Flexible intuitions of Euclidean geometry in an Amazonian indigene group

Edited by Charles R. Gallistel, Rutgers University, Piscataway, NJ, and approved April 25, 2011 (received for review November 12, 2010)
May 23, 2011
108 (24) 9782-9787

Abstract

Kant argued that Euclidean geometry is synthesized on the basis of an a priori intuition of space. This proposal inspired much behavioral research probing whether spatial navigation in humans and animals conforms to the predictions of Euclidean geometry. However, Euclidean geometry also includes concepts that transcend the perceptible, such as objects that are infinitely small or infinitely large, or statements of necessity and impossibility. We tested the hypothesis that certain aspects of nonperceptible Euclidian geometry map onto intuitions of space that are present in all humans, even in the absence of formal mathematical education. Our tests probed intuitions of points, lines, and surfaces in participants from an indigene group in the Amazon, the Mundurucu, as well as adults and age-matched children controls from the United States and France and younger US children without education in geometry. The responses of Mundurucu adults and children converged with that of mathematically educated adults and children and revealed an intuitive understanding of essential properties of Euclidean geometry. For instance, on a surface described to them as perfectly planar, the Mundurucu's estimations of the internal angles of triangles added up to ∼180 degrees, and when asked explicitly, they stated that there exists one single parallel line to any given line through a given point. These intuitions were also partially in place in the group of younger US participants. We conclude that, during childhood, humans develop geometrical intuitions that spontaneously accord with the principles of Euclidean geometry, even in the absence of training in mathematics.

Continue Reading

Acknowledgments

This work is part of a larger project on the nature of quantification. It is based on psychological experiments and linguistics studies conducted in the Mundurucu territory (Pará, Brazil) under the direction of P.P. in accordance with the Consehlo de Desenvolvimento Cientifico et Tecnologicico and the Fundação do Indio (Funaï; Processo 2857/04). We thank L. Braga (SARAH Network of Neurorehabilitation Hospitals, Brazil), A. Ramos (Coordenação Geral de Educação, Funaï), and C. Romeiro (Nucleo de Documentação e Pesquisa, Funaï) for discussion and advice; M. Karu, Y.-H. Liu, and C. Tawe for help with data collection; P. Stephenson (The Array Rainforest Foundation) for the satellite map of the Mundurucu territory; and D. Sutherland and T. R. Virgil for comments on the manuscript. This work was supported by the Institut National de la Santé et de la Recherche Médicale, the Fyssen Foundation (to V.I.), the Département des Sciences Humaines et Sociales of Centre National de la Recherche Scientifique (to P.P.), the National Institutes of Health (to E.S.), a McDonnell Foundation centennial fellowship (to S.D.), and a Starting Grant from the European Research Council (MathConstruction 263179) (to V.I.).

