Maintaining cooperation through vertical communication of trust when removing sanctions

Edited by Karen Cook, Stanford University, Stanford, CA; received August 28, 2024; accepted January 29, 2025
March 17, 2025
122 (12) e2415010122

Significance

An effective way to ensure cooperation is to sanction noncompliance. Yet, once sanctions are removed, cooperation tends to drop. We demonstrate that vertical communication by authorities can help maintain cooperation when abolishing punishment institutions. Specifically, we show that when authorities communicate their trust in the population to cooperate in the absence of punishment institutions, this acts as a self-fulfilling prophecy and leads people to maintain cooperation even in the absence of punishment. Our findings complement previous literature on (horizontal) norms, beliefs, and conditional cooperation. They offer valuable insights that can help authorities maintain voluntary cooperation, even after enforcement mechanisms are terminated (for example, after a pandemic) or extremely difficult to monitor (for example, energy conservation regulations).

Abstract

An effective way to foster cooperation is to monitor behavior and sanction freeriding. Yet, previous studies have shown that cooperation quickly declines when sanctioning mechanisms are removed. We test whether explicitly expressing trust in players’ capability to maintain cooperation after the removal of sanctions, i.e., vertical communication of trust, has the potential to alleviate this drop in compliance. Four incentivized public-goods experiments (N = 2,823) find that the vertical communication of trust maintains cooperation upon the removal of centralized (Study 1), third-party (Study 2a, 2b), and peer punishment (Study 3), and this effect extends beyond single interactions (Study 4). In all studies, vertical trust communication increases mutual trust among players, providing support to the idea that vertically communicating trust can be a self-fulfilling prophecy. Extrapolating our findings to natural environments, they suggest that authorities should carefully consider how they communicate the lifting of rules and sanctions.

Get full access to this article

Purchase, subscribe or recommend this article to your librarian.

Data, Materials, and Software Availability

Data, code, and materials are deposited through the OSF at https://doi.org/10.17605/osf.io/duvrc (48). Studies were preregistered at https://aspredicted.org/ and are accessible via OSF (Studies 1, 2a, 2b, 3, and 4) (48).

Acknowledgments

Funded by the Deutsche Forschungsgemeinschaft (DFG, German Research Foundation) under Germany’s Excellence Strategy—EXC 2126/1-390838866 and the research grant PO 1850/3-1 awarded to A.-C.P. Declaration of AI and AI-assisted technologies in the writing process: During the preparation of this work, we used you.com in order to improve readability and language. After using this service, we reviewed and edited the content as needed and take full responsibility for the content of the publication.

Author contributions

A.-C.P., P.U., S.K., and J.L. designed research; A.-C.P. performed research; A.-C.P. analyzed data; and A.-C.P., P.U., S.K., and J.L. wrote the paper.

Competing interests

The authors declare no competing interest.

Supporting Information

Appendix 01 (PDF)

