Individuals, institutions, and innovation in the debates of the French Revolution

Edited by Danielle S. Bassett, University of Pennsylvania, Philadelphia, PA, and accepted by Editorial Board Member Michael S. Gazzaniga March 19, 2018 (received for review October 9, 2017)
April 17, 2018
115 (18) 4607-4612
PNAS Updates
2018 Cozzarelli Prize recipients
Science Sessions podcast
Rhetoric of the French Revolution

Significance

How do democracies make decisions? We can read transcripts from parliament houses and legislative halls to see how particular ideas are introduced and debated, but we understand very little about the general principles of how these systems deal with information, or the origins of those principles. Here we study the parliamentary assembly of the first 2 years of the French Revolution, a model for democracies and revolutions across the globe, and show how patterns of speaking are created, picked up, and ignored or propagated. Political ideology, top–down rules, and individual charisma all affect how word patterns survive and thrive or, conversely, disappear and drop away.

Abstract

The French Revolution brought principles of “liberty, equality, fraternity” to bear on the day-to-day challenges of governing what was then the largest country in Europe. Its experiments provided a model for future revolutions and democracies across the globe, but this first modern revolution had no model to follow. Using reconstructed transcripts of debates held in the Revolution’s first parliament, we present a quantitative analysis of how this body managed innovation. We use information theory to track the creation, transmission, and destruction of word-use patterns across over 40,000 speeches and a thousand speakers. The parliament as a whole was biased toward the adoption of new patterns, but speakers’ individual qualities could break these overall trends. Speakers on the left innovated at higher rates, while speakers on the right acted to preserve prior patterns. Key players such as Robespierre (on the left) and Abbé Maury (on the right) played information-processing roles emblematic of their politics. Newly created organizational functions—such as the Assembly president and committee chairs—had significant effects on debate outcomes, and a distinct transition appears midway through the parliament when committees, external to the debate process, gained new powers to “propose and dispose.” Taken together, these quantitative results align with existing qualitative interpretations, but also reveal crucial information-processing dynamics that have hitherto been overlooked. Great orators had the public’s attention, but deputies (mostly on the political left) who mastered the committee system gained new powers to shape revolutionary legislation.
The French Revolution was a turning point in European history. Revolutionary commitments to individual liberty collided with ideals of social equality, while the rejection of Divine-Right monarchy and the embrace of laws based on reason opened a host of practical questions about how to govern the most populous state in Europe. The first parliament of the Revolution, the National Constituent Assembly (NCA), was a picture of upheaval from its outset.
Over the course of 2 years, the thousand or more individuals in that Assembly took it upon themselves to propose and argue the previously unimaginable: the revocation of Old-Regime privilege and the reinvention of the relationship between individual and state. But this parliament was more than a debate society for ambitious young men. It was also the origin of a system of rule. In the years that followed, successive legislative bodies declared war on most of Europe, dissolved the French monarchy, declared a Republic, and sentenced the former king to death—all while simultaneously writing constitutions and passing ordinary legislation. Many of their procedures and some of their personnel were drawn from the experience of the NCA.
As a parliament, the NCA confronted the problems that come with managing massive flows of information—problems still faced by the modern deliberative political bodies that, in many cases, are its direct descendants (1). But as the first parliament, the body itself had little precedent to follow. Its members therefore faced a double challenge: how to convey points in a way familiar enough to be intelligible by others, while nonetheless making claims that were in many cases substantially novel (“revolutionary,” even). The NCA was a site, therefore, of both epistemic and political innovation. Conceiving it as such suggests two sets of questions. First, how did new ideas enter that parliament room; how were they adopted, adapted, or discarded by the men who heard them? Second: What institutions did the parliament evolve to manage the onslaught of novelty and reaction, optimism and grievance, philosophical argument and organizational minutiae that characterized the day-to-day tasks of governance and nation-building?
The digitization of historical archives allows us to answer these questions in a fundamentally new way. Using latent Dirichlet allocation (2) and new techniques in information theory drawn from the cognitive sciences (3), we track the emergence and persistence of word-use patterns in over 40,000 speeches made in the NCA and later reconstructed in the Archives Parliamentaires (AP) from detailed records kept at the time. Two critical measures—novelty (how unexpected a speech’s patterns are, given past speeches) and transience (the extent to which those patterns fade or persist in future speeches)—allow us to trace both new manners of speech and the emergence of new institutions. Our mapping of the French Revolution’s turbulent early days in terms of the creation, sharing, and destruction of word-use patterns complements existing studies of specific ideas (much as evolutionary biology analyzes both mechanisms of transmission/selection and the particular phenotypes for which an environment selects).
We find, at high significance, a bias in favor of the propagation of novel patterns. In the framework of cultural evolution, the flow of ideas through NCA is out of equilibrium: the system reveals itself as having preferentially selected for what violated prior expectations. This effect was driven in part by charismatic political radicals such as Robespierre and Pétion de Villeneuve, who not only introduced new patterns more often than their peers but did so in a way such that others followed. By contrast, influential conservative figures such as Abbé Maury and Cazalès acted as inertial dampeners: their speeches maintained past patterns and carried them forward, despite the Assembly’s overall bias toward innovation. Conservatives of the French Revolution “conserved”: not only did they refer to past traditions, but they did so with familiar discursive strategies and inherited word-use patterns.
In parallel with these individual-level differences, our methods reveal a major transition in how the parliament as a whole processed novelty. Roughly halfway through the NCA’s existence, committees—which met outside the parliament but reported to it—gained new power to raise and resolve questions. Orators on the left and right continued to confront each other in public speeches from the floor, but those on the left also captured this new institutional mechanism and used it to their own advantage. The consolidation of this structural shift was accompanied by radicalization of the left and an accelerating flight of conservatives from both the parliament and the country itself.