Supporting Information

Supporting Information (PDF)
Supporting Information

References

1
H Poincare Space and Geometry, Available at http://ac-nancy-metz.fr/enseign/philo/textesph/poincare_espace_geometrie.pdf (in French). Accessed May 6, 2011. (1895).
2
G Hatfield The Natural and the Normative: Theories of Spatial Perception from Kant to Helmholtz (MIT Press, Cambridge, MA, 1998).
3
I Kant Critique of Pure Reason (Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, UK, 2005).
4
NS Newcombe, J Huttenlocher Making Space: The Development of Spatial Representation and Reasoning (MIT Press, Cambridge, MA, 2000).
5
S Dehaene, V Izard, P Pica, E Spelke, Core knowledge of geometry in an Amazonian indigene group. Science 311, 381–384 (2006).
6
RN Shepard, Perceptual-cognitive universals as reflections of the world. Behav Brain Sci 24, 581–601 (2001).
7
CR Gallistel The Organisation of Learning (MIT Press, Cambridge, MA, 1990).
8
K Cheng, NS Newcombe, Is there a geometric module for spatial orientation? Squaring theory and evidence. Psychon Bull Rev 12, 1–23 (2005).
9
G Vallortigara, VA Sovrano, C Chiandetti, Doing Socrates experiment right: Controlled rearing studies of geometrical knowledge in animals. Curr Opin Neurobiol 19, 20–26 (2009).
10
JW Kelly, TP McNamara, B Bodenheimer, TH Carr, JJ Rieser, The shape of human navigation: How environmental geometry is used in maintenance of spatial orientation. Cognition 109, 281–286 (2008).
11
G Grah, R Wehner, B Ronacher, Path integration in a three-dimensional maze: Ground distance estimation keeps desert ants Cataglyphis fortis on course. J Exp Biol 208, 4005–4011 (2005).
12
N Burgess, Spatial cognition and the brain. Ann N Y Acad Sci 1124, 77–97 (2008).
13
EI Moser, E Kropff, M-B Moser, Place cells, grid cells, and the brain's spatial representation system. Annu Rev Neurosci 31, 69–89 (2008).
14
EC Tolman, Cognitive maps in rats and men. Psychol Rev 55, 189–208 (1948).
15
B Landau, H Gleitman, E Spelke, Spatial knowledge and geometric representation in a child blind from birth. Science 213, 1275–1278 (1981).
16
N Chapuis, L Varlet, Shortcut by dogs in natural surroundings. Q J Exp Psychol 9, 49–64 (1987).
17
JL Gould, The locale map of honey bees: Do insects have cognitive maps? Science 232, 861–863 (1986).
18
MN Avraamides, RL Klatzky, JM Loomis, RG Golledge, Use of cognitive versus perceptual heading during imagined locomotion depends on the response mode. Psychol Sci 15, 403–408 (2004).
19
L Tcheang, HH Bülthoff, N Burgess, Visual influence on path integration in darkness indicates a multimodal representation of large-scale space. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 108, 1152–1157 (2011).
20
G Hatfield, Representation and constraints: The inverse problem and the structure of visual space. Acta Psychol (Amst) 114, 355–378 (2003).
21
JJ Koenderink, et al., Does monocular visual space contain planes? Acta Psychol (Amst) 134, 40–47 (2010).
22
RK Luneburg Mathematical Analysis of Binocular Vision (Princeton University Press, Princeton, 1947).
23
T Indow, A critical review of Luneburg's model with regard to global structure of visual space. Psychol Rev 98, 430–453 (1991).
24
JA Asmuth, LJ Rips, Conceptual change in non-Euclidean mathematics. Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society, pp. 30–35 (2006).
25
P Pica, C Lemer, V Izard, S Dehaene, Exact and approximate arithmetic in an Amazonian indigene group. Science 306, 499–503 (2004).
26
S Dehaene, V Izard, E Spelke, P Pica, Log or linear? Distinct intuitions of the number scale in Western and Amazonian indigene cultures. Science 320, 1217–1220 (2008).
27
P Pica, V Izard, S Dehaene, Mundurucu terms with quantitative, spatial or temporal content. Available at http://www.pierrepica.com/publications/., 2011).
28
V Izard, P Pica, ES Spelke, S Dehaene, Exact equality and successor function: Two Key concepts on the path towards understanding exact numbers. Philos Psychol 21, 491–505 (2008).
29
A Hupbach, L Nadel, Reorientation in a rhombic environment: No evidence for an encapsulated geometric module. Cogn Dev 20, 279–302 (2005).
30
A Shusterman, S Ah Lee, ES Spelke, Young children's spontaneous use of geometry in maps. Dev Sci 11, F1–F7 (2008).
31
V Izard, ES Spelke, Development of sensitivity to geometry in visual forms. Hum Evol 23, 213–248 (2009).
32
A Slater, A Mattock, E Brown, JG Bremner, Form perception at birth: Cohen and Younger (1984) revisited. J Exp Child Psychol 51, 395–406 (1991).
33
SF Lourenco, J Huttenlocher, The representation of geometric cues in infancy. Infancy 13, 103–127 (2008).
34
ES Spelke, SA Lee, V Izard, Beyond core knowledge: Natural geometry. Cogn Sci 34, 863–884 (2010).
35
B Landau, Early map use as an unlearned ability. Cognition 22, 201–223 (1986).

