Significance

Stereotypes reflect a society’s inequality and conflict, providing a diagnostic map of intergroup relations. This stereotype map’s fundamental dimensions depict each group’s warmth (friendly, sincere) and competence (capable, skilled). Some societies cluster groups as high on both (positive “us”) vs. low on both (negative “them”). Other societies, including the United States, have us-them clusters but add ambivalent ones (high on one dimension, low on the other). This cross-national study shows peace-conflict predicts ambivalence. Extremely peaceful and conflictual nations both display unambivalent us-them patterns, whereas intermediate peace-conflict predicts high ambivalence. Replicating previous work, higher inequality predicts more ambivalent stereotype clusters. Inequality and intermediate peace-conflict each use ambivalent stereotypes, explaining complicated intergroup relations and maintaining social system stability.

Abstract

A cross-national study, 49 samples in 38 nations (n = 4,344), investigates whether national peace and conflict reflect ambivalent warmth and competence stereotypes: High-conflict societies (Pakistan) may need clearcut, unambivalent group images distinguishing friends from foes. Highly peaceful countries (Denmark) also may need less ambivalence because most groups occupy the shared national identity, with only a few outcasts. Finally, nations with intermediate conflict (United States) may need ambivalence to justify more complex intergroup-system stability. Using the Global Peace Index to measure conflict, a curvilinear (quadratic) relationship between ambivalence and conflict highlights how both extremely peaceful and extremely conflictual countries display lower stereotype ambivalence, whereas countries intermediate on peace-conflict present higher ambivalence. These data also replicated a linear inequality–ambivalence relationship.
Objective and social environmental factors affect individual psychology (e.g., ref. 1). Geography and climate, as well as economic, political, and religious systems, influence thinking and action (2, 3). For instance, income inequality worsens social cohesion (e.g., refs. 4, 5), quality of governance supports well-being (6), and natural environments improve self-regulation (7). Here, conflict within and between societies can predict group stereotypes.
According to the stereotype content model (SCM) (8), stereotypes are not just negative; many societal stereotypes are instead ambivalent, combining positive and negative descriptions of a group. Stereotypes array along two fundamental dimensions of social perception, namely, warmth (sociability, sincerity) and competence (capability, skill); ambivalent stereotypes portray groups as either warm but not competent (disabled people) or as cold but competent (rich people). Groups penalized on one dimension are compensated on the other (9). The positive description may mask the negative description, making ambivalent stereotypes acceptable even to targets (e.g., ref. 10). Mixed combinations rationalize the system.
Warmth and competence map group stereotypes across societies (11, 12), and many societies’ stereotypes are ambivalent (13). However, societies vary in use of ambivalent stereotypes, and such variations link to income inequality: More unequal societies display more ambivalent stereotypes (11). If ambivalence helps maintain societal hierarchies, more equal societies may stereotype fewer groups ambivalently, because most groups deserve inclusion in the social safety net. Relatively unequal societies, instead, may need more ambivalence to mask income disparities, rationalizing unfair conditions, namely, by mixed stereotypes (e.g., deserving and undeserving poor). In support (14), poor people appear less competent (but warmer) and rich people appear colder (but more competent) in relatively unequal vs. equal countries: Given high inequality, the status quo may be reinforced by undermining poor people on the status-relevant dimension (i.e., competence) and rich people on the status-irrelevant dimension, thus justifying the groups’ respective positions in the hierarchy. These data link a distal factor, inequality, with group stereotypes.
However, inequality may also increase the odds of conflict (1517). Intergroup conflict abets negative stereotypes of the “enemy” (18): Negative traits ascribed to all category members justify social actions, dividing “us” vs. “them” (19). Conflicting parties hold mutually negative images (2022), reinforcing the conflict. Thus, countries’ conflict may perpetuate us-them stereotypes.
This cross-national study investigates how a country’s peace/conflict predicts more mixed ascriptions of warmth and competence to its groups. Low-conflict countries are, by definition, unified: Identity is uncontested, perhaps ethnically homogeneous. Switzerland and the Scandinavian countries may be prototypes of peaceful collectives, with harmonious shared identities. National identity is a more unitary whole; thus, various citizen subgroups should receive positive evaluations on both competence and warmth, and in Western settings, favoring the in-groups (12). Noncitizen intruders (refugees, nomads, and undocumented migrants) are simply excluded (negatively evaluated on both dimensions) in our previous data.
More contested, perhaps multiethnic, identity needs more subgroups. Friction likely rises, but conflict is moderate, given a stable system. The Americas, a mix of indigenous peoples as well as histories and continuing flows of immigrants, are prototypes. National subgroups’ distinct fates (deserving vs. undeserving immigrants) may require explanation, namely, ambivalent stereotypes.
If open internal conflict breaks out, civil war might create stark us-them identities, simplifying the map (less ambivalence). Divisions are sharpened. Similarly, if the open conflict is external [the Global Peace Index (GPI) used here combines both internal and external types], citizens may also create stark us-them identities, including all citizens as a united front against external enemies (consider the United States just after “9/11” or during World War II).
To summarize, very peaceful countries may need less ambivalence because most groups fit in the shared national identity, with few outcasts [as earlier data (11) on highly equal societies suggest]; high-conflict societies may also need less ambivalent images of groups to simplify the world, making a clear-cut distinction between friends and foes (1921); and, finally, nations with intermediate conflict may need ambivalence for system stability due to ambiguous intergroup relations, neither fully equal and peaceful nor fully conflictual. As a secondary aim, this work seeks to corroborate the inequality–ambivalence relationship (11).

