Personal hardship narrows the partisan gap in COVID-19 and climate change responses
Contributed by Elke U. Weber; received November 12, 2021; accepted September 19, 2022; reviewed by Megan Mullin and Paul Slovic
This contribution is part of the special series of Inaugural Articles by members of the National Academy of Sciences elected in 2020.
Significance
In highly polarized political environments like the United States, people interpret health and environmental issues through a partisan lens. Democrats have been more concerned about COVID-19 and more willing to socially distance than Republicans—resulting in a substantial partisan gap in concern, action, and policy support. However, using cross-sectional comparisons from a three-wave survey during March to November 2020, we find that self-reported hardship due to COVID-19 is associated with a narrowing of the partisan gap and does not depend on party affiliation. We find similar results in the context of climate change. These findings suggest that personal experience may counter partisan messaging and worldviews, at least in response to an immediate threat.
Abstract
The COVID-19 pandemic in the United States was characterized by a partisan gap. Democrats were more concerned about this novel health threat, more willing to socially distance, and more likely to support policies aimed at mitigating the spread of the virus than Republicans. In cross-sectional analyses of three nationally representative survey waves in 2020, we find that adverse experience with COVID-19 is associated with a narrowing of the partisan gap. The mean difference between Republicans and Democrats in concern, policy support, and behavioral intentions narrows or even disappears at high levels of self-reported adverse experience. Reported experience does not depend on party affiliation and is predicted by local COVID-19 incidence rates. In contrast, analyses of longitudinal data and county-level incidence rates do not show a consistent relationship among experience, partisanship, and behavior or policy support. Our findings suggest that self-reported personal experience interacts with partisanship in complex ways and may be an important channel for concern about novel threats such as the COVID-19 pandemic. We find consistent results for self-reported experience of extreme weather events and climate change attitudes and policy preferences, although the association between extreme weather and experience and climate change is more tenuous.
Sign up for PNAS alerts.
Get alerts for new articles, or get an alert when an article is cited.
The COVID-19 pandemic serves as a unique opportunity for social scientists to examine the public interpretation of a rapidly unfolding global threat in a highly polarized society. When the COVID-19 pandemic was declared in March 2020, the Republican Trump Administration and the Democratic Party leadership took diametrically opposed views about the origin and magnitude of the public health emergency. Beliefs, risk perceptions, and policy preferences became quickly and deeply divided along partisan lines. Meanwhile, experience with the virus was unevenly distributed across the United States, raising the possibility that emerging divisions reflected differences in both partisanship and experience in complex ways. As the virus spread from more Democratic parts of the country to more Republican ones, the shared negative experience with this novel threat grew, but so did partisan polarization.
The similarities and differences between the COVID-19 crisis and climate change offer an opportunity for comparative analysis (1). Both issues require coordinated responses to mitigate risks but they also face partisan polarization around the severity of the threat and necessary responses. However, personal experience with COVID-19 is immediate, salient, and more easily linked to a proximal cause, while the attribution of extreme weather impacts to anthropogenic climate change is more complicated.
We draw on literature from psychology and political science to understand the roles of experience and partisanship in shaping attitudes and beliefs. Adverse personal experience is an effective teacher in many contexts: “seeing is believing” (2). Yet studies of perceptual dynamics in the context of climate change suggest that ideological priors may color the interpretation of events and act as filters on experience: “believing is seeing” (3, 4). Indeed, research on climate change finds evidence for both dynamics. Experience can reinforce preexisting concern, but preexisting concern may also shape one’s experience, interpretation, attribution, and even memory of a natural hazard (5). However, this literature suffers from limited availability of longitudinal data and information about experience intensity and tends to focus on climate change concern rather than behavior or policy preferences (6).
In this study, we focus on how partisanship and experience interact to predict worry, self-reported behaviors, and policy preferences around the novel, immediate threat of COVID-19 and the long-term threat of climate change. We administered three consecutive survey waves to a large representative sample of Americans, starting shortly after the World Health Organization declared COVID-19 a global pandemic and at two later time points before vaccines became available.
In our cross-sectional analyses, we find evidence that both experience and partisanship play a role in responses to these two public threats. Adverse experience with COVID-19 is associated with a narrowing of the mean difference between Republican and Democrat responses to questions about worry, action, and policy support (“partisan gap”), all of which increase more steeply for Republicans with self-reported negative experience than for Democrats. We also find that greater negative experience with natural hazards is associated with a narrowing of the partisan gap on key outcome variables in the context of climate change. Our findings are consistent with the theory that adverse consequences constitute novel information for Republicans because they contradict their prior views, and therefore lead to greater updating of prior beliefs, especially in situations where impacts are immediate and priors are less strong (7).
However, in longitudinal analyses assessing within-participant changes over time, the relationship between experience and our outcome variables is by and large no longer significant. Our primary measure of experience—self-reported hardship—may be endogenous to partisanship in complicated ways. Preexisting attitudes, beliefs, and preferences, including partisanship, may shape both how individuals experience the world around them and how they report that experience. We find that county-level COVID-19 incidence rates predict self-reported adverse experience in similar ways for Democrats and Republicans once we control for demographic information. Nevertheless, Republicans who live in areas with higher COVID-19 incidence rates may be systematically different from Republicans in other areas. We highlight issues of causal identification and opportunities for future research in the discussion. Our results indicate that attitudes toward both the immediate public health threat of COVID-19 and the long-term threat of climate change are predicted by the interaction of partisan- and experience-based information processing channels.
Theory and Hypotheses
Experience Channel.
Extensive research in psychology focuses on how people update their beliefs when confronted with new information. One influential theory holds that people update their beliefs in proportion to the prediction error—the difference between prior expectations and present experience—allowing them to correct inaccurate expectations and adapt to a changing world (8). In these accounts, experience is generally taken to be veridical or objective, and new experience that deviates substantially from expectations results in a strong updating signal.
A separate literature in psychology has identified different “modes” by which individuals process information (9, 10). The analytic and affective modes shape how people evaluate information accessed by direct experience. The affective decision mode uses human associative processing (11), which operates quickly and automatically in response to threats, converting adverse experience into affective responses (e.g., fear, dread, anxiety), and thus represents risk as a feeling rather than as a probability (12, 13). The calculation-based decision mode uses analytic processes that, in contrast, operate more slowly and require conscious effort and control. Personal adverse experience may thus lead to the updating of expectations but also drive protective actions through worry and fear responses.
A substantial body of evidence points to the importance of personal experience with extreme weather events and local temperature abnormalities for climate concern (14–17), behavioral intentions, and policy support (18). However, there is also evidence that learning from experience depends on an individual’s prior beliefs in ways different from the rational updating process described above. People do not impartially detect changes in their local environments. Instead, contextual and cultural factors and motivational drivers shape how people seek (or avoid), attend to, process, interpret, remember, and report new information (19–21). People tend to process information in a way that favors desired conclusions or beliefs, especially when these are strongly held, leading them to accept confirmatory evidence at face value while ignoring or challenging conflicting evidence (22). Such processes can result in motivated reasoning, leading individuals to reject new information when it challenges their beliefs. They also shape how people understand and evaluate new information (23). In the context of climate change, extreme weather is only “evidence” for climate change if people believe that the two are causally related (24, 25). Stronger belief in climate change may make people more likely to look for and thus see evidence supporting it (5, 6).