Results

Social systems are characterized by heteroglossia: the coexistence, sharing, and competition of different patterns of speech. Heteroglossia makes linguistics and rhetoric (the reception, influence, and propagation of language within a community) (4, 5) core components in the quantitative study of culture. Tracking changes in speech patterns within a social body allows us to examine cultural evolution: the circulation, selection, and differential propagation of speech patterns in the group as a whole (rather than, say, tracking the ideas of a single individual). Patterns of heteroglossia demonstrate existing power relations, create new ones, and are a key method for the definition of both institutions and genres (68). Our methods here quantify a key aspect of cultural evolution: the extent to which one agent’s language patterns are used and copied by another (9, 10). To study the flow of rhetorical influence and attention in the NCA over time, we characterize how patterns of language use, uncovered by topic modeling, are propagated from speech to speech. We do so using Kullback–Leibler Divergence (KLD) (11): KLD, or “surprise,” measures the extent to which the expectations of an optimal learner, trained on one pattern, are violated by later patterns. Other work has demonstrated that surprise (in the Kullback–Leibler sense) is a cognitive as well as an information-theoretic quantity. It predicts what a subject will look at in a dynamically evolving visual scene (12) and can be used to map an individual’s higher level activities (detecting, for example, biographically significant transitions in a subject’s intellectual life) (3). Methodologically, this paper extends that work by considering surprise in relation to both past and future.
We use surprise to analyze a corpus of speeches by many different individuals. Surprise here measures both the deviation of one speech from the patterns of prior ones (novelty) and from patterns that appear in the future (transience). High surprise compared with the past indicates the topic mixture is new compared with previous speeches, hence the term “novelty”; high surprise compared with the future indicates that later speeches do not retain that pattern very strongly, hence the term “transience”. We provide a detailed introduction to these methods in SI Appendix.
Novelty and transience track a number of different effects. In addition to capturing intuitive notions of influence—a speaker with high novelty and low transience may have successfully shifted the terms of a discussion—they also track strategic effects (speakers who angle to speak early in a debate may have higher novelty) and external common causes (the first speech after a major event outside the chamber will have higher novelty). We test for a number of these latter effects (see SI Appendix), through day-level fixed effects and debate-level position analysis, to which our results below are robust. Our methods track the replications of patterns of speech and subject matter, rather than (for example) agreement: A speaker may introduce a new subject (high novelty) that is discussed by others (low transience), only to have his position contradicted or rejected.

Innovation Bias.