Overview of Present Research

The GPI measures peace-conflict, and the Gini index measures income inequality. The GPI is produced by the Institute for Economics and Peace, first in 2007, with updates annually. The GPI comprises 22–24 qualitative and quantitative indicators from various sources (e.g., Economist Intelligence Unit, United Nations Office on Drugs and Crime) on three societal themes: safety and security, domestic or international conflict, and militarization. A higher GPI means lower peace, and more conflict. GPI coefficients consistently correlate with other measures of peace (23). This study relates a nation’s GPI to its stereotypes. The Gini index measures inequality in societal distribution of income. As the American Central Intelligence Agency reports, it plots cumulative family income against cumulative number of families, from poorest to richest. Gini coefficients range from 0 (complete equality) to 100 (complete inequality).
Cross-national analyses used 49 samples in 38 nations (Materials and Methods; demographics are provided in Tables S1 and S2). Each nation’s societal groups were rated on the two fundamental dimensions of warmth and competence (Materials and Methods). High warmth-competence (W-C) correlations reflect lower ambivalence, and low ones reflect more ambivalence. To generate cultural maps and to maintain comparability with earlier efforts, new stereotype content data were collected following earlier precedents (8, 11, 12). The new data collection focused on high-conflict countries and on extremely equal countries so as to test the hypotheses regarding peace-conflict and ambivalence. The new data also tested the robustness of earlier results on inequality and ambivalence.
Table S1.
Demographic information in all samples: Preliminary group-listing study
  Participants 
CountryNStudents, nNonstudents, nFemale, %Mean age, yNo. in selected groups
AfghanistanTwo local judges generated the list of groups21
Australia3535 85.7128.5730
BelgiumUnknown    19
Bolivia (UMSA)7070 48.5721.4028
Bolivia (UPB-CB)5858 50.0019.7033
Canada100100 58.0019.4030
Chile104UnknownUnknownUnknownUnknown14
Costa Rica4343 37.00 26
Denmark40 40UnknownUnknown20
Egypt32 3250.0035.5316
England3124738.7135.5030
Finland4141 83.0023.0025
Germany18 1866.6729.0029
Greece62323058.0628.2026
Hong Kong3030 63.0020.0027
India5656 37.5022.0733
Iran4040 30.0021.0337
Iraq30 3040.0033.1024
Israel (Jews)Unknown    25
Israel (Muslims)Unknown    26
Italy55352058.1831.5427
Japan5353 72.0021.3021
Jordan30 3040.0032.5326
Kenya63313239.7024.4821
Lebanon30 3033.3336.0723
Malaysia70343664.2925.9828
Mexico30151550.00 18
New Zealand47 4765.9629.9030
Norway40 4050.0030.9325
Pakistan30 3046.6730.7730
Peru4747 48.9019.9428
Portugal55401547.2737.8020
South Africa6868 50.00Unknown20
South Korea2828 65.00Unknown23
Spain8383 77.1118.8028
Sweden4040 72.5026.7218
Switzerland (Swiss-French)33231051.5227.5824
Switzerland (Swiss-Germans)61313050.8226.0727
Switzerland (Swiss-Italians)49351463.2720.2228
Turkey30 3033.3336.6020
Uganda3030Unknown50.82Unknown19
United States34 3470.59Unknown23
UMSA, Universidad Mayor de San Andres; UPB-CB, Universidad Privada Boliviana-Cochabamba.
Table S2.
Demographic information in all samples: Main survey
SamplesNStudents, nNonstudents, nFemale, %Mean age, y
Afghanistan7171 50.7023.21
Australia (Asians)4646 39.1319.94
Australia (Europeans)4848 60.4223.02
Belgium9191 77.9119.48
Bolivia (UCB)7979 70.9020.58
Bolivia (UMSA)7474 65.7021.96
Bolivia (UPB-CB)6363 57.1020.05
Bolivia (UPB)7070 40.0020.41
Canada272272 78.0019.13
Chile7979 72.2019.97
Costa Rica9898 67.3022.30
Denmark8484 Unknown28.50
Egypt6060 50.0021.53
England6565 30.3219.60
Finland132132 67.0028.00
Germany8282 54.8723.05
Greece164164 87.8019.10
Hong Kong6060 58.00Unknown
India102102 37.6019.72
Iran101101 27.7223.11
Iraq6060 48.3322.55
Israel (Arabs)100100 Unknown21.77
Israel (Jews)104104 Unknown24.25
Italy (nonstudents)41 4160.9842.29
Italy (students)180180 72.2020.84
Japan8282 54.0021.30
Jordan6161 51.6221.39
Kenya (nonstudents)62 6237.1032.32
Kenya (students)6565 13.8521.45
Lebanon6767 40.3021.60
Malaysia168168 81.5521.42
Mexico9090 49.4024.57
New Zealand (Europeans)30 3070.0040.23
New Zealand (Maori)30 3070.0037.23
Norway*2446416150.0035.04
Pakistan78621655.1323.78
Peru7070 61.4319.97
Portugal5757 91.2019.05
South Africa190190 43.1023.31
South Korea9191 60.0022.50
Spain8585 80.0018.71
Sweden6767 58.2124.46
Switzerland (Swiss-French nonstudents)85 8552.3040.54
Switzerland (Swiss-French student)8989 66.3021.63
Switzerland (Swiss-Germans)8383 78.3021.13
Switzerland (Swiss-Italians)5858 65.5018.38
Turkey5757 56.1421.65
Uganda3636 36.1131.06
United States73 7347.0035.24
Further details are provided in the study by Durante et al. (11).
*
Nineteen participants in the Norwegian sample did not provide information about their occupation.

Results

Preliminary Analyses.