Using a longitudinal dataset to distinguish between evidence-based revision of beliefs and belief-motivated perceptions, Myers et al. (3) found evidence for belief-motivated perceptions in people with strong ideological commitments to climate change and evidence-based revision of beliefs among the uncommitted middle. These results are directionally consistent with Bayesian updating (26), such that beliefs should change with disconfirming evidence quickly when initial beliefs are weak and more slowly when beliefs are deeply held (14). Using signal detection theory, Dryden et al. (27) showed that attribution of hurricanes to climate change depends on preexisting climate beliefs, which influence the threshold used to classify hurricanes as “normal” vs. “evidence of climate change.” Respondents who were less certain about the existence of climate change required more evidence before attributing an unusual hurricane season to climate change. Prior beliefs thus acted as a lens through which experience was interpreted rather than blocking it altogether (27).
Partisan Channel.
When belief-motivated perceptions are shaped by salient partisan identities, this interpretive filter is called “partisan-motivated reasoning” (28). Partisan-motivated reasoning shapes public opinion on a variety of social issues (28–30), including climate change (23, 31). Political ideology shapes interpretations of local temperature abnormalities and perceptions of the scientific consensus on climate change (15, 32). Research with farmers found that their recall of local temperature conditions was biased by their preexisting beliefs about climate change and political party (33). Partisan identities can overtake stated policy preferences, with individuals acting against their beliefs or preferences when receiving conflicting information from a trusted partisan source (34). For example, endorsement of climate action by copartisan messengers increases responsiveness to climate change, especially among Republicans (35, 36).
The partisan channel provides one explanation for polarized public views on both COVID-19 and climate change, despite mounting evidence, including through direct exposure, of their adverse consequences. Party affiliation predicts social distancing in response to COVID-19, measured through both survey responses and geolocation data (37–40). Throughout 2020, Democrats and Republicans continued to be divided on the utility and importance of masks and other protective measures recommended by epidemiologists, decisions to close or reopen local economies, and priorities embedded in the various COVID-19 stimulus plans. Likewise, perceptions and actions related to climate change vary significantly by partisan affiliation and political ideology (5, 41, 42). Democrats are more inclined than Republicans to believe that anthropogenic climate change is occurring and are more worried about it (43).
Based on this literature, we hypothesize that there will be substantial partisan polarization on our key outcome measures for both COVID-19 and climate change—worry, protective behaviors, and policy support—and that messaging by copartisan elites will shape policy support, especially among Republicans who are otherwise more likely to be opposed to these measures.
A) Republicans will report less concern and support for mitigative measures than Democrats. B) Republicans will be more likely to endorse contentious COVID-19 or climate policies when they are proposed by copartisans, and this copartisan effect will be muted for Democrats.
Interaction of Channels.
“Who you gonna believe, me or your own eyes?” is an old line attributed to Chico Marx of the Marx Brothers that serves as a metaphor for our theorizing in this paper. When individuals are confronted with novel, complex, and threatening situations, how do their experience and partisan worldviews interact to shape beliefs and preferences? When the partisan and experience channels contradict one another, which gives?
A study of Phoenix, AZ, residents found that political ideology predicted perceptions of regional temperature change, but that objective temperature variation predicted perceptions of local temperature changes in one’s neighborhood (44). These results suggest that prior beliefs affect individuals’ interpretation of ambiguous, distal, or abstract information, while personal experience may supersede prior beliefs when events are proximal, concrete, and immediate. The emergence of and subsequent polarization around COVID-19 offers a unique opportunity to study these interacting dynamics in the context of a consequential and rapidly spreading threat. In this context, experience with the virus was concrete and immediate, and it could either confirm or disconfirm the worldviews put forth by a respondent’s self-reported political group.
Experience is a powerful teacher and can change attitudes and actions. This is especially true when threats are immediate, concrete, and local, as was the case for COVID-19 in the first half of 2020. In view of strong polarization in initial beliefs, we hypothesize that personal experience with COVID-19 will play different roles among Democrats and Republicans. For Republicans, experience might provide a strong error correction signal, leading Republicans to update their beliefs that COVID-19 is a serious public health threat. By contradicting their initial beliefs, adverse experience provides novel information that leads to greater updating of prior beliefs. For Democrats, adverse experience with COVID-19 essentially confirms partisan messages and priors about the severity of the issue. Since Democrats are already worried and more likely to endorse personal and policy actions, we expect the impact of experience on these outcomes to be relatively weak or even nonexistent. We thus hypothesize that Republicans will respond more strongly to direct personal experience with the virus than Democrats.
Adverse experience with COVID-19 will predict greater worry about COVID-19; the strength of this relationship will be greater among Republicans than Democrats.
Adverse experience with COVID-19 will predict more personal protective actions, such as mask wearing and social distancing; the strength of this relationship will be greater among Republicans than Democrats.
Adverse experience with COVID-19 will predict greater support for policies aimed at mitigating the risks of COVID-19 and compensating victims of COVID-19; the strength of this relationship will be greater among Republicans than Democrats.
Nevertheless, where experiential and partisan signals contradict each other, we could observe the opposite. Motivated reasoning could lead Republicans to overlook negative experience with COVID-19, perceive it to be less adverse than a Democrat experiencing the same impacts, remember or report it differently, or attribute the impacts to causes that are consonant with their views. Recent work has identified partisan differences in accuracy and directional motivations in survey responses, such that partisans may respond along party lines despite dissenting personal beliefs or attitudes (45). In an exploratory analysis, we test for evidence of partisan-motivated reasoning in our measure of experience by assessing whether Republicans report less adverse experience with COVID-19 as a function of county-level incidence rates than Democrats, controlling for relevant demographic factors. Even with similar interpretations of experience, Republicans may require more evidence to update strong and contradictory priors relative to Democrats. These arguments, for different reasons, suggest mechanisms for the alternative hypotheses that experience would have little impact on Republicans or might widen the partisan gap. Our study design is unable to test a null hypothesis that experience has no effect; however, we calculate two-tailed P values to allow for the possibility of the opposite relationship in the interaction between partisanship and experience.
Personal experience with COVID-19 might also have broader impacts on perceptions of key institutions and actors involved in the crisis. Trust in government and related institutions is low in the United States right now (ref. 46, p. 104) and divided along partisan lines (SI Appendix, Fig. S8). In exploratory analyses, we investigate these broader effects of experience with COVID-19 by focusing on trust in the Centers for Disease Control and Prevention (CDC), news media, and scientists—actors who played prominent roles in COVID-19 response and communication. We conduct parallel analyses for climate change.
Methods
We analyze original data from three waves of a longitudinal US panel study (wave 1: 1–30 April 2020, n 5,059; wave 2: 15 June to 13 July 2020, n 5,194; wave 3: 9 October to 25 November 2020, n 3,777) collected from the Qualtrics participant panel.* Compensation was paid according to a preexisting agreement between participants and the survey provider, and was worth $4, on average. In addition, participants received a bonus of $5 for completing three waves of the survey. Each wave was sampled to match national distributions of gender, age, race, ethnicity, education, income, and region reported in the 2018 American Communities Survey. The data are also representative on party affiliation using the 2016 American National Election Survey statistics. This is a nonprobability sample, and quotas were not nested, so the relationships among these demographic characteristics may not be preserved. All participants gave informed consent.
Sample.
Our sample is longitudinal with replenishment. The first wave was composed of new participants, and each subsequent wave had new and repeat participants. The second and third waves were first populated by respondents from the previous wave and then replenished strategically to meet our quotas. The overall attrition rate (loss of repeat respondents) was 51% between wave 1 and wave 2 and 26% between wave 2 and wave 3.† Our sampling strategy means that, by wave 3, we had five distinct types of participants: those who completed all three waves (n 1,739), those who completed only waves 1 and 2 (n 1,078), those who completed only waves 2 and 3 (n 747), those who completed waves 1 and 3 (n 7), and those who only participated in a single wave (n 5,149 across the three waves).
