Free access

The Dutch Hunger Winter and the developmental origins of health and disease

September 20, 2010
107 (39) 16757-16758
Research Article
Prenatal undernutrition and cognitive function in late adulthood
Susanne R. de Rooij, Hans Wouters [...] Tessa J. Roseboom
In the early 1980s, David Barker and others noted a paradox: although overall rates of cardiovascular disease increase with rising national prosperity, the least prosperous residents of a wealthy nation suffer the highest rates. He and others proposed over a series of studies that an adverse fetal environment followed by plentiful food in adulthood may be a recipe for adult chronic disease, a claim referred to as the Barker Hypothesis. These studies generally correlated birth weight and other infant parameters to the incidence of adult disease. Detractors, including an editorial in BMJ in 1995, complained that “[e]arly nutrition is inferred indirectly from fetal and infant growth, and fetal growth especially is a doubtful surrogate measure” (1). Most of the epidemiological studies were also vulnerable to confounding factors, particularly social class, that influence both intrauterine and adult environment, which delayed acceptance of the hypothesis. In PNAS, Rooij et al. (2) present another chapter in the ongoing study of the children of the Dutch Hunger Winter, a key test of the hypothesis (2). They show that, in addition to the previously shown effects of food restriction in utero on metabolism and cardiovascular health, there are effects on age-associated decline of cognitive functions. In the winter and spring of 1944 after a railway strike, the German occupation limited rations such that people, including pregnant women, in the western region of The Netherlands, including Amsterdam, received as little as 400–800 calories/d. The famine affected people of all social classes and was followed by growing prosperity in the postwar period. Thus, the Dutch Hunger Winter study, from which results were first published in 1976, provides an almost perfectly designed, although tragic, human experiment in the effects of intrauterine deprivation on subsequent adult health. This study has provided crucial support and fundamental insights for the growing field of the developmental origins of health and disease (DOHaD).

Birth Weight

One of the important observations from the Dutch Hunger Winter Study was that intrauterine exposures that have long-lasting consequences for adult health do not necessarily result in altered birth weight. Women exposed to the famine during mid- to late gestation had babies with significantly reduced birth weights. Babies whose mothers were exposed only during early gestation had normal birth weights; however, they grew up to have higher rates of obesity than those born before and after the war and higher rates than those exposed during mid- to late gestation (3). Thus, although reduced birth weight is the most easily measured proxy for intrauterine deprivation, it is not
Adverse fetal environment followed by plentiful food in adulthood may be a recipe for adult chronic disease.
the cause of later adult disease, and it does not always accompany the types of exposure that lead to adult disease. The programming of adult obesity by intrauterine food restriction without accompanying changes in birth weight has been replicated in rodent and sheep animal models (4, 5).

Critical Periods of Development

A related observation from the Dutch Hunger Winter Study has been the importance of timing in the programming of adult disease. Those who were exposed to the famine only during late gestation were born small and continued to be small throughout their lives, with lower rates of obesity as adults than in those born before and after the famine. However, as indicated above, those exposed during early gestation experienced elevated rates of obesity, altered lipid profiles, and cardiovascular disease. In contrast, markers of reduced renal function were specific to those exposed in mid-pregnancy (3). This idea is extended in the paper by Rooij et al. (2), which shows that only in those exposed to famine during early gestation is there a significant impairment in a test of selective attention at ages 56–59 y. The concept that there are critical windows during development existed before the DOHaD hypothesis, and it is relatively easily tested in laboratory animal experiments (4, 5). However, there is no ethical way to test prospectively for critical periods in human development, and such information is almost impossible to glean from most epidemiological studies. Hence, the Dutch Hunger Winter study is important because of its ability to provide insight into how a starvation diet during limited periods of gestation influences subsequent health of the offspring.
Interestingly, although the importance of exposure during early gestation was identified nearly 35 y ago, the reason that this period is important is still not fully understood. As discussed by Rooij et al. (2), CNS structures are formed in the first trimester of pregnancy, and changes underlying mental illness, altered appetite regulation centers, or even later declines in cognitive function likely occur during this period. Others have shown that alterations in placental growth, which outstrip that of the fetus in early gestation, are programmed by food restriction (6, 7). Because the placenta is responsible for providing the fetus with nutrients and oxygen and because it releases hormones that adapt the mother to fetal needs, any transient exposure to deprivation or excess affecting placental development can affect the fetus at later stages. Finally, animal experiments have shown that even nutritional changes occurring only during oocyte development and very early in gestation may permanently alter the methylation status of many genes and probably their subsequent expression (8).

Inappropriate Adaptation

One of the predictions made by the DOHaD is that fetal adaptations to scarcity become maladaptive only when affected individuals are later exposed to an environment of plenty. This is dramatically shown by comparing those exposed to the Dutch Hunger Winter with babies born after the siege of Leningrad. In both cases, pregnant women were exposed to severe famine. However, whereas The Netherlands returned to a complete diet quite quickly after the time of severe restriction, there were continuing shortages in the U.S.S.R., where those exposed to famine in utero did not exhibit higher rates of either obesity or cardiovascular disease as adults (9). It should be emphasized that those exposed to the Dutch Hunger Winter during late gestation (i.e., were born a few months into the famine) were also exposed during early infancy, whereas those exposed during early gestation (i.e., near the end of the famine) were born several months after the war when diets had improved. The importance of catch-up growth after adverse intrauterine conditions to the programming phenomenon has since been shown in numerous animal studies (10, 11).