Competence and warmth items related to each societal group were averaged across participants in each sample. Subsequently, cluster analyses explored the distribution of means in the W × C 2D Euclidian space, using agglomerative hierarchical cluster analysis (24) to determine the number of clusters and k-means cluster analysis (centroids method) to establish the societal groups in each cluster. Five clusters fit for eight of 16 new samples, and four clusters fit for the other eight. In each sample, competence and warmth were compared within clusters (paired t test) and between clusters (one-way ANOVAs, post hoc Bonferroni’s correction).
We count the ambivalent groups following these criteria: (i) Only within-cluster warmth and competence comparisons, as well as between-cluster, within-dimension (high-low) comparisons, that result in P ≤ 0.05 are considered ambivalent. (ii) A few cases of ambivalent-looking “clusters” have just one group, so neither between- nor within-cluster comparisons are applicable. Looking at the means, however, if we have little doubt that those groups are evaluated ambivalently (i.e., the warmth and competence means differ by a full scale point), we include those groups as well. (iii) The new US ambivalence clusters fit the within-cluster comparisons at P < 0.05, but one of the between-cluster comparisons is P < 0.08, although it fits the one-scale-point rule, and also fits several previous US studies (8, 25). Cluster maps for all these samples are available at www.fiskelab.org/publications, where the study by Durante et al. (11) is cited (“To see cross-cultural warmth and competence maps, click here”). Of course, the descriptive visual maps complement the quantitative correlational analysis.
In Afghan, Finnish, German, Iranian, Jordanian, Norwegian, and Turkish samples, most groups (from 60 to 90.5%) landed in ambivalent clusters. Iraqi, Kenyan (students), Lebanese, and Swedish groups were distributed almost equally into ambivalent and univalent clusters. Danish, Egyptian, Kenyan (nonstudents), Pakistani, and US groups had more groups in univalent than ambivalent clusters.
As previously reported (11), groups’ distribution in the W × C space presented different patterns, some as a circular cloud of points, implying groups dispersed in the four quadrants of the space (both ambivalent and univalent), as Fig. 1 shows. This kind of distribution results in an approximately zero overall W-C correlation, calculated at the societal level within each sample. Other distributions were in the shape of a linear vector (Figs. 2 and 3, from the bottom left to the top right), with most groups evaluated univalently as high or low on both dimensions, resulting in a positive W-C correlation. In one case, a vector ran from the bottom right to the top left (i.e., Jordan), indicating a negative W-C correlation (all ambivalence). Lower W-C correlations implied more ambivalence, whereas higher and positive W-C correlations indicated less ambivalence at a societal level. In other words, the overall W-C correlation indexed ambivalence. The 16 new samples’ W-C correlations ranged from −0.37 (P = 0.07) to 0.92 (P < 0.001), with an average r = 0.27 (Table S3).
Fig. 1.
US cluster analysis. Stars indicate cluster centroids. C, competence; H, high; L, low; W, warmth. A regression line is plotted. The United States has an intermediate (peace-conflict) GPI score of 2.056 and shows stereotype ambivalence (W-C r = 0.11).
Fig. 2.
Pakistan cluster analysis. Stars indicate cluster centroids. M, medium. A regression line is plotted. Pakistan has a high-conflict GPI score of 3.106 and shows low stereotype ambivalence (W-C r = 0.92).
Fig. 3.
Denmark cluster analysis. Stars indicate cluster centroids. A regression line is plotted. Denmark has a low-conflict GPI score of 1.193 and shows low stereotype ambivalence (W-C r = 0.58).
Table S3.
W-C correlations: Level of significance in 16 new samples
SampleW-C
rP
Jordan−0.370.07
Kenya (student sample)−0.280.22
Egypt−0.150.58
Lebanon0.050.81
Germany0.080.68
USA0.110.63
Iraq0.270.20
Sweden0.310.21
Kenya (nonstudent sample)0.330.15
Iran0.440.007
Afghanistan0.470.032
Finland0.480.015
Turkey0.520.019
Denmark0.580.008
Norway0.580.002
Pakistan0.92<0.001
Data are listed according to the W-C correlations: ascending order.

Cross-National Analyses.

The database (49 samples) adds 16 new W-C correlations to previous data. The combined data (Table S4) show 26 significantly positive correlations (i.e., a univalent vector) and 22 nonsignificant correlations (i.e., a cloud that includes ambivalent quadrants). GPI coefficients range from 1.193 [lowest conflict (Denmark)] to 3.416 [highest conflict (Afghanistan)], with a median of 1.724. Gini coefficients range from 24.80 [low inequality (Denmark)] to 63.10 [high inequality (South Africa)], with a median of 36.20. Peaceful and equal countries are more represented, but the database includes extremes of both inequality and conflict.
Table S4.
W-C correlation: GPI and Gini coefficients in all samples
SampleData collection, yW-C, rGPI (y)Gini (y)
Denmark20140.58**1.193 (2014)24.80 (2011)
New Zealand (European)20090.51**1.202 (2009)36.20 (1997)
New Zealand (Maori)20090.48**1.202 (2009)36.20 (1997)
Finland20140.48*1.297 (2014)26.80 (2008)
Norway20130.58**1.359 (2013)26.80 (2010)
Sweden20140.311.381 (2014)24.90 (2013)
Switzerland (French nonstudent)20090.80***1.393 (2009)29.60 (2010)
Switzerland (French student)20090.65**1.393 (2009)29.60 (2010)
Switzerland (German)20090.281.393 (2009)29.60 (2010)
Switzerland (Italian)20090.76***1.393 (2009)29.60 (2010)
Japan20050.031.413 (2007)38.10 (2002)
Canada2008−0.031.451 (2008)32.10 (2005)
Australia (Asians)20090.65***1.476 (2009)30.30 (2008)
Australia (Europeans)20090.56**1.476 (2009)30.30 (2008)
Chile20090.241.481 (2009)52.10 (2009)
Portugal20070.61**1.481 (2007)38.50 (2007)
Belgium20050.48*1.498 (2007)28.00 (2005)
Germany20050.081.523 (2007)28.00 (2005)
Hong Kong20050.121.657 (2007)53.30 (2007)
Spain20080.60**1.683 (2008)32.00 (2005)
Costa Rica20050.361.702 (2007)49.80 (2003)
South Korea2005−0.071.719 (2007)35.10 (2006)
Malaysia20080.74***1.721 (2008)46.20 (2009)
Italy (nonstudent)20050.311.724 (2007)33.00 (2005)
Italy (student)20050.231.724 (2007)33.00 (2005)
England20080.46*1.801 (2008)33.40 (2010)
Jordan2013−0.37****1.858 (2013)39.70 (2007)
Greece20080.61**1.867 (2008)33.00 (2005)
Peru2009−0.011.972 (2009)48.00 (2009)
Bolivia (UCB)20090.39*1.990 (2009)58.20 (2009)
Bolivia (UMSA)20090.261.990 (2009)58.20 (2009)
Bolivia (UPB-BC)20090.54**1.990 (2009)58.20 (2009)
Bolivia (UPB)20090.41*1.990 (2009)58.20 (2009)
USA20100.112.056 (2010)45.00 (2007)
Mexico2005−0.192.125 (2007)50.90 (2005)
Uganda20090.5702.140 (2009)44.30 (2009)
Egypt2013−0.142.258 (2013)30.80 (2008)
South Africa20050.112.399 (2007)65.00 (2005)
Iran20140.44**2.437 (2014)44.50 (2006)
Turkey20130.52*2.437 (2013)40.20 (2010)
Kenya (nonstudent)20140.332.452 (2014)42.50 (2008)
Kenya (student)2014−0.282.452 (2014)42.50 (2008)
India20100.72***2.516 (2010)33.60 (2012)
Lebanon20130.052.575 (2013)
Israel (Arabs)20050.81***3.033 (2007)38.60 (2005)
Israel (Jews)20050.083.033 (2007)38.60 (2005)
Pakistan20130.92***3.106 (2013)29.60 (2011)
Iraq20130.273.245 (2013)
Afghanistan20140.47*3.416 (2014)
Data are listed according to the GPI coefficient (in ascending order). Australia (two samples: Asian-Australians and European-Australians), Bolivia [UPB-CB, UMSA, Universidad Catolica Boliviana (UCB), and Universidad Privada Boliviana (UPB)], Kenya (two samples: students and nonstudents), Israel (two samples, Israeli-Jews and Israeli-Arabs), Italy (two samples, students and nonstudents), New Zealand (two samples: European-New Zealanders and Maori-New Zealanders), Switzerland (four samples: Swiss-German students, Swiss-Italian students, Swiss-French students, and Swiss-French nonstudents).
*
P ≤ 0.05; **P ≤ 0.01;***P ≤ 0.001; ****P = 0.07.
Having data across continents and through many years, and indexing ambivalence as a correlation (i.e., an effect size), meta-analysis techniques appear appropriate. The Hedges–Olkin–Vevea method (26, 27) was applied to the random-effect model for unconditional inferences, allowing the overall sample to comprise samples from different underlying populations (Supporting Information). Meta-regression modeled the relationship between the SCM index of societal ambivalence and the GPI and Gini national indexes. Regression weighted each sample as the inverse of sampling variance, plus a constant representing variability across population effects (28). The MetaReg SPSS macro (29), with maximum likelihood, estimated the model, approximating inverse variance weighting by transformed sample size. Each correlation was Fisher-standardized (27).
The hypothesized relationship between conflict and ambivalence is curvilinear (both peaceful and conflictual nations display lower ambivalence than intermediate ones). First, to exclude the possibility of a linear relationship, we regressed W-C Fisher-standardized correlations onto GPI coefficients, and the model was not significant (Q < 1). To verify a curvilinear relationship, GPI coefficients were mean-centered, and then squared. Both the centered GPI and squared, centered GPI were entered in the meta-regression as independent variables, with W-C Fisher-standardized correlations as the response. The analysis yielded significance [Q(2) = 6.40, P = 0.041], accounting for 11.7% of the variance. As Table 1 shows, the 95% confidence intervals (CIs) for the linear component contained zero, whereas the 95% CIs of the quadratic component did not, suggesting a nonmonotonic relationship between ambivalence and conflict. [The estimate of the intercept variance is ν = 0.12, SE(ν) = 0.03.] (Further tests are discussed in Supporting Information.) As Fig. 4 illustrates, both very peaceful and very conflictual countries show lower ambivalence, whereas countries with intermediate conflict mostly present higher ambivalence. Adding other societal variables [gross domestic product (GDP) for wealth and Human Development Index (HDI) for development] does not eliminate the curvilinear pattern, and there were no outliers (Supporting Information).
Table 1.
Inverse variance-weighted regression results: Moderating role of GPI and Gini on ambivalence
National indexb (95% CI)SEβZP
GPI     
GPI-centered−0.20 (−0.43, 0.04)0.12−0.28−1.610.11
GPI-centered squared0.38 (0.08, 0.67)0.150.452.530.012
 