Data.
The survey included six modules—extreme weather, climate change, COVID-19, trust and community, political attitudes, and demographics (see SI Appendix for question text)—repeated with minimal modification in each wave. Participants took 25 min, on average, to complete the survey. They were not informed of the module categories. The order of the climate change and COVID-19 blocks was randomized across participants.
Most analyses in the main text refer to data from June and July 2020 (wave 2), the period in which COVID-19 had spread across the United States. At this time, vaccines were not yet available, and the presidential election was a few months away. This is also the survey wave with the largest number of questions on a variety of attitudes, personally protective behaviors, and policy preferences. In SI Appendix, we show trends over time in our key explanatory and outcome variables across all three survey waves and conduct within-subjects analyses for variables present in multiple waves.
Our primary outcome variables are described in detail in SI Appendix, Fig. S1 for COVID-19 and SI Appendix, Fig. S2 for climate change. For example, worry about COVID-19 was answered on a four-point scale (“not at all” to “extremely”) in response to the question, “How worried are you about the coronavirus epidemic?” Worry about climate change was solicited on the same response scale but with a broader temporal focus in line with the longer-term nature of the hazard: “How worried are you about current and future global warming/climate change?”‡
Our primary explanatory variables are indices measuring a respondent’s self-reported negative experience of adversity with COVID-19, assessed by experience with natural hazards. Using multiple survey questions, we constructed separate indices of negative personal experience with 1) COVID-19 and 2) natural hazards. In each wave, we asked participants about the economic, health, psychological, and social impacts of COVID-19 and natural hazards that they had experienced. We summed responses to obtain indices of the total negative impacts of COVID-19 and natural hazards across various dimensions of an individual’s life. We asked respondents about experience with natural hazards over a 10-y period since these are low-frequency, high-impact events. We use the term “natural hazards” instead of “extreme weather events” because the list of hazards included earthquakes. We conduct additional analyses removing respondents who experienced earthquakes and report these in the SI Appendix. We took the natural log of the aggregate impact measures to account for their skewed distributions. Our primary COVID-19 experience index combines measures of health and economic impacts, which could have led to very different attitudes toward social distancing and closing businesses. Disaggregating these two dimensions does not substantially change our results (SI Appendix, Tables S38–S41).
In additional analyses, we also use county-level COVID-19 incidence rates and natural hazard risk and exposure as proxies for self-reported experience. For COVID-19 analyses, we used cumulative cases per county from the Johns Hopkins University COVID-19 Data Repository. We deviate from the preregistration by using cumulative COVID-19 case rates as opposed to new cases, since our personal negative experience index measures cumulative experience. We calculate cumulative cases up to each respondent’s survey response date, convert these into values per 100,000 population using county-level population estimates from the 2019 US Census Bureau, and take the natural log to account for the skewed distribution. For climate change analyses, we construct a county-level natural hazard risk index by summing the risk level (zero to three) for each of 11 hazards from the US National Center for Disaster Preparedness. This index captures hazard risk but not necessarily occurrence over the past 10 y. We thus identify a proxy for county-level natural hazard exposure using data from the Federal Emergency Management Agency (FEMA). We sum all natural hazard disaster declarations for the 10-y time frame in wave 2: 1 January 2010 to 15 June 2020. Disaster declarations provide a proxy for the impact of hazards on local populations without overemphasizing regions with more valuable properties or industries, as cost measures might. However, disaster declarations are a step removed from actual exposure and subject to partisan bias due to politicized processes of disaster relief distribution (47, 48). See SI Appendix for more information about the construction of the individual experience indices and county-level measures for both issues.
We also included embedded experiments in our survey. To examine respondents’ willingness to pay for different policies, we randomly assigned subjects to different costs associated with policies to help mitigate COVID-19 or compensate victims. To examine the role of partisan elites’ messaging, we randomly assigned the partisan identity of the proponent of policies related to emissions reductions or COVID-19 mask mandates.§ See SI Appendix, Fig. S1 for question wording.
Analyses.
All analyses, unless otherwise indicated, are based on weighted data to adjust for any deviations from our desired quotas. Deviations were generally small but differ across survey waves, with wave 3 showing the largest deviations. We compute survey weights using the American National Election Studies (ANES) method (49) as implemented by the anesrake package in the R programming language (50).¶
We report the results of multiple linear regressions. All regressions include the following sociodemographic control variables: gender; age; ethnicity; race; income; education; region; a categorical variable indicating whether the respondent lives in an urban, suburban, or rural area; and a dummy variable for repeat respondents. We deviate from the preregistration by adding the dummy variable for repeat respondents to the previously specified list of controls. We include this to account for unobserved differences between respondents who choose to participate in repeated online surveys and those who do not. We include partisan identification as either a primary independent variable (H1) or as a moderator (H2 to H4), depending on the analysis.
Cross-sectional (within wave) analyses are captured by the following specification using weighted least-squares regression:where Dit is the dependent variable of interest for respondent i in wave t, Iit is the primary explanatory variable of interest, Pit is a moderator where applicable, is a vector of controls, and Ωi is a dummy variable for respondents who were repeaters. Most of our models involve the interaction of negative experience with partisan identity (H2 to H4), where Democrats are the omitted category. For example, Table 1 reports the following specifications for t = 2:
[1]
[2]
Table 1.
Dependent variable: worry (standardized) | |
---|---|
Negative experience index | 0.114 |
(0.033) | |
Republican | – 0.740 |
(0.045) | |
Independent | – 0.426 |
(0.043) | |
Age | 0.001 |
(0.01) | |
Income | – 0.018 |
(0.010) | |
Education | 0.058 |
(0.010) | |
Latinx | 0.143 |
(0.037) | |
Asian | 0.204 |
(0.060) | |
Black | – 0.085 |
(0.043) | |
Pacific Islander | 0.220 |
(0.302) | |
Multiracial | – 0.233 |
(0.075) | |
Native American | – 0.106 |
(0.145) | |
Other race | – 0.159 |
(0.063) | |
Male | – 0.185 |
(0.028) | |
Self-describe | – 0.286 |
(0.370) | |
Suburban | 0.103 |
(0.037) | |
Urban | 0.172 |
(0.040) | |
Northeastern region | 0.130 |
(0.042) | |
Southern region | 0.083 |
(0.036) | |
Western region | – 0.056 |
(0.040) | |
Repeat respondent | – 0.078 |
(0.033) | |
Negative experience index * Republican | 0.281 |
(0.048) | |
Negative experience index * Independent | 0.068 |
(0.047) | |
Constant | 0.014 |
(0.068) | |
Observations | 5,194 |
R2 | 0.126 |
Adjusted R2 | 0.122 |
Akaike information criterion | 14,506.370 |
F statistic | 32.446 |
Dependent variable is a standardized z score. Data are from wave 2; weights are included; < 0.1; < 0.05; < 0.01.
For H2 to H4, we standardize dependent variables and include the same control variables in all regressions so coefficients (β) are comparable in terms of SDs of the dependent variable. Statistical significance is evaluated at the p < 0.05 level. We use the Benjamini and Hochberg (52) and Bonferroni methods to account for multiple comparisons within each outcome category when we estimate the variance on β3 for (SI Appendix, Tables S44–S47).
Robustness Analyses.
