Examining the effects of birth order on personality

Edited by Christopher F. Chabris, Union College, Schenectady, NY, and accepted by the Editorial Board September 17, 2015 (received for review April 1, 2015)
October 19, 2015
112 (46) 14224-14229
Commentary
Settling the debate on birth order and personality
Rodica Ioana Damian, Brent W. Roberts

Significance

The question of whether a person’s position among siblings has a lasting impact on that person’s life course has fascinated both the scientific community and the general public for >100 years. By combining large datasets from three national panels, we confirmed the effect that firstborns score higher on objectively measured intelligence and additionally found a similar effect on self-reported intellect. However, we found no birth-order effects on extraversion, emotional stability, agreeableness, conscientiousness, or imagination. This finding contradicts lay beliefs and prominent scientific theories alike and indicates that the development of personality is less determined by the role within the family of origin than previously thought.

Abstract

This study examined the long-standing question of whether a person’s position among siblings has a lasting impact on that person’s life course. Empirical research on the relation between birth order and intelligence has convincingly documented that performances on psychometric intelligence tests decline slightly from firstborns to later-borns. By contrast, the search for birth-order effects on personality has not yet resulted in conclusive findings. We used data from three large national panels from the United States (n = 5,240), Great Britain (n = 4,489), and Germany (n = 10,457) to resolve this open research question. This database allowed us to identify even very small effects of birth order on personality with sufficiently high statistical power and to investigate whether effects emerge across different samples. We furthermore used two different analytical strategies by comparing siblings with different birth-order positions (i) within the same family (within-family design) and (ii) between different families (between-family design). In our analyses, we confirmed the expected birth-order effect on intelligence. We also observed a significant decline of a 10th of a SD in self-reported intellect with increasing birth-order position, and this effect persisted after controlling for objectively measured intelligence. Most important, however, we consistently found no birth-order effects on extraversion, emotional stability, agreeableness, conscientiousness, or imagination. On the basis of the high statistical power and the consistent results across samples and analytical designs, we must conclude that birth order does not have a lasting effect on broad personality traits outside of the intellectual domain.

Continue Reading

Data Availability

Data deposition: All data used in this study are publicly available to scientific researchers. The NCDS data are available from the UK Data Service, ukdataservice.ac.uk; the NLSY97 data from the US Bureau of Labor Statistics, www.bls.gov/nls; and the SOEP data from the German Institute for Economic Research (DIW), www.diw.de/en/soep. To achieve full transparency and to allow the reproducibility of our analyses, the scripts for replicating our data analysis are archived in the Open Science Framework, https://osf.io/m2r3a.

Acknowledgments

The National Child Development Study (NCDS) data were made available by the Centre for Longitudinal Studies at the Institute of Education, University of London, and the UK Data Service. The National Longitudinal Survey of Youth (NLSY) data were made available by the U.S. Bureau of Labor Statistics. The Socio-Economic Panel (SOEP) data were made available by the German Institute for Economic Research (DIW). We thank these institutions for providing these datasets.

