
Submission history
Published online: May 7, 2012
Published in issue: May 22, 2012
Keywords
Acknowledgments
The authors thank Zach Alexander, Kimberlee D’Ardenne, Juan Manuel Contreras, Richard Hackman, Joe Moran, Kenneth Parreno, Emma Templeton, Adam Waytz, and Jamil Zaki for helpful advice and assistance. D.I.T. was supported by a graduate research fellowship from the National Science Foundation.
Notes
This article is a PNAS Direct Submission.
Authors
Competing Interests
The authors declare no conflict of interest.
Metrics & Citations
Metrics
Citation statements
Altmetrics
Citations
If you have the appropriate software installed, you can download article citation data to the citation manager of your choice. Simply select your manager software from the list below and click Download.
Cited by
Loading...
View Options
View options
PDF format
Download this article as a PDF file
DOWNLOAD PDFLogin options
Check if you have access through your login credentials or your institution to get full access on this article.
Personal login Institutional LoginRecommend to a librarian
Recommend PNAS to a LibrarianPurchase options
Purchase this article to access the full text.