We include additional robustness analyses to evaluate endogeneity in our self-reported experience variables. These include 1) analyses using Eq. 1 with a dependent variable of self-reported experience and an interaction between county-level COVID-19 incidence and partisan identity, 2) the analyses described above using county-level measures of exposure in place of self-reported measures of experience, and 3) longitudinal panel data analyses of repeat respondents. We assess change over time within respondents by looking at the difference-in-differences of variables of interest in an analysis of our repeater group. These analyses are ordinary least-squares regressions with the same demographic controls, but the dependent and independent variables of interest are now the differences between t2 and t1, and we include control variables measured at t1 and the value of the dependent variable at t1 to control for baseline effects.
Results
Polarization on Worry, Action, and Policy Support.
In H1, we hypothesize that our respondents are polarized along partisan lines across three categories of dependent variables: 1) worry, 2) behavior (i.e., self-reported mask wearing and a host of other protective behaviors), and 2) policy preferences (i.e., willingness to pay to mitigate pandemic risks). Fig. 1 shows respondents’ mean responses on these variables as a function of political party for wave 2.
Fig. 1.

Fig. 1, Left shows that worry is lower for Republicans than Democrats, resulting in a substantial partisan gap that persists across all three waves and is consistent with other studies (SI Appendix, Fig. S3 and ref. 53).# Fig. 1, Middle shows that partisan differences are also present in a key behavioral response to the pandemic, with Democrats more likely to report wearing masks. Partisan gaps persist across waves (SI Appendix, Fig. S4) and the other self-reported protective behaviors we assess (SI Appendix, Figs. S4 and S5), consistent with research on social distancing compliance (39, 54, 55). Fig. 1, Right shows partisan differences in willingness to pay to support policies to reduce the likelihood of pandemics (see SI Appendix, Figs. S6, S7, and S9 for partisan gaps across the other COVID-19 policies we assess). Support is highest for policies incurring no or low costs to respondents, but Democrats are more willing to support the policy at all price levels than Republicans or Independents.
Polarization on these outcome variables is likely influenced by messaging from party leaders. In an embedded survey experiment, we find that respondents are more likely to support emissions reduction policies that are proposed by leaders of their own political parties as opposed to identical statements from other messengers (Fig. 2). This is especially true for Republicans.‖
Fig. 2.

In summary, we find evidence of partisan polarization on worry, behaviors, and policy preferences, and that the affiliation of political messengers can sway public support for contentious policies when issues are polarized.
Experience with COVID-19 and Partisan Gaps on Worry, Action, and Policy Support.
In keeping with H2, we find that hardship with COVID-19 is associated with increased worry for all groups, but especially for Republicans. At high levels of reported negative experience, there is no longer a partisan gap in mean levels of worry about COVID-19 in the spring and summer of 2020 (waves 1 and 2; Fig. 3).** As shown in Table 1 for wave 2, and SI Appendix, Table S11 for all waves, using the Likert scale outcome, we find a significant negative coefficient (P < 0.01) on the base term for Republicans (i.e., lower worry than Democrats under no negative experience) and a significant positive coefficient (P < 0.01) on the interaction term (i.e., a steeper slope of negative experience on worry for Republicans than for Democrats). The same relationships hold for perceived current and future harm to a respondent’s community (SI Appendix, Table S12).
Fig. 3.

We find similar results when assessing the role of experience on attitudes toward four personally protective actions (H3): vaccination intent, mask wearing, importance of self-isolating, and the retrospective view that one was too slow to self-isolate (SI Appendix, Table S13). We consistently find a significant negative coefficient on the base term for Republicans and a significant positive coefficient on the interaction term.†† These results suggest a strong association between experience and personally protective actions among Republicans. Republicans with high levels of adverse experience report behaviors that are more similar to those of Democrats than to those of Republicans with low levels of experience, resulting in a narrowing of the partisan gap.
We again find similar results when looking at support across distinct COVID-19 policy responses (H4): a retrospective evaluation about whether one’s state was too slow to mandate social distancing, support for caution in reopening the economy, and support for contributions by the United States to national and global efforts to fight the pandemic (SI Appendix, Table S14). Support for these policies is not related to personal experience for Democrats, but there is a positive and statistically significant coefficient on the interaction term (i.e., the slope on experience for Republicans vs. Democrats).‡‡
Adverse experience is associated with more support for policies aimed at mitigating the risks or impacts of the virus, even when the policies incur personal costs. Fig. 4 shows that negative experience is strongly associated with the willingness to pay for a policy to mitigate COVID-19 for both Democrats and Republicans.§§
Fig. 4.

Adverse experience and trust in institutions.
In exploratory analyses, we examine the relationship among personal hardship, political affiliation, and broad measures of trust in key crisis institutions and actors—specifically, the CDC, news media, and scientists. We find that Republicans who report no adverse experience tend to also report significantly lower trust in these actors than Democrats. The partisan gap is especially pronounced for trust in news media (SI Appendix, Table S15). Reported personal experience with COVID-19 is not correlated with trust among Democrats, but the interaction term is positive and significant for Republicans, indicating that those reporting greater adverse experience also report significantly greater levels of trust in key crisis institutions and actors.
Additional analyses and robustness checks.
We conduct additional analyses and robustness checks to further evaluate the relationship between experience and partisan gaps on worry, behaviors, policy preferences, and trust, and to look for evidence of partisan-motivated reasoning. So far, we have shown strong correlational evidence that self-reported experience predicts our outcomes of interest. Here, we use county-level incidence rates and longitudinal data, where possible, to start to identify any causal role of experience on our outcome variables.
Democrats report greater negative experience with COVID-19 than Republicans (SI Appendix, Fig. S12), but this difference was only significant in wave 3. Furthermore, once we include demographic and geographic controls that capture variation in disease exposure that is not due to party affiliation, we no longer find significant party differences in reported experience (SI Appendix, Table S10). Reported COVID-19 experience is highly correlated with county-level incidence rates, and this relationship does not vary by party across any of our waves (SI Appendix, Table S10), suggesting that objective incidence rates are related to self-reported experience. Nevertheless, we acknowledge that Republicans in high-incidence counties may differ systematically from Republicans in low incidence counties (see Discussion).
Next, we reanalyze the data 1) using the log of county cumulative COVID-19 cases per 100,000 in place of self-reported experience and 2) examining changes within subjects over time. We use county-level incidence in place of negative experience to explore the overall relationship between contextual conditions and individual responses, and to assess whether this “objective” proxy for COVID-19 experience differs systematically from our self-reported experience measure.
Overall, we do not see a consistent relationship among county incidence rates, partisanship, and our three classes of outcome variables. County incidence rates do not have a statistically significant effect on worry (SI Appendix, Table S16), most likely because the effect of county incidence on individual worry is subject to many demographic and social factors.¶¶ Looking at personally protective actions (SI Appendix, Table S17), we find that Republicans in high-incidence counties are more likely to report wearing a mask in public than Republicans in low-incidence counties (negative base term relative to Democrats, positive interaction term with experience). However, the interaction of county incidence and Republican identity is not statistically significant for our other actions. Turning to policy support (SI Appendix, Table S18), we find that Republicans in high-incidence counties show stronger support for global efforts to mitigate the pandemic (negative base term, positive interaction term). The interaction of county incidence rates and Republican identity is not statistically significant for the other policies. Finally, we find that being in a high-incidence county is correlated with greater trust in news media among Republicans (negative base term, positive interaction term; SI Appendix, Table S19). The interaction terms on other trust variables are not statistically significant.