Speeches in the NCA span a wide range in both novelty and transience; Fig. 1 summarizes the system at the level of individual speeches, in this case at the relatively rapid time scale (window width, w) of seven speeches. While the majority of speeches concentrate near the symmetry line—speeches with high novelty are likely to have similarly high transience—two results stand out. First, the scatter is large: many speeches lie far off the novelty–transience line of equality, and it is easy (for example) to find speeches with top-quartile novelty that have bottom-quartile transience. “What is new is quickly forgotten” is a useful heuristic but holds only in the average. Below, we consider two potential drivers of these diverse receptions: speaker and context.
Fig. 1.
Novelty, transience, and resonance in the French Revolution. (Left) A density plot of transience vs. novelty per speech at scale w=7. Resonant speeches, with low transience compared with their novelty, fall below the identity (x=y) line. Resonant speeches at any time j are more surprising compared with preceding speeches (time jd, 1dw) than successors (time j+d). This temporal asymmetry can be seen in the center plot of surprise for speech delay d surrounding highly resonant speeches from the selection at Left. (Right) Resonance vs. novelty, with regression line. Although novelty is tied to transience, it is also necessary to achieve resonance.
Second, novel speeches were unexpectedly influential. We quantify this with “resonance,” the imbalance between novelty and transience (see Materials and Methods). Resonance, the quality of at once differing from the past and leaving traces on the future, increases with novelty, as shown in the rightmost plot of Fig. 1. We refer to this positive relationship as innovation bias: penalties to high novelty speeches are lower than expected in a system at equilibrium. This bias is measured by Γ, the slope of the novelty–resonance line; positive Γ indicates innovation bias. We find this bias in place from the most rapid time scales (one speech to the next, w=1), to w1000, those on the order of days (see SI Appendix). This innovation bias lasts for at least the course of a day, as speakers deliberately turn to new topics, but fades away on longer time scales.

Organizational Roles and Individual Strategies.