Supporting Information

Supporting Information (PDF)
Supporting Information

References

1
F Galton English Men of Science: Their Nature and Nurture (Thoemmes, Bristol, UK, 1874).
2
A Adler, Characteristics of the first, second and third child. Children 3, 14–52 (1928).
3
L Belmont, FA Marolla, Birth order, family size, and intelligence. Science 182, 1096–1101 (1973).
4
HM Breland, Birth order, family configuration, and verbal achievement. Child Dev 45, 1011–1019 (1974).
5
T Bjerkedal, P Kristensen, GA Skjeret, JI Brevik, Intelligence test scores and birth order among young Norwegian men (conscripts) analyzed within and between families. Intelligence 35, 503–514 (2007).
6
KJ Barclay, A within-family analysis of birth order and intelligence using population conscription data on Swedish men. Intelligence 49, 134–143 (2015).
7
RB Zajonc, GB Markus, Birth order and intellectual development. Psychol Rev 82, 74–88 (1975).
8
P Kristensen, T Bjerkedal, Explaining the relation between birth order and intelligence. Science 316, 1717 (2007).
9
C Ernst, J Angst Birth Order: Its Influence on Personality (Springer, Berlin, 1983).
10
C Schooler, Birth order effects: Not here, not now. Psychol Bull 78, 161–175 (1972).
11
FJ Sulloway Born to Rebel: Birth Order, Family Dynamics, and Creative Lives (Vintage, New York, 1996).
12
FJ Sulloway, Why siblings are like Darwin’s finches: Birth order, sibling competition and adaptive divergence within the family. The Evolution of Personality and Individual Differences, eds DM Buss, PH Hawley (Oxford Univ Press, Oxford), pp. 87–119 (2010).
13
OP John, LP Naumann, CJ Soto, Paradigm shift to the integrative Big-Five trait taxonomy: History, measurement, and conceptual issues. Handbook of Personality: Theory and Research, eds OP John, RW Robins, LA Pervin (Guilford, New York), pp. 114–158 (2008).
14
F Ostendorf, A Angleitner, Reflections on different labels for Factor V. Eur J Pers 8, 341–349 (1994).
15
FJ Sulloway, Birth order. Encyclopedia of Creativity, eds MA Runco, SR Pritzker (Academic, San Diego), pp. 189–202 (1999).
16
F Townsend, Birth order and rebelliousness: Reconstructing the research in “Born to Rebel”. Politics Life Sci 19, 135–156 (2000).
17
DL Paulhus, PD Trapnell, D Chen, Birth order effects on personality and achievement within families. Psychol Sci 10, 482–488 (1999).
18
T Jefferson, JH Herbst, RR McCrae, Associations between birth order and personality traits: Evidence from self-reports and observer ratings. J Res Pers 32, 498–509 (1998).
19
WD Parker, Birth-order effects in the academically talented. Gift Child Q 42, 29–38 (1998).
20
RL Michalski, TK Shackelford, An attempted replication of the relationships between birth order and personality. J Res Pers 36, 182–188 (2002).
21
A Bleske-Rechek, JA Kelley, Birth order and personality: A within-family test using independent self-reports from both firstborn and laterborn siblings. Pers Individ Dif 56, 15–18 (2014).
22
RI Damian, BW Roberts, The associations of birth order with personality and intelligence in a representative sample of U.S. high school students. J Res Pers 58, 96–105 (2015).
23
DL Paulhus, Birth order. Encyclopedia of Infant and Early Childhood Development, eds MM Haith, JB Benson (Academic, Amsterdam), pp. 204–211 (2008).
24
NC Herrera, RB Zajonc, G Wieczorkowska, B Cichomski, Beliefs about birth rank and their reflection in reality. J Pers Soc Psychol 85, 142–150 (2003).
25
; Centre for Longitudinal Studies, Institute of Education, University of London, National Child Development Study: Childhood Data, Sweeps 0-3, 1958–1974 (UK Data Archive, Colchester, Essex, UK). (2008).
26
; Centre for Longitudinal Studies, Institute of Education, University of London, National Child Development Study, Sweep 8, 2008–2009 (UK Data Archive, Colchester, Essex, UK). (2010).
27
; Bureau of Labor Statistics, US Department of Labor, National Longitudinal Survey of Youth (NLSY), 1997 cohort, 1997–2011 (rounds 1–15) (Center for Human Resource Research, The Ohio State University, Columbus, OH). (2013).
28
GG Wagner, JR Frick, J Schupp, The German Socio-Economic Panel Study (SOEP): Evolution, scope and enhancements. J Appl Soc Sci Stud 127, 139–169 (2007).
29
; German Institute for Economic Research (DIW), Socio-Economic Panel, Data for Years 1984-2013 (DIW/SOEP, Berlin), Version 30 beta. (2014).
30
J Specht, B Egloff, SC Schmukle, Stability and change of personality across the life course: The impact of age and major life events on mean-level and rank-order stability of the Big Five. J Pers Soc Psychol 101, 862–882 (2011).
31
PA Freund, N Kasten, How smart do you think you are? A meta-analysis on the validity of self-estimates of cognitive ability. Psychol Bull 138, 296–321 (2012).
32
JM Beer, JM Horn, The influence of rearing order on personality development within two adoption cohorts. J Pers 68, 789–819 (2000).
33
MD Healey, BJ Ellis, Birth order, conscientiousness, and openness to experience. Evol Hum Behav 28, 55–59 (2007).
34
L Festinger, A theory of social comparison processes. Hum Relat 7, 117–140 (1954).
35
JR Harris, Context-specific learning, personality, and birth order. Curr Dir Psychol Sci 9, 174–177 (2000).
36
VA Marini, JE Kurtz, Birth order differences in normal personality traits: Perspectives from within and outside the family. Pers Individ Dif 51, 910–914 (2011).
37
FJ Sulloway, Psychology. Birth order and intelligence. Science 316, 1711–1712 (2007).
38
LR Goldberg, et al., The international personality item pool and the future of public-domain personality measures. J Res Pers 40, 84–96 (2006).
39
SD Gosling, PJ Rentfrow, WB Swann, A very brief measure of the Big-Five personality domains. J Res Pers 37, 504–528 (2003).
40
FR Lang, D John, O Lüdtke, J Schupp, GG Wagner, Short assessment of the Big Five: Robust across survey methods except telephone interviewing. Behav Res Methods 43, 548–567 (2011).
41
P Shepherd 1958 National Child Development Study: Measures of Ability at Ages 7 to 16 (Centre for Longitudinal Studies, Institute of Education, University of London, London, 2012).
42
WA Sands, BK Waters, JR McBride Computerized Adaptive Testing: From Inquiry to Operation (American Psychological Association, Washington, DC, 1997).
43
S Lehrl, G Triebig, B Fischer, Multiple choice vocabulary test MWT as a valid and short test to estimate premorbid intelligence. Acta Neurol Scand 91, 335–345 (1995).
44
PD Allison Fixed Effects Regression Models (Sage, Los Angeles, 2009).