Leveraging our longitudinal panel data, we analyze within-subject changes in experience over time on the outcome measures that were asked about in multiple waves. This analysis begins to address the potential endogeneity between attitudes and experience by focusing on the effect of change in experience on our outcome variables. Unfortunately, our experience index is limited in its ability to capture variation over time within respondents. Respondents did not provide a number when asked whether they had close friends and family who contracted, were hospitalized for, or died from COVID-19, so we are unable to capture changes in the intensity of experience over time. Furthermore, most job losses or pay cuts occurred early in the pandemic, making it unlikely for a respondent to report a change on those dimensions in subsequent waves. The vast majority of our respondents do not approach a ceiling effect on the index, but it likely underdetects changes in a respondent’s experience over time.
By and large, we find evidence that a within-subject change in adverse experience with the virus increases worry, behaviors, and policy support, but we no longer find that experience had a stronger effect for Republicans than for Democrats. Respondents with increased negative experience between waves exhibit increased worry (SI Appendix, Tables S31 and S32, column 1), but the interaction between change in negative experience and partisanship is no longer statistically significant. (See preregistered hypothesis W2.2.1.3: Change in negative experience of COVID-19 between April and June will correlate positively with change in worry about COVID-19, especially among Republicans.) Similarly, we generally do not find a significant interaction between experience and partisanship for our behavior (SI Appendix, Tables S33 and S34), policy (SI Appendix, Table S35), or trust outcome measures (SI Appendix, Tables S36 and S37). Behavior outcomes that are comparable across waves are “important for individuals to self-isolate” (waves 1 through 3), “wear mask in public” (waves 2 and 3), and “support for vaccine” (waves 2 and 3). Only one policy outcome was asked in the same way across consecutive waves: the retrospective evaluation that one’s state was slow to social distance, asked in waves 2 and 3. Together, these results suggest that adverse experience may increase worry, but we do not find a strong or reliable causal relationship between experience and behavior or policy support.
Experience with Natural Hazards and Partisan Gaps on Worry, Action, and Policy Support.
With the caveat that the attribution of extreme weather events to climate change involves greater uncertainty for both experts and the general public (6), we briefly report the results of parallel analyses for climate change.
We find a substantial partisan gap on worry about climate change that stays constant across all three waves (SI Appendix, Fig. S15). In contrast to the COVID-19 results, we do not see a general decline in worry about climate change across waves. We also find that the county-level FEMA-based exposure measure is a positive and significant predictor of our negative experience index (SI Appendix, Table S53); this relationship is not statistically significant for county-level hazard risk (SI Appendix, Table S21).## In keeping with our COVID-19 findings, partisan affiliation does not moderate the relationship between county-level exposure and self-reported experience with natural hazards. Like COVID-19, we expect that the effect of county-level natural hazard risk or exposure on responses also depends on the vulnerability of the respondent’s social networks, media discussion of hazards and variation in disaster response and preparedness policies that are only partially captured by our demographic controls.
Adverse experience with natural hazards is associated with a narrowing of the partisan gap on climate change worry across all three waves (SI Appendix, Fig. S16). The large substantive difference may be due to the low baseline worry about climate change among Republicans with no experience, which is lower than baseline worry about COVID-19. We also find significant positive relationships between reported experience and perceptions of current and future harm from climate change among Republicans (SI Appendix, Table S22).
When looking at comparable measures of individual behaviors, policy support, and trust in key actors, we find that the role of adverse experience parallels our COVID-19 findings in several ways. Among Republicans, adverse personal experience is positively and significantly associated with an increase in the perceived importance of taking individual action to mitigate climate change. We find significant positive effects of experience on support for national and international efforts to mitigate climate change (SI Appendix, Table S23) and trust in key actors (Environmental Protection Agency [EPA], news media, and scientists; SI Appendix, Table S24). Greater reported adverse experience among Republicans is associated with a narrowing of the partisan gap on these measures.
Discussion
Our results suggest that two key interpretive channels play a role in opinion about COVID-19: a partisan channel and an experience channel. We find a significant partisan divide on worry, protective behaviors, and support for policies intended to mitigate the risks and impacts of COVID-19. This is consistent with emerging literature finding partisan divides in outcomes like social distancing and responsiveness to government policies (37–40, 53–55) and broader impacts of COVID-19 on political outcomes such as voter turnout in different contexts (56, 57). In an embedded survey experiment, we find evidence of “party over policy” effects. Respondents, especially Republicans, are more likely to support the same COVID-19 policy when it is proposed by leaders of their own political party. These findings show that copartisan messaging effects (35, 36) extend to a novel health threat and are an important avenue for garnering policy support on polarized issues. These findings suggest widespread partisan-motivated interpretations of a novel and immediate, yet quickly politicized, threat. However, they may also be masking underlying differences in experience with the virus at the time of the survey.
In this study, we evaluate how personal experience is associated with varying public attitudes toward the novel and rapidly polarized threat of COVID-19. We evaluate the interaction between the two channels and find that negative COVID-19 experience is associated with a narrowed partisan gap on a number of responses. Republicans with no negative experience tend to have lower concern about the virus and lower support for personal protective actions and policies to mitigate COVID-19 than Democrats. Personal hardship or experience with a threat is correlated with greater Republican endorsement of actions and policies aimed at mitigating the risks of present and future pandemics, much more so than for Democrats. Additionally, and perhaps most important for issues beyond COVID-19, we find that reported experience with COVID-19 is correlated with trust in several institutional actors important for keeping citizens informed during crises (e.g., CDC, EPA, news, scientists), especially among Republicans. Democrats tend to have a higher baseline on all measures, although they are not near ceiling on our scales. While an optimistic view might take our findings to suggest that direct experience may bridge partisan gaps by increasing concern and responsiveness to a polarized threat, our results are correlational, and analyses of county-level incidence rates and panel data tell a more somber story.
Exposure to COVID-19 is not exogenous, and the significant relationships we identify rely primarily on self-reported experience. Respondents who report negative experience may be more concerned about the virus for reasons other than experience (e.g., they may be essential workers), and these drivers of concern, behavior, and policy support may interact with how experience is reported and with political attitudes in complicated ways. Reassuringly, we find that party affiliation does not predict reported experience, nor does it moderate the relationship between local incidence rates and reported experience. This finding suggests that informative signals about a threat are not always filtered through a partisan lens and that seeing is, at least sometimes, believing. This finding stands in contrast to the literature on partisan-motivated interpretation of extreme weather events (23, 31), but is consistent with recent literature showing a significant albeit small role for personal experience of temperature changes and extreme weather in eliciting concern about climate change (58–60).
However, when we run our analyses with county-level incidence rates instead of reported experience, we find that the coefficients are generally in the predicted direction but no longer statistically significant in most of our analyses. Local incidence rates depend, to some extent, on county-level factors that are correlated with partisanship, such as population density, income levels, types of employment, age, and COVID-related policy decisions. These factors likely also affect individual behaviors and policy opinions both independently of and through personal experience. County-level incidence rates also depend on COVID-19 testing and data reporting procedures, which varied across states and counties in ways that may have been systematically correlated with the political party in power (61). Future research could further investigate the effect of this variation on attitudes and behaviors.
Furthermore, the panel structure of our data allowed us to analyze the effect of within-subject changes in experience on our outcome variables. While we find significant effects of experience on worry, the effects on behavior and policy support are mixed. We no longer find a robust interaction between experience and party affiliation in accounting for variation in concern. This result could reflect that Republicans living in areas with higher COVID-19 incidence rates (and therefore greater adverse experience, on average) may be systematically different from those in other areas. These differences may lead them to recall or report their negative experience differently than Republicans in low-incidence counties for myriad reasons that we cannot disentangle with our data.