Gini−0.01 (−0.02, −0.0004)0.006−0.29−2.030.043
Mixed model (random intercept, fixed slopes); maximum likelihood estimate.
Fig. 4.
GPI coefficients and raw W-C correlations. A curvilinear (quadratic) pattern is significant (Table 1). UCB, Universidad Catolica Boliviana; UMSA, Universidad Mayor de San Andres; UPB, Universidad Privada Boliviana; UPB-CB, Universidad Privada Boliviana-Cochabamba.
Inverse variance-weighted meta-regression also replicated inequality predicting ambivalence. W-C Fisher-standardized correlations were regressed onto the Gini coefficients. The model explained 8.3% of the variance [Q(1) = 4.11, P = 0.043]; the regression coefficient was significant, and the related 95% CIs did not contain zero (Table 1). This finding corroborates previous findings: more income inequality and more societal ambivalence (Supporting Information).

Discussion

The stereotype ambivalence first discovered in North America and in some samples in Western Europe, East Asia, Africa, and South America replicates here mainly for relatively unequal countries intermediate on the peace-conflict continuum. Besides admiring the middle class as high on both dimensions and rejecting homeless people as low on both, intermediate nations, such as the United States, find older people to be warm but incompetent and rich people to be cold but competent, both ambivalent images that situate some poor people as deserving and others as not (likewise for higher status people). Ambivalent stereotypes can reinforce inequality (11), as well as intermediate peace-conflict.
In contrast, univalent vectors appear in two kinds of nations. First, peaceful, equal countries (Denmark) report positive stereotypes for social groups that share national identity (citizens, middle class, students, older people, retired people), but also report negative stereotypes of societal outcasts (criminals, immigrants, Muslims). Univalent stereotypes can reinforce equality, but only for the communal in-group. Second, extremely conflict-ridden countries (Pakistan) report positive stereotypes of in-groups and allies (Muslims in general, educated people) and negative stereotypes of outcasts (beggars, illiterate people) and foes (Christians). Such univalent stereotypes can reinforce conflict.
The limitations of these data suggest future research. First, although the effect sizes are medium by social science standards and significant by statistical convention, the variance explained is small; other explanations for stereotype ambivalence await discovery. Still, the data offer insights about stereotypes, especially ambivalent ones, in conflictual societies and in peaceful, equal societies.
Second, the data include few high-conflict countries so far. Unfortunately, we could not widen our survey among similar countries due to inaccessibility of data. Despite this flaw, the few high-conflict countries provided powerful insight into the ambivalence pattern. The GPI reveals a previously unexplored nonmonotonic relationship in stereotype ambivalence.
Another challenge typifies cross-cultural research: Potential confounds abound, and are not all ruled out. Similarly, broad-brush conclusions do not capture each nation’s every particularity in stereotype mapping. Our speculation that peaceful, equal, unambivalent countries include more groups in their shared identity, excluding those groups beyond the pale, does not explain all groups.
Further, as comparative correlational research, the data do not show causality. Ambivalence predicts inequality, and vice versa; univalence predicts extremes of peace-conflict, and vice versa. Ideally, longitudinal data could track a nation’s changing peace-conflict score, as a natural experiment. Unfortunately, we do not have GPI data before 2007, nor do we have longitudinal stereotype data, except in the United States (30): Four data points over 70+ y do not show changing ambivalence, but the data points are not positioned by changing national peace-conflict levels.
Finally, the GPI is a composite of both internal and external peace-conflict, although our participants evaluated groups within their own society. Still, the GPI calculation weights internal indicators more than external ones (60% vs. 40%). This decision relies on the “notion that a greater level of internal peace is likely to lead to, or at least correlate with, lower external conflict; in other words, if ‘charity begins at home,’ so might peace” (ref. 31, p. 6). More broadly, charity might also reflect inclusive group images, expanding the in-group to include groups viewed ambivalently elsewhere, with resulting societal peace.