Information & Authors

Information

Published in

The cover image for PNAS Vol.112; No.46
Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences
Vol. 112 | No. 46
November 17, 2015
PubMed: 26483461

Classifications

Data Availability

Data deposition: All data used in this study are publicly available to scientific researchers. The NCDS data are available from the UK Data Service, ukdataservice.ac.uk; the NLSY97 data from the US Bureau of Labor Statistics, www.bls.gov/nls; and the SOEP data from the German Institute for Economic Research (DIW), www.diw.de/en/soep. To achieve full transparency and to allow the reproducibility of our analyses, the scripts for replicating our data analysis are archived in the Open Science Framework, https://osf.io/m2r3a.

Submission history

Published online: October 19, 2015
Published in issue: November 17, 2015

Keywords

  1. birth order
  2. personality
  3. Big Five
  4. within-family analyses
  5. siblings

Acknowledgments

The National Child Development Study (NCDS) data were made available by the Centre for Longitudinal Studies at the Institute of Education, University of London, and the UK Data Service. The National Longitudinal Survey of Youth (NLSY) data were made available by the U.S. Bureau of Labor Statistics. The Socio-Economic Panel (SOEP) data were made available by the German Institute for Economic Research (DIW). We thank these institutions for providing these datasets.

Notes

This article is a PNAS Direct Submission. C.F.C. is a guest editor invited by the Editorial Board.
*Sulloway postulated an opposing effect of birth order on dominance as a facet of extraversion (15). However, because the extraversion items in the questionnaires used in this study did not include the dominance facet, we were unable to test this additional hypothesis.
Parental age might be a potential confounding variable that is causing the effects on intelligence and intellect. For example, a higher paternal age at conception carries the risk of a higher number of new genetic mutations that might lower intelligence in later-borns. Assuming this kind of process, one would expect that spurious birth-order effects caused by differences in parental age would become larger with increasing age gaps between siblings. We tested this possibility by including the difference in age between the target person and firstborn as an additional predictor in our between- and within-family analyses of intelligence and intellect. Age differences did not significantly explain any variance above and beyond birth-order position in any of these four analyses (all P > 0.52). This result suggests that parental age is not the driving force behind the effects on intelligence and intellect.
See Commentary on page 14119.

Authors

Affiliations

Julia M. Rohrer
Department of Psychology, University of Leipzig, 04109 Leipzig, Germany;
Department of Psychology, Johannes Gutenberg University of Mainz, 55099 Mainz, Germany
Stefan C. Schmukle1 [email protected]
Department of Psychology, University of Leipzig, 04109 Leipzig, Germany;

Notes

1
To whom correspondence should be addressed. Email: [email protected].
Author contributions: B.E. and S.C.S. designed research; J.M.R. and S.C.S. analyzed data; and J.M.R., B.E., and S.C.S. wrote the paper.

Competing Interests

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

Metrics & Citations

Metrics

Note: The article usage is presented with a three- to four-day delay and will update daily once available. Due to ths delay, usage data will not appear immediately following publication. Citation information is sourced from Crossref Cited-by service.


Altmetrics

Citations

Export the article citation data by selecting a format from the list below and clicking Export.

Cited by

    Loading...

    View Options

    View options

    PDF format

    Download this article as a PDF file

    DOWNLOAD PDF

    Login options

    Check if you have access through your login credentials or your institution to get full access on this article.

    Personal login Institutional Login

    Recommend to a librarian

    Recommend PNAS to a Librarian

    Purchase options

    Purchase this article to access the full text.

    Single Article Purchase

    Examining the effects of birth order on personality
    Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences
    • Vol. 112
    • No. 46
    • pp. 14103-E6410

    Figures

    Tables

    Media

    Share

    Share

    Share article link

    Share on social media