Taken together, our results suggest a complicated relationship between experience and partisan channels in shaping the interpretation of and response to a novel, immediate, and consequential public health threat. Recently, there has been a call for more longitudinal data in order to disentangle these two influences on behaviors and policy support in the context of climate change (5). Our longitudinal data show that a change in adverse experience does indeed predict greater worry, but it does not reliably predict an increase in mitigative behaviors or policy support. These findings suggest that individual concern may be malleable in the face of direct experience (and new information) about the severity and reality of the disease, but that this does not necessarily translate into action or policy support, and does not depend on party affiliation.
Direct experience with a serious threat may be insufficient to elicit bipartisan action or policy support for polarized issues because the experience is insufficient to overcome priors or because experience itself may be shaped by an individual’s ideological views, as has been shown for climate change (5). Furthermore, partisanship may systematically affect how experience and preferences are remembered or (mis)reported on surveys (45). We cannot fully control for these factors given our mostly cross-sectional evidence. Still, our findings suggest that, while partisan-motivated reasoning may be a useful sense-making heuristic when other forms of evidence are lacking, experience could be a powerful teacher (62). Recent research shows that partisan differences in beliefs and actions in response to threats are not categorical but rather a matter of degree (27). Future work should focus on understanding how heterogeneity within and across partisan groups shapes reports of and responses to experience with different collective threats. Additional work could explore how perceptions of experience vary by party depending on the political party in power.
Our findings with respect to climate change are consistent with our results for COVID-19. However, in contrast to COVID-19, it is much more difficult to attribute specific extreme weather events (or their impacts) to climate change, not to mention the impacts of one’s own actions or even policy measures on climate change. Additionally, individuals are less able to directly control their own level of risk exposure in the case of climate change, especially with low-cost and readily available actions such as mask wearing and social distancing. Greater uncertainty around climate change may thus create an even larger role for partisan influences in shaping perceptions of climate change (6, 62). More research is needed to understand how the general public attributes different natural hazards to climate change, whether this, in turn, makes a difference for behaviors and policy support, and how this information should be effectively communicated to a polarized public (63).
Our results reaffirm previous work demonstrating partisan polarization on COVID-19 and climate change. They show that partisan messaging can increase polarization and suggest that personal experience can, under some conditions, narrow it. Prior views, partisan messaging, and personal experience interact in complex ways to shape public attitudes toward two contemporary crises.
Materials and Methods
Anonymized data files and replication code have been deposited in Open Science Framework (OSF) (https://osf.io/6x2yt/) (64). This study received institution review board approval from Princeton University (12754) and Texas A&M University (2020-1078). Both universities granted the project exempt status.
Data, Materials, and Software Availability
Anonymized data (CSV files) and replication code (in R) have been deposited in OSF (https://osf.io/6x2yt/).
Acknowledgments
This research was supported by NSF Grant SES-2030800 “RAPID: Public Responses to Personal and Societal Risk: Attitudes and Behavior on COVID-19 and Global Change.” We appreciate the support of the Andlinger Center for Energy and the Environment and the Princeton Institute of International and Regional Studies at Princeton University. We thank the participants at the International Studies Association Annual Convention 2021, American Political Science Association Annual Meeting 2021, and Annual Conference of Environmental Politics and Governance 2021 for thoughtful comments.
Supporting Information
Appendix 01 (PDF)
- Download
- 2.49 MB
References
1
E. U. Weber, “Giving the future a chance: Behavioral economic responses to the dual challenges of covid-19 and the climate crisis” in The Behavioral Economics Guide, A. Samson, Ed. (Behavioral Science Solutions Ltd, 2020), pp. 2–15.
2
R. L. Gregory, Eye and Brain: The Psychology of Seeing (McGraw-Hill, 1973).
3
T. A. Myers, E. W. Maibach, C. Roser-Renouf, K. Akerlof, A. A. Leiserowitz, The relationship between personal experience and belief in the reality of global warming. Nat. Clim. Chang. 3, 343–347 (2013).
4
E. U. Weber, Seeing is believing: Understanding & aiding human responses to global climate change. Daedalus 149, 139–150 (2020).
5
P. D. Howe, J. R. Marlon, M. Mildenberger, B. S. Shield, How will climate change shape climate opinion? Environ. Res. Lett. 14, 113001 (2019).
6
P. D. Howe, Extreme weather experience and climate change opinion. Curr. Opin. Behav. Sci. 42, 127–131 (2021).
7
R. Rescorla, A. Wagner, “A theory of pavlovian conditioning: Variations in the effectiveness of reinforcement and nonreinforcement” in Classical Conditioning II: Current Research and Theory, A. Black, W. Prokasy, Eds. (Appleton Century Crofts, New York, 1972), pp. 64–99.
8
H. E. den Ouden, P. Kok, F. P. de Lange, How prediction errors shape perception, attention, and motivation. Front. Psychol. 3, 548 (2012).
9
E. U. Weber, P. G. Lindemann, “From intuition to analysis: Making decisions with our head, our heart, or by the book” in Intuition in Judgment and Decision Making, H. Plessner, C. Betsch, T. Betsch, Eds. (Lawrence Erlbaum, Mahwah, NJ, 2007), pp. 191–208.
10
S. Constantino, M. Schlüter, E. Weber, N. Wijermans, Cognition and behavior in context: A framework and theories to explain natural resource use decisions in social-ecological systems. Sustain. Sci. 16, 1651–1671 (2021).
11
S. A. Sloman, The empirical case for two systems of reasoning. Psychol. Bull. 119, 3–22 (1996).
12
G. F. Loewenstein, E. U. Weber, C. K. Hsee, N. Welch, Risk as feelings. Psychol. Bull. 127, 267–286 (2001).
13
P. Slovic, B. Fischhoff, S. Lichtenstein, “Facts and fears: Understanding perceived risk” in Societal Risk Assessment, R. C. Schwing, W. A. Albers, Eds. (Springer, 1980), pp. 181–216.
14
E. U. Weber, Seeing is believing. Nat. Clim. Chang. 3, 312–313 (2013).
15
E. U. Weber, What shapes perceptions of climate change? Wiley Interdiscip. Rev. Clim. Change 1, 332–342 (2010).
16
L. Zaval, E. A. Keenan, E. J. Johnson, E. U. Weber, How warm days increase belief in global warming. Nat. Clim. Chang. 4, 143–147 (2014).
17
M. R. Sisco, V. Bosetti, E. U. Weber, When do extreme weather events generate attention to climate change? Clim. Change 143, 227–241 (2017).
18
C. Demski, S. Capstick, N. Pidgeon, R. G. Sposato, A. Spence, Experience of extreme weather affects climate change mitigation and adaptation responses. Clim. Change 140, 149–164 (2017).
19
S. M. Smith, E. Vela, Environmental context-dependent memory: A review and meta-analysis. Psychon. Bull. Rev. 8, 203–220 (2001).
20
Z. Kunda, The case for motivated reasoning. Psychol. Bull. 108, 480–498 (1990).
21
D. M. Kahan et al., “The tragedy of the risk-perception commons: Culture conflict, rationality conflict, and climate change” (Legal Studies Research Paper No. 2011-26, Temple University, (2011).
22
E. Dawson, T. Gilovich, D. T. Regan, Motivated reasoning and performance on the Wason Selection Task. Pers. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 28, 1379–1387 (2002).
23
J. N. Druckman, M. C. McGrath, The evidence for motivated reasoning in climate change preference formation. Nat. Clim. Chang. 9, 111–119 (2019).