References

1
E. Fehr, I. Schurtenberger, Normative foundations of human cooperation. Nat. Hum. Behav. 2, 458–468 (2018).
2
J. C. Yong, B. K. C. Choy, Noncompliance with safety guidelines as a free-riding strategy: An evolutionary game-theoretic approach to cooperation during the COVID-19 pandemic. Front. Psychol. 12, 646892 (2021).
3
E. Fehr, S. Gächter, Cooperation and punishment in public goods experiments. Am. Econ. Rev. 90, 980–994 (2000).
4
E. Fehr, S. Gächter, Altruistic punishment in humans. Nature 450, 137–140 (2002).
5
Ö. Gürerk, B. Irlenbusch, B. Rockenbach, The competitive advantage of sanctioning institutions. Science 312, 108–111 (2006).
6
B. Herrmann, C. Thöni, S. Gächter, Antisocial punishment across societies. Science 319, 1362–1367 (2008).
7
L. Klingert, Denmark is first in EU to lift all coronavirus restrictions. Politico (2021). https://www.politico.eu/article/denmark-first-eu-lift-coronavirus-restrictions/. Accessed 30 November 2024.
8
M. B. Petersen, COVID lesson: Trust the public with hard truths. Nature 598, 237–237 (2021).
9
R. Spears, H. J. Smith, Experiments as politics. Polit. Psychol. 22, 309–330 (2001).
10
S. Milgram, Obedience to Authority: An Experimental View (Harper Row, 1974).
11
O. Bochet, T. Page, L. Putterman, Communication and punishment in voluntary contribution experiments. J. Econ. Behav. Organ. 60, 11–26 (2006).
12
J. C. Cardenas, J. Stranlund, C. Willis, Local environmental control and institutional crowding-out. World Dev. 28, 1719–1733 (2000).
13
M. Bacharach, G. Guerra, D. J. Zizzo, The self-fulfilling property of trust: An experimental study. Theory Decis. 63, 349–388 (2007).
14
P. Dasgupta, Trust and cooperation among economic agents. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B Biol. Sci. 364, 3301–3309 (2009).
15
K. S. Cook, R. Hardin, M. Levi, Cooperation without Trust? (Russell Sage Foundation, 2005).
16
D. Gill, Y. Rosokha, Beliefs, learning, and personality in the indefinitely repeated prisoner’s dilemma. Am. Econ. J. Microecon. 16, 259–283 (2024).
17
J. Kim, L. Putterman, X. Zhang, Trust, beliefs and cooperation: Excavating a foundation of strong economies. Eur. Econ. Rev. 147, 104166 (2022).
18
K. Schmelz, Enforcement may crowd out voluntary support for COVID-19 policies, especially where trust in government is weak and in a liberal society. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 118, e2016385118 (2021).
19
B. Battu, T. Rahwan, Cooperation without punishment. Sci. Rep. 13, 1213 (2023).
20
U. Fischbacher, S. Gachter, E. Fehr, Are people conditionally cooperative? Evidence from a public goods experiment. Econ. Lett. 71, 397–404 (2001).
21
U. Fischbacher, S. Gächter, Social preferences, beliefs, and the dynamics of free riding in public goods experiments. Am. Econ. Rev. 100, 541–556 (2010).
22
S. Gächter, F. Kölle, S. Quercia, Reciprocity and the tragedies of maintaining and providing the commons. Nat. Hum. Behav. 1, 650–656 (2017).
23
C. Engel, S. Kube, M. Kurschilgen, Managing expectations: How selective information affects cooperation and punishment in social dilemma games. J. Econ. Behav. Organ. 187, 111–136 (2021).
24
R. Galbiati, K. H. Schlag, J. J. Van Der Weele, Sanctions that signal: An experiment. J. Econ. Behav. Organ. 94, 34–51 (2013).
25
J. Tyran, L. P. Feld, Achieving compliance when legal sanctions are non-deterrent. Scand. J. Econ. 108, 135–156 (2006).
26
M. Sutter, S. Haigner, M. G. Kocher, Choosing the carrot or the stick? Endogenous institutional choice in social dilemma situations. Rev. Econ. Stud. 77, 1540–1566 (2010).
27
R. Galbiati, P. Vertova, Obligations and cooperative behaviour in public good games. Games Econ. Behav. 64, 146–170 (2008).
28
E. Fehr, U. Fischbacher, Third-party punishment and social norms. Evol. Hum. Behav. 25, 63–87 (2004).
29
D. Balliet, L. B. Mulder, P. A. M. Van Lange, Reward, punishment, and cooperation: A meta-analysis. Psychol. Bull. 137, 594–615 (2011).
30
T. Markussen, L. Putterman, J.-R. Tyran, Self-organization for collective action: An experimental study of voting on sanction regimes. Rev. Econ. Stud. 81, 301–324 (2014).
31
K. Kamei, L. Putterman, J.-R. Tyran, State or nature? Endogenous formal versus informal sanctions in the voluntary provision of public goods. Exp. Econ. 18, 38–65 (2015).
32
N. Schwarz, G. L. Clore, Mood, misattribution, and judgments of well-being: Informative and directive functions of affective states. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 45, 513–523 (1983).
33
G. Hertel, J. Neuhof, T. Theuer, N. L. Kerr, Mood effects on cooperation in small groups: Does positive mood simply lead to more cooperation? Cogn. Emot. 14, 441–472 (2000).
34
D. Balliet, P. A. M. Van Lange, Trust, conflict, and cooperation: A meta-analysis. Psychol. Bull. 139, 1090–1112 (2013).
35
Y. Schul, R. Mayo, E. Burnstein, Encoding under trust and distrust: The spontaneous activation of incongruent cognitions. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 86, 668–679 (2004).
36
Y. Schul, R. Mayo, E. Burnstein, The value of distrust. J. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 44, 1293–1302 (2008).
37
M. Katzir, A.-C. Posten, Are there dominant response tendencies for social reactions? Trust trumps mistrust—Evidence from a Dominant Behavior Measure (DBM). J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 125, 57–81 (2023).
38
O. Schilke, M. Reimann, K. S. Cook, Trust in social relations. Annu. Rev. Sociol. 47, 239–259 (2021).
39
G. Spadaro et al., Corrupt third parties undermine trust and prosocial behaviour between people. Nat. Hum. Behav. 7, 46–54 (2022).
40
R. B. Cialdini, R. R. Reno, C. A. Kallgren, A focus theory of normative conduct: Recycling the concept of norms to reduce littering in public places. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 58, 1015 (1990).
41
K. Keizer, S. Lindenberg, L. Steg, The importance of demonstratively restoring order. PLoS One 8, e65137 (2013).
42
S. Gavrilets, P. J. Richerson, Collective action and the evolution of social norm internalization. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 114, 6068–6073 (2017).
43
M. M. Blair, L. A. Stout, Trust, trustworthiness, and the behavioral foundations of corporate law. Univ. Pa. Law Rev. 149, 1735 (2001).
44
E. Krupka, R. A. Weber, The focusing and informational effects of norms on pro-social behavior. J. Econ. Psychol. 30, 307–320 (2009).
45
A. P. Fiske, The four elementary forms of sociality: Framework for a unified theory of social relations. Psychol. Rev. 99, 689–723 (1992).
46
E. U. Weber, Climate change demands behavioral change: What are the challenges? Soc. Res. Int. Q. 82, 561–580 (2015).
47
Deutsche Gesellschaft für Psychologie (DGPs), Ethisches Handeln in der psychologischen Forschung Empfehlungen der Deutschen Gesellschaft für Psychologie für Forschende und Ethikkommissionen (Hogrefe, 2018).
48
A.-C. Posten, P. Ugurlar, S. Kube, J. Lammers, Vertical trust maintains cooperation. OSF. https://doi.org/10.17605/OSF.IO/DUVRC. Deposited 30 November 2024.
49
J. Andreoni, L. K. Gee, Gun for hire: Delegated enforcement and peer punishment in public goods provision. J. Public Econ. 96, 1036–1046 (2012).