New Directions

DOHaD has expanded considerably beyond examining the effects of nutrient restriction on subsequent rates of adult obesity and cardiovascular risks. The effects of other intrauterine exposures such as environmental toxicants (12), maternal disease states (13), and overnutrition on adult health have now been shown. Additional endpoints are also being examined, as in the PNAS report of accelerated cognitive aging in those exposed to the Dutch Hunger Winter during early gestation (2). Although the DOHaD field began largely within epidemiology and reproductive biology, studies like this one necessarily involve expertise in other areas, including psychology, developmental biology, and physiology of numerous systems. Much of the 20th century was dominated by debates about nature versus nurture. The DOHaD field has helped us come to a more nuanced understanding of the inseparable actions of nature and nurture that will inform biological investigations in multiple fields moving forward.

References

1
N Paneth, M Susser, Early origin of coronary heart disease (the “Barker hypothesis”). BMJ 310, 411–412 (1995).
2
WH Rooij, JE Yonker, RC Painter, TJ Roseboom, Prenatal undernutrition and cognitive function in late adulthood. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 107, 16881–16886 (2010).
3
T Roseboom, S de Rooij, R Painter, The Dutch famine and its long-term consequences for adult health. Early Hum Dev 82, 485–491 (2006).
4
AP Jones, MI Friedman, Obesity and adipocyte abnormalities in offspring of rats undernourished during pregnancy. Science 215, 1518–1519 (1982).
5
J Bispham, et al., Maternal endocrine adaptation throughout pregnancy to nutritional manipulation: Consequences for maternal plasma leptin and cortisol and the programming of fetal adipose tissue development. Endocrinology 144, 3575–3585 (2003).
6
J Dandrea, et al., Maternal nutritional manipulation of placental growth and glucose transporter 1 (GLUT-1) abundance in sheep. Reproduction 122, 793–800 (2001).
7
M Desai, CN Hales, Role of fetal and infant growth in programming metabolism in later life. Biol Rev Camb Philos Soc 72, 329–348 (1997).
8
MH Oliver, P Hawkins, JE Harding, Periconceptional undernutrition alters growth trajectory and metabolic and endocrine responses to fasting in late-gestation fetal sheep. Pediatr Res 57, 591–598 (2005).
9
SA Stanner, JS Yudkin, Fetal programming and the Leningrad Siege study. Twin Res 4, 287–292 (2001).
10
AP Jones, SA Assimon, MI Friedman, The effect of diet on food intake and adiposity in rats made obese by gestational undernutrition. Physiol Behav 37, 381–386 (1986).
11
M Desai, D Gayle, J Babu, MG Ross, Programmed obesity in intrauterine growth-restricted newborns: Modulation by newborn nutrition. Am J Physiol Regul Integr Comp Physiol 288, R91–R96 (2005).
12
WV Welshons, SC Nagel, FS vom Saal, Large effects from small exposures. III. Endocrine mechanisms mediating effects of bisphenol A at levels of human exposure. Endocrinology 147, S56–S69 (2006).
13
JA Stilley, R Woods-Marshall, M Sutovsky, P Sutovsky, KL Sharpe-Timms, Reduced fecundity in female rats with surgically induced endometriosis and in their daughters: A potential role for tissue inhibitors of metalloproteinase 1. Biol Reprod 80, 649–656 (2009).

Information & Authors

Information

Published in

Go to Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences
Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences
Vol. 107 | No. 39
September 28, 2010
PubMed: 20855592

Classifications

    Submission history

    Published online: September 20, 2010
    Published in issue: September 28, 2010

    Notes

    See companion article on page 16881.

    Authors

    Affiliations

    Laura C. Schulz1 [email protected]
    Department of Obstetrics, Gynecology and Women’s Health, University of Missouri, Columbia, MO 65211

    Notes

    Author contributions: L.C.S. wrote the paper.

    Competing Interests

    The author declares no conflict of interest.

    Metrics & Citations

    Metrics

    Note: The article usage is presented with a three- to four-day delay and will update daily once available. Due to ths delay, usage data will not appear immediately following publication. Citation information is sourced from Crossref Cited-by service.


    Citation statements




    Altmetrics

    Citations

    If you have the appropriate software installed, you can download article citation data to the citation manager of your choice. Simply select your manager software from the list below and click Download.

    Cited by

      Loading...

      View Options

      View options

      PDF format

      Download this article as a PDF file

      DOWNLOAD PDF

      Get Access

      Login options

      Check if you have access through your login credentials or your institution to get full access on this article.

      Personal login Institutional Login

      Recommend to a librarian

      Recommend PNAS to a Librarian

      Purchase options

      Purchase this article to access the full text.

      Single Article Purchase

      The Dutch Hunger Winter and the developmental origins of health and disease
      Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences
      • Vol. 107
      • No. 39
      • pp. 16751-17059

      Media

      Figures

      Tables

      Other

      Share

      Share

      Share article link

      Share on social media