24
H. Boudet, L. Giordono, C. Zanocco, H. Satein, H. Whitley, Event attribution and partisanship shape local discussion of climate change after extreme weather. Nat. Clim. Chang. 10, 69–76 (2020).
25
C. A. Ogunbode, C. Demski, S. B. Capstick, R. G. Sposato, Attribution matters: Revisiting the link between extreme weather experience and climate change mitigation responses. Glob. Environ. Change 54, 31–39 (2019).
26
T. Bayes, LII. An essay towards solving a problem in the doctrine of chances. By the late Rev. Mr. Bayes, FRS communicated by Mr. Price, in a letter to John Canton, AMFRS. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. 53, 370–418 (1763).
27
R. Dryden, M. G. Morgan, S. Broomell, Lay detection of unusual patterns in the frequency of hurricanes. Weather Clim. Soc. 12, 597–609 (2020).
28
T. Bolsen, J. N. Druckman, F. L. Cook, The influence of partisan motivated reasoning on public opinion. Polit. Behav. 36, 235–262 (2014).
29
T. Bolsen, R. Palm, Motivated Reasoning and Political Decision Making in Oxford Research Encyclopedia of Politics (Oxford University Press, 2019).
30
W. Bullock, K. Imai, J. N. Shapiro, Statistical analysis of endorsement experiments: Measuring support for militant groups in Pakistan. Polit. Anal. 19, 363–384 (2011).
31
R. Bayes, J. N. Druckman, Motivated reasoning and climate change. Curr. Opin. Behav. Sci. 42, 27–35 (2021).
32
A. Leiserowitz, Climate change risk perception and policy preferences: The role of affect, imagery, and values. Clim. Change 77, 45–72 (2006).
33
E. U. Weber, S. Sonka, “Production and pricing decision in cash-crop farming: Effects of decision traits and climate change expectations” in Farmers’ Decision Making – A Descriptive Approach, Proceedings of 38th European Association of Agricultural Economics (EAAE) Seminar, B. H. Jacobsen, D. E. Pedersen, J. Christensen, S. Rasmussen, Eds. (Institute of Agricultural Economics and The Royal Veterinary and Agricultural University Denmark, 1994), pp. 203–218.
34
G. L. Cohen, Party over policy: The dominating impact of group influence on political beliefs. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 85, 808–822 (2003).
35
L. C. Stokes, C. Warshaw, Renewable energy policy design and framing influence public support in the United States. Nat. Energy 2, 17107 (2017).
36
T. Bolsen, R. Palm, J. T. Kingsland, The impact of message source on the effectiveness of communications about climate change. Sci. Commun. 41, 464–487 (2019).
37
J. Clinton, J. Cohen, J. Lapinski, M. Trussler, Partisan pandemic: How partisanship and public health concerns affect individuals’ social mobility during COVID-19. Sci. Adv. 7, eabd7204 (2021).
38
H. Allcott et al., Polarization and public health: Partisan differences in social distancing during the coronavirus pandemic. J. Public Econ. 191, 104254 (2020).
39
M. Painter, T. Qiu, Political beliefs affect compliance with covid-19 social distancing orders. SSRN [Preprint] (2020). https://ssrn.com/abstract=3569098. Accessed 1 June 2021.
40
K. Lipsitz, G. Pop-Eleches, The partisan divide in social distancing. SSRN [Preprint] (2020). https://ssrn.com/abstract=3595695. Accessed 1 June 2021.
41
P. J. Egan, M. Mullin, Climate change: US public opinion. Annu. Rev. Polit. Sci. 20, 209–227 (2017).
42
P. J. Egan, M. Mullin, Turning personal experience into political attitudes: The effect of local weather on Americans’ perceptions about global warming. J. Polit. 74, 796–809 (2012).
43
A. Leiserowitz et al., Climate Change in the American Mind: April 2020 (Yale Program on Climate Change Communication, New Haven, CT, 2020).
44
D. Ruddell, S. L. Harlan, S. Grossman-Clarke, G. Chowell, Scales of perception: Public awareness of regional and neighborhood climates. Clim. Change 111, 581–607 (2012).
45
J. G. Bullock, G. Lenz, Partisan bias in surveys. Annu. Rev. Polit. Sci. 22, 325–342 (2019).
46
R. D. Putnam, The Upswing: How America Came Together a Century Ago and How We Can Do It Again (Simon and Schuster, 2020).
47
A. Cooperman, (un)natural disasters: Electoral cycles in disaster relief. Comp. Polit. Stud. 55, 1158–1197 (2022).
48
A. J. Oliver, A. Reeves, “The politics of disaster relief” in Emerging Trends in the Social and Behavioral Sciences: An Interdisciplinary, Searchable, and Linkable Resource, S. M. Kosslyn, Eds. (Wiley, 2015), pp. 1–8.
49
M. DeBell, J. A. Krosnick, “Computing weights for American national election study survey data” (ANES Tech. Rep. Series nes012427, American National Election Studies, 2009).
50
J. Pasek, M. J. Pasek, Package ‘Anesrake (The Comprehensive R Archive Network, 2018).
51
J. L. Herman, A. R. Flores, K. K. O’Neill, How Many Adults and Youth Identify as Transgender in the United States? (The Williams Institute, 2022).
52
Y. Benjamini, Y. Hochberg, Controlling the false discovery rate: A practical and powerful approach to multiple testing. J. R. Stat. Soc. Ser. B Methodol. 57, 289–300 (1995).
53
C. Makridis, J. T. Rothwell, The real cost of political polarization: Evidence from the covid-19 pandemic. SSRN [Preprint] (2020). https://ssrn.com/abstract=3638373. Accessed 1 June 2021.
54
D. A. Goldstein, J. Wiedemann, Who do you trust? The consequences of partisanship and trust for public responsiveness to COVID-19 orders. Perspect. Polit. 20, 412–438 (2022).
55
A. Gollwitzer et al., Partisan differences in physical distancing are linked to health outcomes during the COVID-19 pandemic. Nat. Hum. Behav. 4, 1186–1197 (2020).
56
S. M. Constantino, A. D. Cooperman, T. M. Q. Moreira, Voting in a global pandemic: Assessing dueling influences of Covid-19 on turnout. Soc. Sci. Q. 102, 2210–2235 (2021).
57
A. Santana, S. Aguilar, How costly is voting? Explaining individual differences in the costs of voting. J. Elections Public Opin. Parties 31, 119–139 (2021).
58
P. Bergquist, C. Warshaw, Does global warming increase public concern about climate change? J. Polit. 81, 686–691 (2019).
59
M. Bergquist, A. Nilsson, P. W. Schultz, Experiencing a severe weather event increases concern about climate change. Front. Psychol. 10, 220 (2019).
60
D. M. Konisky, L. Hughes, C. H. Kaylor, Extreme weather events and climate change concern. Clim. Change 134, 533–547 (2016).
61
B. Neelon, F. Mutiso, N. T. Mueller, J. L. Pearce, S. E. Benjamin-Neelon, Associations between governor political affiliation and covid-19 cases, deaths, and testing in the US. Am. J. Prev. Med. 61, 115–119 (2021).
62
S. M. Constantino, E. U. Weber, Decision-making under the deep uncertainty of climate change: The psychological and political agency of narratives. Curr. Opin. Psychol. 42, 151–159 (2021).
63
B. Fischhoff, Making decisions in a COVID-19 world. JAMA 324, 139–140 (2020).
64
S. M. Constantino, A. D. Cooperman, R. O. Keohane, E. U. Weber, Replication Materials for PNAS Concern and Action on COVID-19 and Climate. Open Science Framework. https://osf.io/6x2yt/. Deposited 23 August 2022.