In choosing when to speak and what to say, speakers had some control over the relative novelty of their speeches. A speaker’s attitude toward the Assembly as an institution, like other political or philosophical commitments, would contribute to his willingness to create new word patterns or copy earlier speakers. Conversely, speakers had much less control over the reception of what they said. Idiosyncratic properties of the speaker, ranging from the political (e.g., faction membership) and the social (e.g., demeanor or prestige) to the rhetorical (everything from word choice to pitch and volume of speech) could have altered the reception of their words.
Patterns at the level of the individual could, of course, temporarily break the system-level trend that favored innovation. An unskilled but ambitious deputy might have tried to introduce new ideas that his colleagues tended to dismiss, registering in our data as high novelty with low, or even negative, resonance. Conversely, the prestige or social power of another deputy might have meant he combined low novelty with high resonance, thereby keeping the conversation on track.
Decisions at the individual level cannot account for all deviations from system-level patterns, however. From its first days, the Assembly organized itself in such a way as to assign explicit roles to particular speakers. These information-processing functions overruled an individual’s personal characteristics. An example from the NCA, still common to many parliaments today, is the role of president, who served as point of contact for the King and enforcer of the daily agenda (13). The president’s role—one in which only 49 of the NCA’s more than a thousand members ever served—was largely functional and organizational.
The NCA also created another specialized entity: the committee. Committees, whose members were notionally selected on the basis of expertise, deliberated in private. They developed content outside the debate process and then presented it to the full body for public review. While a speaker on the Assembly floor might play to the audience in the visitors’ galleries, committee members addressed only each other.
Lexical markers in our data identify when a committee proxy was speaking and allow us to classify his speech into two categories: “new-item” speech and “in-debate” committee speech (see SI Appendix). New-item speeches introduced official content to the Assembly floor (typically draft legislation to be debated by the body) and mark transitions in attention from one topic to another. In-debate speeches occurred when a committee member or spokesman engaged with other delegates following the item’s introduction.
To understand how individual-level differences and system-imposed roles affected the production and reception of new patterns, we consider the novelty and resonance of speeches given by those in three distinct Assembly roles: the 40 most common orators, the President (regardless of who held the position), and committee proxies. We calculate the average novelty and resonance for each category (scaled by z score), z(N) and z(R), identifying both potentially idiosyncratic speech-pattern innovations and their reception by the system as a whole, at scales of w from 1 (one speech compared with the next, and prior, speech) to 5,000 (one speech compared with 3 mo of speeches before or after).
The system’s overall bias in favor of innovation would predict that the category of speakers with the highest novelty would also have the highest resonance. To determine if this expectation is validated in our sources, we compare the overall novelty–resonance relationship of the system (depicted by the fit line in Fig. 1) to the measured resonance for speeches from each of the three categories (orator, President, committee proxy). Specifically, for each category we report Δz(R), defined as z(R)𝐄[z(R)|z(N)], the difference between the measured mean resonance of speeches and the expected mean resonance under the OLS model z(R)z(N). While z(R) measures the effect speakers had on later discourse, Δz(R) measures the extent to which they broke system-level trends in achieving those effects. For example, a speaker with high novelty may have high resonance but negative Δz(R), indicating that his adventurousness was rewarded less than expected. Full results for w=36 (roughly half a day) are shown in Table 1; results for novelty are stable on all time scales, while resonance at w=36 is strongly correlated from w=3 to w=100 (see SI Appendix).
Table 1.
Mean novelty and resonance by speaker at scale 36, for role and type (in bold) and the top 40 orators
Namez(N)z(R)Δz(R)Type
High novelty, high resonance    
Jérôme Pétion de Villeneuve0.100.28***+0.25***3g
Maximilien Robespierre0.110.18**+0.14*3g
Jean-Denis Lanjuinais0.060.16***+0.15**3g
Alexandre Lameth0.17*0.14+0.092g
Charles Antoine Chasset0.31***0.13+0.043g
Committee (new item)1.31***0.12***-0.27***
Philippe-Antoine Merlin0.27***0.05-0.033g
Pierre-François Gossin0.65***0.03-0.17*3g
Jacques François Menou0.40***0.02-0.102g
Committee (in debate)0.29***0.02-0.07***
Left wing0.07***0.02*0.00(g)
3rd estate0.06***0.01-0.02*
High novelty, low resonance    
Jacques Guillaume Thouret0.16**0.00-0.053g
Jacques-Joseph Defermon0.35***-0.03-0.13*3-
François Denis Tronchet0.24***-0.04-0.11*3g
Armand-Gaston Camus0.29***-0.04-0.13***3g
President0.02-0.07***-0.08***
Théodore Vernier0.55***-0.14-0.31***3g
Low novelty, high resonance    
Guillaume Goupil-Préfelne-0.21***0.13+0.20***3g
Jean-François Reubell-0.18***0.11+0.16**3g
Jacques Antoine de Cazalès-0.44***0.08+0.21***2d
Pierre Victor Malouet-0.27***0.08+0.16***3d
Jean-Siffrein Maury-0.46***0.07+0.20***1d
Pierre-Louis Prieur-0.27***0.05+0.13**3g
1st and 2nd estates-0.10***0.03***+0.05***
Jean-François Gaultier de-0.13*0.03+0.063g
Biauzat    
Right wing-0.32***0.03*+0.10***(d)
Low novelty, low resonance    
Antoine de Folleville-0.44***-0.01+0.122d
Michel Le Peletier de-0.20***-0.01+0.052g
Saint-Fargeau    
François-Dominique de-0.61***-0.02+0.17*2d
Montlosier    
Louis Foucauld de Lardimalie-0.53***-0.05+0.112d
Charles Lameth-0.15*-0.06-0.022g
Pierre François Bouche-0.09*-0.10-0.073g
Antoine Barnave-0.04-0.12**-0.113g
Bolded categories include all speeches by speakers who match either the type (estate or political affiliation; based on ref. 14), or role (committee or president; defined in text). z(N): novelty compared with system average; z(R): resonance compared with system average; Δz(R): resonance relative to predicted resonance given novelty. “Type” codes for estate (3: bourgeoisie; 2: nobility; 1: clergy) and political affiliation (g: gauche, left-; d: droit, right-wing). p values corrected for multiple comparisons using Holm–Bonferroni (15).
Institutional roles—speaking on behalf of a committee, presiding over the Assembly—did not follow these system-level trends. High novelty and high resonance together characterize committees as gatherers of new information that they injected into debate in ways that defined downstream discussion. In contrast, the president’s role as agenda enforcer led to lower than average resonance: he acted, at best, to summarize what had come before, while having less influence on patterns of speech that followed. Though he might break from conversation to further the agenda, the content he introduced tended not to persist. The overall novelty bias cannot be explained by the taking-in of new information from committees: while committees show above average resonance (z(R) greater than zero), they have lower resonance than expected given their novelty (Δz(R) less than zero).
The data similarly show individual orators departing from system-wide trends. Of the top 40 speakers in the assembly, 27 show significant deviations from aggregate patterns in either novelty or resonance at at least the p<0.05 level, with 22 speakers showing deviations at p<103. Speakers deviate in both directions, with some showing anomalously high tendencies to break with past patterns and others showing similarly strong tendencies to preserve them. High-novelty speakers are overwhelmingly associated with the left wing and the bourgeoisie, while all of our right-wing speakers, and the vast majority of nobility, are low-novelty. The order in which speakers joined a debate appears to be driving some, but not all, of these effects (SI Appendix).
While more than half of the top 40 orators show surprisingly high or low novelty, fewer are distinguished by their resonance. The latter were, however, among the key players of the Revolution. The celebrated radicals Robespierre and Pétion achieved not only the highest resonance but also significantly higher resonance than even that due to the system-wide novelty bias [positive Δz(R)]. In contrast, speakers such as Armand-Gaston Camus and Théodore Vernier, called on primarily for their specialized knowledge in canon law and finances, show high novelty but low resonance: they presented information that either failed to make an impact or (more likely) settled questions so conclusively that the room moved on to completely different discussions. Finally, prominent political conservatives such as Jean-Siffrein Maury and Jacques de Cazalès appear in the low-novelty, high-resonance quadrant. They break the system-level novelty bias and are notable not only for keeping the conversation on track (low novelty) but for speaking in ways that persist forward (high resonance). In this, Maury and Cazalès are characteristic of the right-wing overall: while the novelty-biased left was composed of both high- and low-resonance speakers, right-wing speech patterns persist, with positive z(R) and Δz(R) despite their anomalously low novelty.

