No strong evidence that authoritarian attitudes are driven by a lack of control

January 23, 2018
115 (6) E1076-E1077
Research Article
When the appeal of a dominant leader is greater than a prestige leader
Hemant Kakkar, Niro Sivanathan
Kakkar and Sivanathan (1) provide new evidence of the link between economic crises and the preference for dominant leaders and strengthen previous evidence of the influence of economic crises on authoritarianism (25). However, the authors’ interpretation of why economic uncertainty favors dominant leaders entails two further predictions that are not borne out by our follow-up analyses of their dataset.
According to the authors, individuals choose more dominant leaders in times of uncertainty “to reduce the aversive state of low personal control when plagued by collective uncertainty”; in other words, “seek[ing] a dominant leader [is] a compensatory strategy to restore their sense of personal control” (1). This hypothesis predicts that people should prefer dominant leaders compared with prestigious leaders but also compared with having no leader at all. However, while study 1 reveals that economic uncertainty increases the preference for dominant compared with prestigious leaders, it has no impact on the probability of choosing a dominant leader compared with no leader at all (b = 0.24, P > 0.250). It thus appears that economic uncertainty only influences the type of leader that is preferred and does not trigger a compensatory strategy that would lead people to seek dominant leaders in general.
Another important point in Kakkar and Sivanathan’s demonstration is the predictive power of the lack of control in leader preferences (1). However, our follow-up analysis of their data does not provide strong evidence for their claim. Running a Bayes factor analysis on the World Value Survey data indeed fails to reveal evidence in favor of the model including sense of control compared with the model with control variables only or to those also including change in unemployment (Bayes factors < 1.19; Fig. 1). This analysis nonetheless confirmed the explanatory power of economic uncertainty as the model including change in unemployment was substantially more probable than the one that only included control variables (Bayes factor = 1.71e8). These additional analyses thus cast doubts on the role played by the sense of control in explaining the preference for strong leaders in times of economic uncertainty.
Fig. 1.
Bayes factor analysis of the explanatory power of change in unemployment, lack of control, and interpersonal distrust on the preference for dominant leaders. There is no evidence of the power of the sense of lack of control (Lack of Control) in explaining the preference for dominant leaders both alone and combined with the change in unemployment (∆Unemployment). By contrast, including change in unemployment and interpersonal distrust (Distrust) significantly increases the probability of the control model (including only the control variables) both separately and together.
On the contrary, an increased perceived adaptiveness of dominant features, such as untrustworthiness, in uncertain contexts may provide a better explanation of the effect reported by Kakkar and Sivanathan (1). For instance, including interpersonal distrust increases by more than 10,000 times the probability of the model with the control variables only and that of the model including economic uncertainty (Bayes factor > 1.00e26). Importantly, further analyses confirmed that interpersonal distrust mediated the effect of economic uncertainty on leader preferences (b = 0.002, P < 0.001; proportion mediated: 9%).
Based on these new analyses, it thus appears that economic uncertainty influences individuals’ preferred type of leader by shaping their perception of the environment (3, 6) rather than by increasing the absolute need of a strong leader who would contribute to restoring people’s decreased sense of control.

References

1
H Kakkar, N Sivanathan, When the appeal of a dominant leader is greater than a prestige leader. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 114, 6734–6739 (2017).
2
E Onraet, K Dhont, A Van Hiel, The relationships between internal and external threats and right-wing attitudes: A three-wave longitudinal study. Pers Soc Psychol Bull 40, 712–725 (2014).
3
L Safra, et al., Childhood harshness predicts long-lasting leader preferences. Evol Hum Behav 38, 645–651 (2017).
4
RM Doty, BE Peterson, DG Winter, Threat and authoritarianism in the United States, 1978–1987. J Pers Soc Psychol 61, 629–640 (1991).
5
A Mirisola, M Roccato, S Russo, G Spagna, A Vieno, Societal threat to safety, compensatory control, and right-wing authoritarianism. Polit Psychol 35, 795–812 (2014).
6
R Perry, CG Sibley, J Duckitt, Dangerous and competitive worldviews: A meta-analysis of their associations with social dominance orientation and right-wing authoritarianism. J Res Pers 47, 116–127 (2013).

Information & Authors

Information

Published in

The cover image for PNAS Vol.115; No.6
Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences
Vol. 115 | No. 6
February 6, 2018
PubMed: 29363605

Classifications

Submission history

Published online: January 23, 2018
Published in issue: February 6, 2018

Authors

Affiliations

Evolution and Social Cognition Group, Laboratoire de Neurosciences Cognitives, Département d’Etudes Cognitives, INSERM U960, Ecole Normale Supérieure, Université de Recherche Paris Sciences et Lettres, F-75005 Paris, France;
Evolution and Social Cognition Group, Institut Jean Nicod, Département d’Etudes Cognitives, CNRS UMR8129, Ecole Normale Supérieure, Université de Recherche Paris Sciences et Lettres, F-75005 Paris, France
Nicolas Baumard
Evolution and Social Cognition Group, Institut Jean Nicod, Département d’Etudes Cognitives, CNRS UMR8129, Ecole Normale Supérieure, Université de Recherche Paris Sciences et Lettres, F-75005 Paris, France
Coralie Chevallier
Evolution and Social Cognition Group, Laboratoire de Neurosciences Cognitives, Département d’Etudes Cognitives, INSERM U960, Ecole Normale Supérieure, Université de Recherche Paris Sciences et Lettres, F-75005 Paris, France;

Notes

1
To whom correspondence should be addressed. Email: [email protected].
Author contributions: L.S. designed the analyses; L.S. analyzed data; and L.S., N.B., and C.C. wrote the paper.

Competing Interests

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

Metrics & Citations

Metrics

Note: The article usage is presented with a three- to four-day delay and will update daily once available. Due to ths delay, usage data will not appear immediately following publication. Citation information is sourced from Crossref Cited-by service.


Citation statements




Altmetrics

Citations

Export the article citation data by selecting a format from the list below and clicking Export.

Cited by

    Loading...

    View Options

    View options

    PDF format

    Download this article as a PDF file

    DOWNLOAD PDF

    Login options

    Check if you have access through your login credentials or your institution to get full access on this article.

    Personal login Institutional Login

    Recommend to a librarian

    Recommend PNAS to a Librarian

    Purchase options

    Purchase this article to access the full text.

    Single Article Purchase

    No strong evidence that authoritarian attitudes are driven by a lack of control
    Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences
    • Vol. 115
    • No. 6
    • pp. 1129-E1330

    Media

    Figures

    Tables

    Other

    Share

    Share

    Share article link

    Share on social media