Materials and Methods

Data on societal ambivalent stereotypes came partly from an earlier database (11) composed of 37 samples collected in 25 nations. Here, to explore the ambivalence–conflict relationship optimally, it was pivotal to include samples drawn from highly conflictual countries, which were hard to get and therefore absent from previous work. Similarly, to corroborate the ambivalence-inequality findings, it was crucial to collect data in countries with high equality, which were also absent from previous work. Therefore, the earlier database (11) was integrated with 16 new samples: 13 new samples collected between 2013 and 2014 [in Afghanistan, Egypt, Iran, Iraq, Jordan, Kenya (two samples: students and nonstudents), Lebanon, Pakistan, and Turkey (i.e., countries with high conflict) and in Denmark, Finland, and Sweden (i.e., countries with the highest available recorded level of equality, and peace, in the world]. [In Afghanistan, Egypt, Iraq, Jordan, Lebanon, Pakistan, Turkey, and Kenya, participants completed the questionnaire (both for the preliminary and main surveys) in return for about US $10.] Moreover, we added groups’ warmth and competence data from Norway (ref. 32, study 1) and Germany (33) (again, very equal countries), which were collected independently and reanalyzed here. Notably, because the two US samples included in the earlier database (11) were collected in the year 2000 (before 9/11 and before the GPI reports), we decided to replace them with more recent US data (ref. 34, study 1). Additionally, we reckoned that the two Northern Irish samples should not be included in the study because the GPI coefficient referred to the whole UK area, therefore masking the level of conflict in Northern Ireland. [The Institute for Economics and Peace has recently released the UK Peace Index (UKPI 2013); it divides United Kingdom into areas. “The most peaceful region in the United Kingdom is South East England. The least peaceful region is Greater London, immediately preceded by Scotland and Northern Ireland” (ref. 35, p. 17)]. Cross-national analyses used 49 samples collected in 38 nations. (See also Table S5.) The Princeton University Institutional Review Board approved all procedures, including the consent form, and all participants provided informed consent.
Table S5.
Inverse variance-weighted regression results: Moderating role of GPI and Gini on ambivalence in 53 samples
National indexKb (95% CI)SEβZP
GPI53    
GPI centered −0.18 (−0.43, 0.07)0.13−0.24−1.440.15
GPI centered, squared 0.33 (0.02, 0.64)0.160.342.070.04
 
Gini*50−0.01 (−0.02, −0.002)0.006−0.30−2.220.027
Random model (random intercept, fixed slopes); maximum likelihood estimation. K, number of samples included in the meta-analysis. GPI meta-regression model [Q(2) = 4.36, P = 0.11], Gini meta-regression model [Q(1) = 4.91, P = 0.027].
*
Gini coefficients were not available for Afghanistan, Iraq, and Lebanon.

Preliminary Group-Listing Study.

In each country, a preliminary study identified societal groups that could be considered as most salient. A self-administered, open-ended questionnaire asked participants to list which types of people are generally categorized into groups in their society, what are the low-status groups, and what groups they belong to. [In Jordan and Afghanistan, participants listed the criteria used by their society to categorize people into groups (i.e., age, race, ability). In Jordan, a follow-up questionnaire asked the same participants to provide specific examples of social groups based on the criteria they mentioned. In Afghanistan, two local judges listed the most salient groups in the current Afghan scene; a similar approach was used by Fiske et al. (8) in study 1.] The questionnaire was administered in the nations’ languages. (In Kenya, participants could complete the questionnaire in either an English or Kiswahili version of the questionnaire.) In all, 406 participants (n = 30–42), who were mostly nonstudents (62.56%) and 47.5% female, with a weighted mean age = 28.98 y, completed the questionnaire on a voluntary basis. Groups mentioned by at least 15% of participants then appeared in that country’s main survey questionnaire. Across the new samples, the number of distinct groups ranged between 16 and 37. An analogous procedure generated the list of groups in Germany (33), Norway (32), and the United States (34). Table S1 summarizes demographic information for each sample in our extended database: With total numbers missing for three samples, ∼1,796 participants (54.81% female, weighted mean age = 25.92) took part in this phase.

Main Survey.

Participants.

As mentioned, 13 new samples were recruited, one from each country, except for Kenya, where we collected data from both students and nonstudents. Respondents (n = 965) voluntarily participated in the main survey [with sample sizes varying in the range of n = 57–132, almost all students (91.92%), 44.25% female, weighted mean age = 24.45 y]. Samples from Germany (n = 82, students, 54.87% female, mean age = 23.05), Norway (n = 244, mostly nonstudents, 50% female, mean age = 35.04), and the United States (n = 73, nonstudents, 64.38% female, mean age = 35.24) were similar. Table S2 shows demographic information for each sample in our extended database: Overall, 4,344 participants (57.44% female, weighted mean age = 23.73) participated in the main survey.

Questionnaire and procedure.

Questionnaires were administered in the nations’ local languages. (In Kenya, participants could complete either an English or Kiswahili version of the questionnaire.) For questionnaires administered in Middle Eastern countries, the translation/back-translation procedure was used (e.g., ref. 3). Participants in each sample evaluated the groups resulting from their respective preliminary surveys on items reflecting the fundamental dimensions of social perception. [We measured group status and competition (8) plus targeted emotions and behavioral tendencies (25), but these items fall outside the present scope and so are not presented.] More specifically, three items assessed competence (competent, capable, and skilled), five items assessed warmth (warm, friendly, sincere, well-intentioned, and moral), and they were presented intermixed. [Norwegian warmth items were friendly, warm, good-natured, and sincere; competence items were competent, confident, capable, and skillful (32). German warmth items were likeable, warm, and good-natured; competence items were competent, competitive, and independent (33). The recent US study (34) used bipolar items, namely, warm-cold, friendly-unfriendly (warmth), competent-incompetent, and capable-incapable (competence), and scales ranging from 1 to 7. Main survey procedures in Norway, Germany, and the United States were analogous to the procedure illustrated here.] Evaluations were made on five-point scales (1 = not at all to 5 = extremely). As in previous SCM studies, participants were asked to evaluate groups according to the society’s point of view. To this purpose, instructions declared that we were not interested in their personal beliefs, rather in how they think such groups were viewed by the majority of their fellow citizens. This guideline was intended to mitigate social desirability issues, while identifying the culturally shared stereotypes. To avoid participant fatigue, the group list was split into sublists in each sample. “Because results are analyzed primarily at the group level (i.e., each group receives mean ratings, which are then compared with other groups’ mean ratings), randomly assigning different participants to rate different groups and then combining the datasets seemed permissible” (ref. 8, p. 891). A similar procedure was used to collect data in Norway, Germany, and the United States.