Information & Authors
Information
Published in
Classifications
Copyright
Copyright © 2022 the Author(s). Published by PNAS. This article is distributed under Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial-NoDerivatives License 4.0 (CC BY-NC-ND).
Data, Materials, and Software Availability
Anonymized data (CSV files) and replication code (in R) have been deposited in OSF (https://osf.io/6x2yt/).
Submission history
Received: November 12, 2021
Accepted: September 19, 2022
Published online: November 14, 2022
Published in issue: November 15, 2022
Keywords
Acknowledgments
This research was supported by NSF Grant SES-2030800 “RAPID: Public Responses to Personal and Societal Risk: Attitudes and Behavior on COVID-19 and Global Change.” We appreciate the support of the Andlinger Center for Energy and the Environment and the Princeton Institute of International and Regional Studies at Princeton University. We thank the participants at the International Studies Association Annual Convention 2021, American Political Science Association Annual Meeting 2021, and Annual Conference of Environmental Politics and Governance 2021 for thoughtful comments.
Notes
Reviewers: M.M., Duke University; and P.S., Decision Research.
*Hypotheses and methods are outlined in the preregistration available at https://osf.io/4ajxd/. The preregistration provides the basis for multiple papers. Here, we report the findings relevant to this study, as noted in the preregistration dated 9 July 2020, which corresponds to wave 2, the data used for the key analyses in this paper.
†
Attrition could affect our within-subjects and cross-sectional analyses. Repeaters in the sample are more likely to be Democrats, older, female, and Asian, and to reside in suburban areas and in the Northeast. We find that higher baseline negative experience with COVID-19 is associated with higher likelihood of attrition, but this does not vary systematically by party (SI Appendix, Table S42). Higher endline county-level COVID-19 cumulative case rates are not associated with higher likelihood of attrition (SI Appendix, Table S43). These descriptive analyses suggest that attrition may limit the generalizability of our within-subjects analyses. Repeat respondents may also respond differently if they remember the survey and their previous answers from a few months prior. In cross-sectional analyses, we thus weight the data to account for deviations from our target quotas, and include standard demographic covariates and a dummy variable for whether a respondent is naive to the survey or not, to account for latent traits that correlate with taking the survey more than once. We weight the data based on the same sources used for the initial quotas.
‡
We also asked respondents about the degree to which both COVID-19 and climate change elicited calm concern vs. gut-level dread: “Is the risk of coronavirus [global warming/climate change] a risk that people have learned to live with and can think about reasonably calmly, or is it one that people have great dread for on the level of a gut reaction?” Answers to the questions about worry were significantly predicted by reported dread, suggesting that our worry measure is, in part, capturing a visceral or affective response.
§
For the survey experiment about emissions reductions, respondents were randomly assigned to one of six treatment conditions: 1) COVID-19 focus from Democratic leaders, 2) COVID-19 focus from Republican leaders, 3) COVID-19 focus from political leaders, 4) climate change focus from Democratic leaders, 5) climate change focus from Republican leaders, or 6) climate change focus from political leaders. Therefore, half of the subjects responded to a question about COVID-19-related emissions reductions. For the survey experiment about mask mandates, respondents were randomly assigned to one of three treatment conditions: Democratic governors, Republican governors, or governors.
¶
The ANES weighting algorithm (also known as raking and proportional fitting) uses an iterative procedure to select multiplicative weights that reconcile survey marginals with the target marginals for a selection of variables. In our case, these were based on the American Community Survey (ACS) and ANES quotas described above. In each iteration, a vector of weights is adjusted such that the sum of the weights for a particular variable accounts for the same proportion of the overall sample as in the target values until the algorithm converges. The ACS does not include data for gender nonconforming respondents, but we allowed respondents to select male, female, and prefer to self-describe. We use an estimate that 0.125% of adults are gender nonconforming, based on recent statistics that 0.5% of adults are transgender, and 25% of transgender adults are gender nonconforming (51). We subtract this share evenly from the ACS marginals for male and female.
#
See preregistered hypothesis W2.2.1: The US public is polarized on both issues (Democrats and Republicans report statistically different worry) but is less polarized in its response to COVID-19 than to climate change. We deviate by showing raw mean levels rather than the contrast between COVID-19 and climate change polarization.
‖
For similar analyses of a mask mandate policy, see SI Appendix, Table S5. We find that Republicans are significantly more likely to approve of the policy when the messenger is also a Republican (column 1). However, the analysis of the data based on a copartisan dummy is not significant (column 2).
**See preregistered hypothesis W2.2.2: Existing polarization [of worry] on both issues will be lower among those with personal exposure to COVID-19/climate change. We deviate slightly from the preregistration by analyzing the issues of COVID-19 and climate change separately, since subsequent discussions led us to understand that there are differences in the underlying mechanisms; see Discussion.
††
Looking at the base term on negative experience, which is the slope for Democrats, we see that there is no association between negative experience on mask wearing, vaccine support, or importance of self-isolating among Democrats. Democrats do show a positive significant relationship between negative experience and the perception that they were slow to self-isolate, which could reflect Bayesian updating about one’s own behavior and its consequences.
‡‡
After adjusting for multiple comparisons within each outcome category, we find that P values on the interaction term () for outcomes of worry/harm, personally protective actions, and policy support remain statistically significant at P < 0.05, except the outcome of “state slow to social distance” (Benjamini–Hochberg P = 0.02; Bonferroni P = 0.07). See SI Appendix, Tables S44–S47.
§§
See preregistered hypothesis W2.5.1: Negative personal experience with COVID-19 is correlated with increased willingness to pay to compensate those hardest hit by COVID-19, but less so for Republicans. We deviate from the preregistration by subsetting the analysis by partisan affiliation, since a triple interaction between randomized amount (0, 1, 10, 50, 100), negative experience index, and partisan affiliation would be challenging to interpret.
¶¶
See preregistered hypothesis W2.2.3.1: Increased incidence of COVID-19, since wave 1, in a state or community will reduce partisan polarization [about worry] on COVID-19. The demographic and social factors include personal experience, but also information from respondents’ social networks, media consumption, variation in local policies and mandates, etc. The potential endogeneity and belief-motivated perception of personal hardship might lead researchers to use an instrumental variables or two-stage least-squares approach. In this analysis, we would first predict personal experience based on county incidence, then use predicted experience as the explanatory variable for worry. However, the many channels by which county incidence could affect worry, independent of personal hardship, would violate the exclusion restriction.
##
Since earthquakes are included in these indices but are not scientifically attributable to climate change, we reran the same analyses excluding respondents who experienced earthquakes and removing earthquakes from the county-level hazard variables. These respondents were primarily concentrated in California, thus altering the underlying characteristics of the sample. We calculated new weights given these changes. In this subsample, neither the FEMA-based exposure measure nor the hazard risk measure significantly predicts self-reported negative personal experience (SI Appendix, Tables S56 and S57).
Authors
Competing Interests
The authors declare no competing interest.
Metrics & Citations
Metrics
Altmetrics
Citations
Cite this article
Personal hardship narrows the partisan gap in COVID-19 and climate change responses, Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A.
119 (46) e2120653119,
https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.2120653119
(2022).
Copied!
Copying failed.
Export the article citation data by selecting a format from the list below and clicking Export.
Cited by
Loading...
View Options
View options
PDF format
Download this article as a PDF file
DOWNLOAD PDFLogin options
Check if you have access through your login credentials or your institution to get full access on this article.
Personal login Institutional LoginRecommend to a librarian
Recommend PNAS to a LibrarianPurchase options
Purchase this article to access the full text.