The Emergence and Evolution of the Committee.

Committees were a key NCA innovation, allowing the system to manage vast amounts of information without overwhelming legislative debate. Committees in the NCA did not appear overnight. Our previous section establishes their unusual functional role, but the AP’s comprehensive coverage allows study of their role’s emergence as well. In this section, we show how returns to novelty, Γ, were modulated by committee roles over time. We fit, separately, two terms that quantify the additional boost (or decrement) to the novelty–resonance relationship when speeches either introduce new committee items (Γn) or advocate on behalf of a committee during debate (Γd). A speech of novelty N, for example, achieves on average a resonance R equal to (Γ+Γn)(NN0) when made by a committee member introducing a new item, compared with Γ(NN0) when the speaker acts on his own behalf.
We look for discrete shifts in committee function, doing change-point detection with a maximum-likelihood model of the novelty–resonance relationship where Γ, Γn, and Γd are allowed to vary in time. Following ref. 3, we consider a two-epoch model, where all three quantities are fixed to constant values in each epoch, with a single discrete change at a particular time point whose position is a free parameter. The two-epoch model is preferred to a single-epoch model, as well as to a linear (secular shift) model under AIC. Our maximum likelihood change-point in the nature of committee functions occurs in late 1790; the modal best fit date across all scales is October 31, 1790. Allowing the intercepts of the new-item and in-debate speeches, as well as their slopes, to vary produces nearly identical results. A separate frequentist analysis rejects a randomly ordered null model at p<102 (see SI Appendix).
Fig. 2 displays the novelty–transience relationships for the two epochs, demonstrating the new role of committee speech over time. In the first epoch, the resonance of new items was indistinguishable from speeches of similar novelty in the system as a whole. However, speech received a resonance bonus when delivered by a committee member as part of a debate. In other words, committee representatives injected new information in a fashion similar to other delegates (new-item speech) but had privileged abilities in guiding the subsequent debate (in-debate speech).
Fig. 2.
Information-processing functions of NCA committees before (first column) and after (second column) the late-1790 change-point. (Top) The shift in the novelty–resonance relationship for new-item and in-debate committee speech, with 99% confidence intervals. (Bottom) Scatter plots and fit lines at scale 27 for these speech types, compared with all other speeches. The “undebated tail” appears in the second epoch as a new cloud of green points along the dotted line, generated by committees with new powers to propose and dispose.
The pattern is different in the second epoch, where new-item speech has anomalously low resonance at high novelty. Inspection of the speeches themselves suggests that this high-novelty/low-resonance tail is associated not with (as in the case of individual speakers) failure to alter the course of debate but rather with the increasing power committees had to “propose and dispose”: once the committee presented their findings to the parliament, they were increasingly accepted with minimal discussion. In some cases, committee reports are noted as “passed without debate”; for a sample of these cases, we find the mean z(N) equal to 1.57±0.16 and z(R) equal to 0.14±0.13—that is, committee speeches with these hand-annotated outcomes follow a high-novelty, low-resonance pattern similar to other new-item speeches, further validating the interpretation of the undebated tail as a signal of emergent committee power. This power can also be seen when substantive debate on committee matters does occur: “in debate” speeches made by committee members retain a privileged role in fixing and propagating the patterns that define that debate.