Information & Authors

Information

Published in

The cover image for PNAS Vol.122; No.12
Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences
Vol. 122 | No. 12
March 25, 2025
PubMed: 40096613

Classifications

Data, Materials, and Software Availability

Data, code, and materials are deposited through the OSF at https://doi.org/10.17605/osf.io/duvrc (48). Studies were preregistered at https://aspredicted.org/ and are accessible via OSF (Studies 1, 2a, 2b, 3, and 4) (48).

Submission history

Received: August 28, 2024
Accepted: January 29, 2025
Published online: March 17, 2025
Published in issue: March 25, 2025

Keywords

  1. cooperation
  2. vertical trust
  3. punishment
  4. public good
  5. experiment

Acknowledgments

Funded by the Deutsche Forschungsgemeinschaft (DFG, German Research Foundation) under Germany’s Excellence Strategy—EXC 2126/1-390838866 and the research grant PO 1850/3-1 awarded to A.-C.P. Declaration of AI and AI-assisted technologies in the writing process: During the preparation of this work, we used you.com in order to improve readability and language. After using this service, we reviewed and edited the content as needed and take full responsibility for the content of the publication.
Author contributions
A.-C.P., P.U., S.K., and J.L. designed research; A.-C.P. performed research; A.-C.P. analyzed data; and A.-C.P., P.U., S.K., and J.L. wrote the paper.
Competing interests
The authors declare no competing interest.

Notes

This article is a PNAS Direct Submission.
*
In our four main experiments, the communicating authority is the experimenter, and in a complementary experiment (Study 2b), it is a participant who is granted this authority.
Studies were conducted in a different chronological order than the order in which they appear here. We thank two anonymous referees for suggesting complementary Study 2b.
For all studies, planned contrasts controlled for participants’ individual propensity to cooperate (i.e., round 1 contributions).

Authors

Affiliations

Department of Psychology, University of Limerick, Limerick V94 T9PX, Ireland
Department of Psychology, Özyeğin University, Istanbul 34794, Türkiye
Sebastian Kube
Institute for Applied Microeconomics, University of Bonn, Bonn 53113, Germany
Center for Economics and Neuroscience, University of Bonn, Bonn 53113, Germany
Max Planck Institute for Research on Collective Goods, Research Group Behavioral Law and Economics, Bonn 53113, Germany
Department of Psychology, Social Cognition Center Cologne, University of Cologne, Cologne 50931, Germany

Notes

1
To whom correspondence may be addressed. Email: [email protected].

Metrics & Citations

Metrics

Note: The article usage is presented with a three- to four-day delay and will update daily once available. Due to ths delay, usage data will not appear immediately following publication. Citation information is sourced from Crossref Cited-by service.


Altmetrics

Citations

Export the article citation data by selecting a format from the list below and clicking Export.

View Options

Login options

Check if you have access through your login credentials or your institution to get full access on this article.

Personal login Institutional Login

Recommend to a librarian

Recommend PNAS to a Librarian

Purchase options

Purchase this article to access the full text.

Single Article Purchase

Maintaining cooperation through vertical communication of trust when removing sanctions
Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences
  • Vol. 122
  • No. 12

View options

PDF format

Download this article as a PDF file

DOWNLOAD PDF

Figures

Tables

Media

Share

Share

Share article link

Share on social media