Information & Authors

Information

Published in

The cover image for PNAS Vol.108; No.24
Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences
Vol. 108 | No. 24
June 14, 2011
PubMed: 21606377

Classifications

Submission history

Published online: May 23, 2011
Published in issue: June 14, 2011

Keywords

  1. mathematical cognition
  2. spatial cognition
  3. culture

Acknowledgments

This work is part of a larger project on the nature of quantification. It is based on psychological experiments and linguistics studies conducted in the Mundurucu territory (Pará, Brazil) under the direction of P.P. in accordance with the Consehlo de Desenvolvimento Cientifico et Tecnologicico and the Fundação do Indio (Funaï; Processo 2857/04). We thank L. Braga (SARAH Network of Neurorehabilitation Hospitals, Brazil), A. Ramos (Coordenação Geral de Educação, Funaï), and C. Romeiro (Nucleo de Documentação e Pesquisa, Funaï) for discussion and advice; M. Karu, Y.-H. Liu, and C. Tawe for help with data collection; P. Stephenson (The Array Rainforest Foundation) for the satellite map of the Mundurucu territory; and D. Sutherland and T. R. Virgil for comments on the manuscript. This work was supported by the Institut National de la Santé et de la Recherche Médicale, the Fyssen Foundation (to V.I.), the Département des Sciences Humaines et Sociales of Centre National de la Recherche Scientifique (to P.P.), the National Institutes of Health (to E.S.), a McDonnell Foundation centennial fellowship (to S.D.), and a Starting Grant from the European Research Council (MathConstruction 263179) (to V.I.).

Notes

*This Direct Submission article had a prearranged editor.

Authors

Affiliations

Véronique Izard2,1 [email protected]
Laboratoire Psychologie de la Perception, Université Paris Descartes, 75006 Paris, France;
CNRS UMR 8158, 75006 Paris, France;
Department of Psychology, Harvard University, Cambridge, MA 02138;
Pierre Pica1
Laboratoire Structure Formelle du Langage, Université Paris 8, 93200 Saint-Denis, France;
CNRS UMR 7023, 93200 Saint Denis, France;
Elizabeth S. Spelke
Department of Psychology, Harvard University, Cambridge, MA 02138;
Stanislas Dehaene
Cognitive Neuroimaging Unit, Institut National de la Santé et de la Recherche Médicale, F-91191 Gif-sur-Yvette, France;
Commissariat à l'Energie Atomique, I2BM, NeuroSpin, F-91191 Gif-sur-Yvette, France;
Université Paris-Sud, F-91405 Orsay, France; and
Collège de France, 75005 Paris, France

Notes

2
To whom correspondence should be addressed. E-mail: [email protected].
Author contributions: V.I., P.P., E.S.S., and S.D. designed research; V.I. and P.P. performed research; V.I. and S.D. analyzed data; and V.I., P.P., E.S.S., and S.D. wrote the paper.
1
V.I. and P.P. contributed equally to this work.

Competing Interests

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

Metrics & Citations

Metrics

Note: The article usage is presented with a three- to four-day delay and will update daily once available. Due to ths delay, usage data will not appear immediately following publication. Citation information is sourced from Crossref Cited-by service.


Citation statements

Altmetrics

Citations

Export the article citation data by selecting a format from the list below and clicking Export.

Cited by

    Loading...

    View Options

    View options

    PDF format

    Download this article as a PDF file

    DOWNLOAD PDF

    Login options

    Check if you have access through your login credentials or your institution to get full access on this article.

    Personal login Institutional Login

    Recommend to a librarian

    Recommend PNAS to a Librarian

    Purchase options

    Purchase this article to access the full text.

    Single Article Purchase

    Flexible intuitions of Euclidean geometry in an Amazonian indigene group
    Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences
    • Vol. 108
    • No. 24
    • pp. 9727-10022

    Figures

    Tables

    Media

    Share

    Share

    Share article link

    Share on social media