Discussion

The turbulent months at the beginning of the French Revolution led to a durable transformation in the very nature of European government (16). Our study of the elite debates at the center of these events focuses on pattern transmission as a proxy for the differential propagation of the ideas these patterns may communicate. This provides an alternate perspective to qualitative analyses, which usually focus on analyzing the logic of particular ideas and arguments over time (17, 18).
Our analysis reveals clear differences between how the left- and right-wing figures created and transmitted patterns of language use. Conservatives may indeed “stand athwart History, yelling Stop” (19); our results show they did so here less by redirecting conversation from one set of patterns to another than by maintaining the already established patterns of the conversations in which they participated. Indeed, the spatial metaphor of left and right is itself misleading: from the point of view of the debates themselves, the right wing appears as an inertial center, holding off conversation drift, while left-wing speakers produce a wide spectrum of innovations on a much larger periphery, only some of which survive. These roles become visible without reference to the ideas that are propagated: Robespierre emerges as a high-novelty, high-resonance speaker even before we consider the content of the speeches that made him an icon of revolutionary politics.
We also find an emergent distinction between orators and institutional players. Talented speakers like Robespierre, Pétion, and Maury could achieve power through rhetoric and debate, appealing to the crowds in the galleries as much as their colleagues. Yet not every ambitious delegate could take this road. Quite apart from a delegate’s rhetorical capacities or political stature, the physical venue itself was demanding: loud and (literally) resonant voices were required to be heard. While the NCA provided a place for rhetorical masters to thrive, in other words, it also ended up producing a second class of delegate who concentrated on committee work. Deliberating outside the parliament room, committees contributed specialized knowledge and abilities and became indispensable workhorses for the emerging government (13). The information-processing functions that committees took on distinguish them clearly from other forms of speech, while Fig. 2 shows how these functions emerged over time; once in place, committees were both strong sources of new patterns of speech and new sources of extrarhetorical power. Both individuals and institutions, in other words, mattered, playing distinct roles in the development of the parliament over time.
Many delegates welcomed these extraparliamentary functions: committees, wrote the assembly member Jacques Dinochau, “regulate the order of debate, classify questions, and maintain a continuity of principles, thus preventing an incoherence which might otherwise have menaced our decrees” (13). Yet the private nature of these committees was in dramatic contrast to the public debates in the hall itself—their increasing influence a testimony to the emergent distinction between the spectacle of democracy and the actual ways it functioned in a body and polity too large for direct participation. The dramatic and early appearance of a specialized information-processing role for committees foreshadows their appearance in modern democracies, where they emerge endogenously (20) to serve as essential information-management systems (21).
The power of these committees continued to grow. While early committees were devoted to technical matters such as monetary and fiscal policy, they were also instrumental to key developments such as the dismantling of feudal and religious privileges. When the revolution collapsed into chaos in 1793, it was committees, such as the famous “Committee of Public Safety” with Robespierre at its head, that effectively became the republican government.

Conclusion

The history of human culture is more than just the rise and fall of particular ideas. It is also the emergence of new information-processing mechanisms and media, and roles that individuals and institutions play in creating and propagating these ideas through time. In the language of biological evolution, we must understand not only the characteristics for which an environment selects but the strength of that selection over time and the shifting and heterogeneous nature of the transmission mechanisms.
By quantifying the flow of word patterns between times and speakers, we see not just the traces of a conscious battle of ideas but also—more important and more clearly—the contours of a new rhetorical space. This latter was neither intentionally produced, nor was it the exclusive property of any one political group. New word patterns from the left resonated, but so too did old ones from the right. Political actors did not just take different ideological positions, but played different roles in the propagation of patterns. And together, in both cooperation and competition, they invented new mechanisms for the collective management of information.