SCM Reliability.

Cronbach’s alpha reliability coefficients for each SCM construct were calculated across societal groups in each of the 16 new samples, and proved generally sufficient: warmth α-range: 0.62–0.97 (median = 0.82); competence α-range: 0.61–0.98 (median = 0.80).

GPI and Gini.

Because data were gathered over many years (i.e., 2005–2014), for each country, we matched the year of data collection with the year of the GPI and Gini coefficients for the very same country as closely as possible (Table S4). [GPI reports were downloaded from economicsandpeace.org/reports/ through the years. In April 2016, only GPI reports from 2011 to 2015 were downloadable, but previous reports are available from the authors. Gini coefficients were retrieved from https://www.cia.gov/library/publications/download/index.html and https://www.cia.gov/library/publications/the-world-factbook/fields/2172.html (April 11, 2016).] Because GPI was launched in 2007, 2007 GPI coefficients were held constant for all data samples collected in prior years. Gini coefficients were not available for Afghanistan, Iraq, and Lebanon, accordingly excluding them from the analyses involving the income inequality index, which were therefore run on 46 samples.

Notes on Meta-Regressions on W-C Fisher-Standardized Correlations

i)
Checking for outliers was based on Fisher standardized W-C correlations, with a cutoff of ±3 SD (36). No case lay outside those limits. As an additional test, we used a more conservative criterion as a cutoff (i.e., ±2.5 SD). With this criterion, only Pakistan qualified as an outlier. We then performed the curvilinear meta-regression excluding Pakistan from our database [Q(2) = 5.34, P = 0.07]. Both the linear and curvilinear regression coefficients were significant (β = −0.38, P = 0.034 and β = 0.37, P = 0.039, respectively). Because the higher order variable coefficient prevails in the interpretation of the results, we cannot reject the nonmonotonicity of the conflict–ambivalence relationship. The fact that the negative first-order slope is significant hints at further investigations. To this aim, we ran a graphical local regression using SPSS 23, first considering all samples and then excluding Pakistan. The change in the slope occurs regardless of the exclusion of Pakistan, suggesting that a global first-order regression would not be fully appropriate.
ii)
Before performing inverse-weighted meta-regression, we tested the homogeneity of the effect sizes in the random effect model using the SPSS Macro MeanES (29). Results showed a mean ES equal to 0.42 (Fisher zr), [Q(48) = 637.29, P < 0.001] (i.e., a violation of the homogeneity assumption). The index of heterogeneity I2 was calculated (37):
I2=(Q-(K-1)/Q)100=92.47%,
where K is the number of studies (in this case, samples) included in the meta-analysis. The result suggests that moderation analyses in meta-regression models explain the sample-to-sample variation.
iii)
To corroborate the evidence of a curvilinear relationship between ambivalence and conflict, we applied a local polynomial regression analysis. More specifically, we split the GPI domain into two regions. The abscissa axis was shifted, with the point of minimum as the origin. The meta-regression run on these two distinct regions proved significant (β = −0.36, P = 0.06 and β = 0.45, P = 0.036).
iv)
As mentioned in the main text, confounding variables are common in cross-cultural studies. We checked the robustness of the link between the level of conflict/peace and ambivalent stereotypes by controlling for economic (i.e., GDP per capita) and societal development (HDI) factors, although in our case, these confounds more likely affect the independent variable rather than the dependent one. The GDP and HDI have been found to be highly and negatively correlated with the GPI (23). [GDP data were retrieved from the World Bank (data.worldbank.org/): “GDP per capita is gross domestic product divided by midyear population. GDP is the sum of gross value added by all resident producers in the economy plus any product taxes and minus any subsidies not included in the value of the products. It is calculated without making deductions for depreciation of fabricated assets or for depletion and degradation of natural resources. Data are in current US dollars”]. [As defined by the United Nations Development Program (hdr.undp.org/en/content/human-development-index-hdi): The HDI is a summary measure of average achievement in key dimensions of human development: a long and healthy life, being knowledgeable, and [having] a decent standard of living. The HDI is the geometric mean of normalized indices for each of the three dimensions. The health dimension is assessed by life expectancy at birth, [and] the education dimension is measured by mean years of schooling for adults aged 25 y and more and expected years of schooling for children of school-entering age. The standard of living dimension is measured by gross national income per capita. The HDI uses the logarithm of income to reflect the diminishing importance of income with increasing GNI (gross national income). The scores for the three HDI dimension indices are then aggregated into a composite index using [the] geometric mean.”
As in the case of the GPI and Gini, the GDP per capita that we considered for each sample referred to the same year of samples’ data collection. Centered GPI, centered squared GPI, and GDP were entered in the meta-regression as independent variables, and W-C Fisher-standardized correlations were entered as the response. The model was significant [Q(3) = 7.90, P = 0.048, R2 = 14.01% (vs. 11.7% reported for GPI alone)]. Whereas the regression coefficients for the GPI linear component (β = −0.07, P = 0.79) and GDP (β = 0.23, P = 0.25) were largely nonsignificant, the same coefficient for the GPI quadratic component was marginally significant, (β = 0.33, P = 0.098). It is worth mentioning that the GPI and GDP show a fairly high correlation [r(47) = −0.63, P < 0.001]. Therefore, estimates might suffer from instability, although our results still hold.
Turning to the HDI, for issues of comparability across years and countries, we considered the trend data, based on consistent data and methodology, reported in the 2015 HDI report (statistical annex). Trend data were available for all countries for the following years: 1990, 2000, 2010, 2011, 2012, 2013, and 2014. Thus, we matched the year of samples’ data collection with the HDI historical years as follows: HDI values referring to the year 2000 were considered for samples collected in 2005, whereas HDI values referring to the year 2010 were considered for samples collected between 2007 and 2009. For the remaining samples, the matching year was exact.
Curvilinear meta-regression was run including HDI values in the model, which resulted in a marginal significance [Q(3) = 6.40, P = 0.094, R2 = 11.7%]. The regression coefficients for the GPI linear component (β = −0.28, P = 0.26) and HDI (β = 0.003, P = 0.99) coefficients were nonsignificant, whereas the coefficient for the GPI quadratic component stayed significant (β = 0.45, P = 0.018). The nonmonotonicity of the relationship between conflict and ambivalence held. If the significance of the HDI regression coefficient is almost 1, then such an independent variable should be removed, leading us back to the curvilinear model reported in the paper.
v)
Because we performed an internal meta-analysis (38), with data collected in two waves, we regarded it as appropriate to run an “interim” analysis on the Gini regressor. (The interim analysis is actually post hoc, but assesses robustness across the two stages of data collection.) We used WinDL software (39) (https://www.biostat.wisc.edu/content/lan-demets-method-statistical-programs-clinical-trials), following the step-by-step guide provided in the supplementary material of the study by Lakens (40) (available at https://osf.io/uygrs/files/). The first wave involved 33 samples from the earlier database (11) and kept in the present research; the second wave included 16 new samples. The sequential technique allowed us to estimate a CI of ±2.126, with a monitoring (i.e., before adding samples) adjusted P value of 0.034 vs. a meta-regression P value of 0.043, supporting the overall significance of our results. With two waves only, a computation of confidence boundaries was excluded. On the contrary, an interim analysis was not applicable to the GPI regressor due to its specification into the model for the second wave only. To clarify our methodological choices, we collected as many samples as possible in each wave, in a practical amount of time, and we did not perform intermediate analyses while we were collecting samples.
vi)
As reported in the main text, four samples from the earlier database (11) were not considered for the present research. Two samples (from the United States) were excluded because the gap between the year of data collection (i.e., 2000) and the year of the first GPI coefficient available (i.e., 2007) was too wide, and two samples (Northern Ireland) were excluded for lack of precision (the GPI coefficient refers to the whole UK area, which differs dramatically across areas). However, as a robustness check, meta-regressions were also performed on W-C Fisher-standardized correlations, including in the analyses all samples collected in the previous research (11) (i.e., 37 samples) plus the 16 new samples collected for the present research (i.e., a total of 53 samples). Results are summarized in Table S5.

Acknowledgments

A Department of Justice subcontract supported some data.

Supporting Information

Supporting Information (PDF)
Supporting Information

References

1
S Oishi, Socioecological psychology. Annu Rev Psychol 65, 581–609 (2014).
2
S Oishi, J Graham, Social ecology: Lost and found in psychological science. Perspect Psychol Sci 5, 356–377 (2010).
3
MJ Gelfand, et al., Differences between tight and loose cultures: A 33-nation study. Science 332, 1100–1104 (2011).
4
R de Vries, S Gosling, J Potter, Income inequality and personality: Are less equal U.S. states less agreeable? Soc Sci Med 72, 1978–1985 (2011).
5
EM Uslaner, M Brown, Inequality, trust, and civic engagement. Am Polit Res 33, 868–894 (2005).
6
JF Helliwell, H Huang, How’s your government? International evidence linking good government and well-being. Br J Polit Sci 38, 595–619 (2008).
7
S Kaplan, MG Berman, Directed attention as a common resource for executive functioning and self-regulation. Perspect Psychol Sci 5, 43–57 (2010).
8
ST Fiske, AJ Cuddy, P Glick, J Xu, A model of (often mixed) stereotype content: Competence and warmth respectively follow from perceived status and competition. J Pers Soc Psychol 82, 878–902 (2002).
9
CM Judd, L James-Hawkins, V Yzerbyt, Y Kashima, Fundamental dimensions of social judgment: understanding the relations between judgments of competence and warmth. J Pers Soc Psychol 89, 899–913 (2005).
10
PW Eastwick, et al., Is traditional gender ideology associated with sex-typed mate preferences? A test in nine nations. Sex Roles 54, 603–614 (2006).
11
F Durante, et al., Nations’ income inequality predicts ambivalence in stereotype content: How societies mind the gap. Br J Soc Psychol 52, 726–746 (2013).
12
AJ Cuddy, et al., Stereotype content model across cultures: Towards universal similarities and some differences. Br J Soc Psychol 48, 1–33 (2009).
13
ST Fiske, F Durante, Stereotype content across cultures: Variations on a few themes. Handbook of Advances in Culture and Psychology, eds MJ Gelfand, CY Chiu, YY Hong (Oxford Univ Press, New York) Vol 6, 209–258 (2016).
14
F Durante, C Bearns Tablante, ST Fiske, Poor but warm, rich but cold (and competent): Social classes in the stereotype content model. J Social Issues, 2017).
15
H Bartusevičius, The inequality–conflict nexus re-examined: Income, education and popular rebellions. J Peace Res 51, 35–50 (2014).
16
R MacCulloch, The impact of income on the taste for revolt. Am J Pol Sci 48, 830–848 (2004).
17
EW Nafziger, J Auvinen, Economic development, inequality, war, and state violence. World Dev 30, 153–163 (2002).
18
M Hewstone, K Greenland, Intergroup conflict. Int J Psychol 35, 136–144 (2000).
19
H Tajfel, JC Turner, An integrative theory of intergroup conflict. Social Psychology of Intergroup Relations, eds WG Austin, S Worchel (Brooks/Cole, Monterey, CA), pp. 33–47 (1979).
20
HC Kelman, Social-psychological dimensions of international conflict. Peacemaking in International Conflict: Methods and Techniques, eds IW Zartman, JL Rasmussen (US Institute of Peace, Washington, DC), pp. 191–238 (2007).
21
HC Kelman, A social-psychological approach to conflict analysis and resolution. Handbook of Conflict Analysis and Resolution, eds D Sandole, S Byrne, I Sandole-Staroste, J Senehi (Routledge, New York), pp. 170–183 (2008).
22
U Bronfenbrenner, The mirror image in Soviet-American relations: A social psychologist’s report. J Soc Issues 17, 45–56 (1961).
23
R Fischer, K Hanke, Are societal values linked to global peace and conflict? Peace Conflict 15, 227–248 (2009).
24
JH Ward, Hierarchical grouping to optimize an objective function. J Am Stat Assoc 58, 236–244 (1963).
25
AJC Cuddy, ST Fiske, P Glick, The BIAS map: Behaviors from intergroup affect and stereotypes. J Pers Soc Psychol 92, 631–648 (2007).
26
LV Hedges, I Olkin Statistical Methods for Meta-Analysis (Academic, Orlando, FL, 1985).
27
LV Hedges, JL Vevea, Fixed- and random-effects models in meta-analysis. Psychol Methods 3, 486–504 (1998).
28
MW Lipsey, DB Wilson Practical Meta-Analysis (Sage, Thousand Oaks, CA, 2001).
29
DB Wilson, Meta-analysis macros for SAS, SPSS, and Stata. Available at mason.gmu.edu/∼dwilsonb/ma.html. Accessed September 18, 2015. (2005).
30
HB Bergsieker, LM Leslie, VS Constantine, ST Fiske, Stereotyping by omission: Eliminate the negative, accentuate the positive. J Pers Soc Psychol 102, 1214–1238 (2012).
31
; Institute for Economic & Peace, Global Peace Index. Methodology, results and findings. Available at economicsandpeace.org/. Accessed March 11, 2016. (2008).
32
HH Bye, H Herrebrøden, GJ Hjetland, GØ Røyset, LL Westby, Stereotypes of Norwegian social groups. Scand J Psychol 55, 469–476 (2014).
33
F Asbrock, Stereotypes of social groups in Germany in terms of warmth and competence. Soc Psychol 41, 76–81 (2010).
34
N Kervyn, ST Fiske, VY Yzerbyt, Integrating the stereotype content model (warmth and competence) and the Osgood semantic differential (evaluation, potency, and activity). Eur J Soc Psychol 43, 673–681 (2013).
35
; Institute for Economic & Peace, UK Peace Index. Exploring the fabric of peace in the UK from 2003 and 2012. Available at economicsandpeace.org/. Accessed November 23, 2013. (2013).
36
JW Osborne, A Overbay, The power of outliers (and why researchers should always check for them). Practical Assessment, Research, and Evaluation 9(6). Available at pareonline.net/getvn.asp?v=9&n=6. Accessed August 22, 2016. (2004).
37
JPT Higgins, SG Thompson, Quantifying heterogeneity in a meta-analysis. Stat Med 21, 1539–1558 (2002).
38
T Ueno, GM Fastrich, K Murayama, Meta-analysis to integrate effect sizes within an article: Possible misuse and Type I error inflation. J Exp Psychol Gen 145, 643–654 (2016).
39
DM Reboussin, DL DeMets, KM Kim, KK Lan, Computations for group sequential boundaries using the Lan-DeMets spending function method. Control Clin Trials 21, 190–207 (2000).
40
D Lakens, Performing high-powered studies efficiently with sequential analyses. Eur J Soc Psychol 44, 701–710 (2014).