Materials and Methods

The AP is the definitive source for parliamentary transcripts of the Revolution, reconstructed from primary sources including transcripts, minutes, and newspaper reports. The French Revolution Digital Archive (https://frda.stanford.edu) is a digital version, with full-text speeches and text encoding initiative markup from the beginning of the NCA in July 1789 to its end in September 1791. After stop-word removal, speeches are represented as count vectors over a vocabulary of 10,000 most common words. The resulting corpus contains 4,765,773 words in 44,913 speeches. Each speech is matched to a speaker and may be tagged with a “role”: as a speech made by a delegate acting as the assembly president or on behalf of a committee (to introduce a report or comment on it). See SI Appendix for details on corpus preparation. All of the delegates to the Assembly, and all of the speakers in our data, were male.
We use latent Dirichlet allocation (LDA) (2) to quantify the semantic content of speeches. LDA categorizes speeches by identifying co-occurring word patterns (“topics”); any particular speech is a weighted combination of these topics. Many map remarkably well to the semantics of the speeches themselves. In addition to identifying subject matter content (topics in the ordinary sense, such as discussion of the church, finance, or corruption), they are also sensitive to rhetorical moves (e.g., logical arguments vs. appeals to patriotism). The SI Appendix presents interpretations of the LDA topics and a close reading of speeches from a particular debate. LDA allows for a variable number of topics, corresponding to an effective resolution; we use K=100 topics.
Having decomposed speeches into topics, we can then track the ways in which these topic combinations deviate from those in previous speeches (novelty) and are discarded (or not) by speeches that follow (transience).

Novelty, Transience, Resonance.

Novelty at the smallest time scales (a speech compared with the one just previous) is measured by the KLD of the jth speech, s(j), relative to the previous speech, s(j1).
Averaging this measure further backward in the debate allows us to see longer trends beyond the back-and-forth of a single exchange,
KLD(s(j)|s(j1))=i=1Ksi(j)log2(si(j)si(j1)).
[1]
to show the extent to which the current speaker has introduced new patterns to the debate given, say, the last 10 speeches. We refer to this quantity as novelty N at time j on scale w,
Nw(j)=1wd=1wKLD(s(j)|s(jd)),
[2]
Any speech can break abruptly from its past, but the new patterns it introduces may not persist. Consider an interjection that other speakers ignore to return to the matter at hand. It would be surprising given the past but equally surprising in comparison with the future. In contrast, a rhetorically effective interjection would move the conversation in a new direction (that of its own rhetoric). This shift would appear as a surprise asymmetry around the interjection. We define this asymmetry as resonance, R:
Rw(j)=1wd=1w[KLD(s(j)|s(jd))KLD(s(j)|s(j+d))]Nw(j)Tw(j).
[3]
Resonance is novelty minus transience, T, where the latter is novelty in Eq. 2 under time reversal. Novel speeches, which also influence future discourse, are pivot points in conversation. Novelty’s effectiveness, Γ, is the rate at which resonance increases with novelty, d𝐄[R|N]dN, approximated with a linear model.

Acknowledgments

J.H. acknowledges Research Experience for Undergraduates National Science Foundation Grant ACI-1358567 at the Santa Fe Institute.

Supporting Information

Appendix (PDF)