Information & Authors

Information

Published in

The cover image for PNAS Vol.114; No.4
Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences
Vol. 114 | No. 4
January 24, 2017
PubMed: 28069955

Classifications

Submission history

Published online: January 9, 2017
Published in issue: January 24, 2017

Keywords

  1. stereotypes
  2. peace
  3. conflict
  4. inequality
  5. ambivalence

Acknowledgments

A Department of Justice subcontract supported some data.

Authors

Affiliations

Federica Durante1 [email protected]
Department of Psychology, University of Milano-Bicocca, 20126 Milan, Italy;
Susan T. Fiske1 [email protected]
Department of Psychology and Woodrow Wilson School of Public and International Affairs, Princeton University, Princeton, NJ 08544;
Michele J. Gelfand
Department of Psychology, University of Maryland, College Park, MD 20742;
Franca Crippa
Department of Psychology, University of Milano-Bicocca, 20126 Milan, Italy;
Chiara Suttora
Department of Psychology, University of Milano-Bicocca, 20126 Milan, Italy;
Amelia Stillwell
Stanford University Graduate School of Business, Stanford University, Stanford, CA 94305;
Frank Asbrock
Department of Psychology, Chemnitz University of Technology, 09107 Chemnitz, Germany;
Zeynep Aycan
Department of Psychology & Faculty of Management, Koc University, 34450 Istanbul, Turkey;
Hege H. Bye
Department of Psychosocial Science, Faculty of Psychology, University of Bergen, N-5020 Bergen, Norway;
Rickard Carlsson
Department of Psychology, Linnaeus University, 391 82 Kalmar, Sweden;
Fredrik Björklund
Department of Psychology, Lund University, 221 00 Lund, Sweden;
Munqith Dagher
Independent Institute for Administration and Civil Society Studies (IACSS), Amman 11941, Jordan;
Armando Geller
Scensei, 8032 Zurich, Switzerland;
Christian Albrekt Larsen
Centre for Comparative Welfare Studies, Department of Political Science, Aalborg University, 9220 Aalborg, Denmark;
Abdel-Hamid Abdel Latif
The Egyptian Research and Training Center, 11341 Cairo, Egypt;
Tuuli Anna Mähönen
Open University, University of Helsinki, FI-00014 Helskini, Finland;
Inga Jasinskaja-Lahti
Open University, University of Helsinki, FI-00014 Helskini, Finland;
Ali Teymoori
School of Psychology, Bordeaux Population Health Research Centre (Inserm U1219), University of Bordeaux, 33000 Bourdeaux, France

Notes

1
To whom correspondence may be addressed. Email: [email protected] or [email protected].
Author contributions: F.D., S.T.F., and M.J.G. designed research; F.A., Z.A., H.H.B., R.C., F.B., M.D., A.G., C.A.L., A.-H.A.L., T.A.M., I.J.-L., and A.T. performed research; F.D., F.C., C.S., and A.S. analyzed data; F.D., M.J.G., and A.S. managed new cross-national data collection; F.A., Z.A., H.H.B., R.C., F.B., M.D., A.G., C.A.L., A.-H.A.L., T.A.M., I.J.-L., and A.T. added new data and local expertise; and F.D. and S.T.F. wrote the paper.
Reviewers: C.J., University of Colorado Boulder; and S.O., University of Virginia.

Competing Interests

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

Metrics & Citations

Metrics

Note: The article usage is presented with a three- to four-day delay and will update daily once available. Due to ths delay, usage data will not appear immediately following publication. Citation information is sourced from Crossref Cited-by service.


Altmetrics




Citations

Export the article citation data by selecting a format from the list below and clicking Export.

Cited by

    Loading...

    View Options

    View options

    PDF format

    Download this article as a PDF file

    DOWNLOAD PDF

    Login options

    Check if you have access through your login credentials or your institution to get full access on this article.

    Personal login Institutional Login

    Recommend to a librarian

    Recommend PNAS to a Librarian

    Purchase options

    Purchase this article to access the full text.

    Single Article Purchase

    Ambivalent stereotypes link to peace, conflict, and inequality across 38 nations
    Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences
    • Vol. 114
    • No. 4
    • pp. 611-E655

    Figures

    Tables

    Media

    Share

    Share

    Share article link

    Share on social media