References

1
BD Jones, FR Baumgartner The Politics of Attention: How Government Prioritizes Problems (Univ of Chicago Press, Chicago, 2005).
2
DM Blei, AY Ng, MI Jordan, Latent Dirichlet allocation. J Mach Learn Res (JMLR) 3, 993–1022 (2003).
3
J Murdock, C Allen, S DeDeo, Exploration and exploitation of Victorian science in Darwin’s reading notebooks. Cognition 159, 117–126 (2017).
4
WL Benoit, MJ Smythe, Rhetorical theory as message reception: A cognitive response approach to rhetorical theory and criticism. Commun Stud 54, 96–114 (2003).
5
RA Harris, Reception studies in the rhetoric of science. Tech Commun Q 14, 249–255 (2005).
6
M Foucault Power/Knowledge: Selected Interviews and Other Writings, 1972–1977 (Pantheon, New York, 1980).
7
C Danescu-Niculescu-Mizil, L Lee, B Pang, J Kleinberg, Echoes of power: Language effects and power differences in social interaction. Proceedings of the 21st International Conference on World Wide Web (Association of Computing Machinery, Lyon, France), pp. 699–708 (2012).
8
S Klingenstein, T Hitchcock, S DeDeo, The civilizing process in London’s Old Bailey. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 111, 9419–9424 (2014).
9
MM Bakhtin The Dialogic Imagination (Univ of Texas Press, Austin, TX, 2010).
10
RA Blythe, W Croft, S-curves and the mechanisms of propagation in language change. Language 88, 269–304 (2012).
11
S Kullback, R Leibler, On information and sufficiency. Ann Math Stat 22, 79–86 (1951).
12
L Itti, P Baldi, Bayesian surprise attracts human attention. Vis Res 49, 1295–1306 (2009).
13
T Tackett Becoming a Revolutionary (Princeton Univ Press, Princeton, NJ, 2014).
14
EH Lemay, C Favre-Lejeune, Y Fauchois, A Patrick Dictionnaire des constituants: 1789–1791 (Universitas Press, Paris) Vol 2, 996–997 (1991).
15
S Holm, A simple sequentially rejective multiple test procedure. Scand J Stat 6, 65–70 (1979).
16
WH Sewell, Historical events as transformations of structures: Inventing revolution at the Bastille. Theor Soc 25, 841–881 (1996).
17
KM Baker Inventing the French Revolution: Essays on French Political Culture in the Eighteenth Century (Cambridge Univ Press, Cambridge, UK, 1990).
18
KM Baker, D Edelstein, Scripting Revolution: A Historical Approach to the Comparative Study of Revolutions. (Stanford Univ Press, Stanford, CA). (2015).
19
Jr WF Buckley, Our mission statement. Natl Rev 1, 1 (1955).
20
DP Baron, Legislative organization with informational committees. Am J Polit Sci 44, 485–505 (2000).
21
KA Shepsle, BR Weingast, Positive theories of Congressional institutions. Legislative Stud Q 19, 149–179 (1994).

Information & Authors

Information

Published in

The cover image for PNAS Vol.115; No.18
Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences
Vol. 115 | No. 18
May 1, 2018
PubMed: 29666239

Classifications

Submission history

Published online: April 17, 2018
Published in issue: May 1, 2018

Keywords

  1. cultural evolution
  2. political science
  3. cognitive science
  4. computational social science
  5. digital history

Acknowledgments

J.H. acknowledges Research Experience for Undergraduates National Science Foundation Grant ACI-1358567 at the Santa Fe Institute.

Notes

This article is a PNAS Direct Submission. D.S.B. is a guest editor invited by the Editorial Board.

Authors

Affiliations

Alexander T. J. Barron
School of Informatics, Computing, and Engineering, Indiana University, Bloomington, IN 47408;
Jenny Huang
School of Informatics, Computing, and Engineering, Indiana University, Bloomington, IN 47408;
Santa Fe Institute, Santa Fe, NM 87501;
Rebecca L. Spang
Department of History, College of Arts and Sciences, Indiana University, Bloomington, IN 47405;
Santa Fe Institute, Santa Fe, NM 87501;
Department of Social and Decision Sciences, Dietrich College, Carnegie Mellon University, Pittsburgh, PA 15213

Notes

1
To whom correspondence should be addressed. Email: [email protected].
Author contributions: A.T.J.B., R.L.S., and S.D. designed research; A.T.J.B., J.H., R.L.S., and S.D. performed research; A.T.J.B., J.H., R.L.S., and S.D. contributed new reagents/analytic tools; A.T.J.B., J.H., R.L.S., and S.D. analyzed data; and A.T.J.B., J.H., R.L.S., and S.D. wrote the paper.

Competing Interests

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

Metrics & Citations

Metrics

Note: The article usage is presented with a three- to four-day delay and will update daily once available. Due to ths delay, usage data will not appear immediately following publication. Citation information is sourced from Crossref Cited-by service.


Citation statements




Altmetrics

Citations

Export the article citation data by selecting a format from the list below and clicking Export.

Cited by

    Loading...

    View Options

    View options

    PDF format

    Download this article as a PDF file

    DOWNLOAD PDF

    Login options

    Check if you have access through your login credentials or your institution to get full access on this article.

    Personal login Institutional Login

    Recommend to a librarian

    Recommend PNAS to a Librarian

    Purchase options

    Purchase this article to access the full text.

    Single Article Purchase

    Individuals, institutions, and innovation in the debates of the French Revolution
    Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences
    • Vol. 115
    • No. 18
    • pp. 4519-E4313

    Figures

    Tables

    Media

    Share

    Share

    Share article link

    Share on social media