Hominin brain size increase has emerged from within-species encephalization
Edited by G.Philip Rightmire, Harvard University, Cambridge, MA; received May 13, 2024; accepted October 11, 2024 by Editorial Board Member Richard G. Klein
Significance
Our study significantly advances our comprehension of human brain evolution by employing a unique approach to dissect changes in brain size throughout the complete fossil record of hominins. By disentangling the dynamics of brain size change that occur within species from those occurring across species, we unveil that increases in brain size primarily occurred within the lineages comprising a single species. Such a pattern gives rise to the overall brain expansion that scientists herald as a trademark of modern humanity. Furthermore, we reveal a trend of accelerating brain size growth in more recent lineages. This nuanced understanding deepens our insight into the evolutionary trajectory of human cognition and behavior, crucial for unraveling the complexities of our species’ unique traits.
Abstract
The fact that rapid brain size increase was clearly a key aspect of human evolution has prompted many studies focusing on this phenomenon, and many suggestions as to the underlying evolutionary patterns and processes. No study to date has however separated out the contributions of change through time within vs. between hominin species while simultaneously incorporating effects of body size. Using a phylogenetic approach never applied before to paleoanthropological data, we show that relative brain size increase across ~7 My of hominin evolution arose from increases within individual species which account for an observed overall increase in relative brain size. Variation among species in brain size after accounting for this effect is associated with body mass differences but not time. In addition, our analysis also reveals that the within-species trend escalated in more recent lineages, implying an overall pattern of accelerating relative brain size increase through time.
Sign up for PNAS alerts.
Get alerts for new articles, or get an alert when an article is cited.
One of the most evident evolutionary changes during human evolution and one intimately associated with our unique cognitive and behavioral traits has been an increase in brain size (1–5). Encephalization (i.e., relative brain size increase) during human evolution has long been debated, and several studies have compared hominin cranial capacities across species to propose possible adaptive mechanisms acting upon brain size variation among hominins (5–10). Some have argued for gradual growth over time (6, 11, 12), while others propose punctuated equilibrium with rapid increases followed by stasis (13–16). Other studies support a combination of both models (7, 17, 18), while others claim they cannot be distinguished (19). These contradictory views arise in part from conflating distinct phenomena (8, 20, 21); namely, the role of speciation events on trait diversification (anagenesis vs. cladogenesis), and the relative importance of gradual vs. pulsed evolution (i.e., different aspects of punctuated equilibrium) (20). One previous study also emphasized a need to partition hominin brain size evolution in order to assess change within lineages (i.e., phyletic change) and between lineages (i.e., cladogenesis or lineage extinction), as such an approach could provide a more detailed evaluation for the evolution of this trait (8).
Variation within-species is also an important consideration in understanding macroevolutionary patterns in encephalization, as well as in absolute brain size, and as such, other researchers have focused on both relative and absolute brain size change at the intraspecific level. These studies are mostly limited to those species in which their hypodigm is large enough to enable meaningful analyses (e.g., Homo sapiens, Homo neanderthalensis, Homo heidelbergensis, Homo erectus, Homo habilis, Paranthropus boisei, Australopithecus afarensis) (17, 22–29). Inconsistent results have even been found for several species, with some reporting that there is no evident trend through time (10, 24, 29), while others seem to point toward an increase (17, 26–28), and yet others report apparent reductions (30). Inconsistent results have even been shown for the same species (24, 28). These conflicting results hint at complex patterns not fully captured by approaches used previously. Simultaneously analyzing within- and between-species variation in brain size while accounting for body size has the potential to provide a more comprehensive and nuanced understanding of hominin brain evolution. Here, we present a comprehensive set of analyses that allow us to study relative cranial capacity evolution through time, by explicitly considering a) body mass, b) within- and between-species variability, and c) phylogenetic relatedness and uncertainty.
Fig. 1 illustrates the different evolutionary scenarios that our analyses enable us to distinguish between. First, a between-species brain size increase correlated with time but not within-species (Fig. 1A). Second, a within-species brain increment associated with time but no evident trend between species (Fig. 1B). Third, a combined between- and within-species trend toward increasing brain size (Fig. 1C). Fourth, a scenario where later species exhibited larger cranial capacities, and where each lineage could show its own within-species trend that could be horizontal, positive, or negative (Fig. 1D). Fifth, variable within-species encephalization patterns with no between-species trend (Fig. 1E). Any of these brain size increase scenarios can occur together with different within- and between-species body mass relationships (Fig. 1 F–J) and our analyses can reveal which of these alternative scenarios is most consistent with the data.
Fig. 1.
We first undertook a “combined-evidence” Bayesian phylogenetic reconstruction of hominin species (31, 32) using stratigraphic, molecular, and morphological data to account for shared ancestry and its associated uncertainty in our comparative analyses (Fig. 2A, Materials and Methods, and SI Appendix, Fig. S1 and Table S1). We obtained a posterior sample of phylogenetic trees from which we randomly sampled 1,000. We then removed the gorilla, chimpanzee, and Denisovan prior to our subsequent analyses (i.e., restricting analyses to hominins for which cranial capacity data are available). Such an approach has the advantage of incorporating uncertainty about hominin relationships: Rather than assuming a single supposedly correct hominin phylogeny, we were able to test our models using a sample of phylogenetic trees showing different topologies. We compiled the largest extinct hominin dataset to date comprising cranial capacity (n = 285), body mass (n = 431), and chronometric age data for multiple specimens representing the species in our phylogenies (Fig. 2B) (Materials and Methods). We associated every specimen with cranial capacity with a species category, body mass value, and chronometric age using well-defined criteria (Materials and Methods) which allowed us to consider uncertainties related to taxonomic assignment, body mass estimates, and temporal range (an alternative phylogenetic imputation procedure was also tested but yielded qualitatively identical results; Materials and Methods). This process was repeated 1,000 times resulting in 1,000 unique datasets, each one of them comprising the same 285 individuals with cranial capacities but now with associated body mass and chronometric age values, as well as a taxonomic label. This means that our 1,000 datasets contain samples of both phylogenetic trees and hominin data, and as such incorporate the uncertainty in both. One of the main advantages of such an approach is that it enables testing different modeling scenarios more easily, thus facilitating effective assessment of diverse modeling decisions, including alternative taxonomic classifications. Therefore, we decided to robustly assess alternative modeling assumptions by repeating the above procedures with additional modeling sets (See SI Appendix, Table S2 for a complete list of the 24 tested models, as well as associated numerical results; please note that each of the models was run 1,000 times using our 1,000 datasets and 1,000 phylogenies). This modeling approach represents, to our knowledge, the most comprehensive attempt to incorporate uncertainty and assess modeling decisions ever undertaken in the study of encephalization. Furthermore, as our data are available here, other alternative scenarios can naturally be envisioned and easily tested by informed readers using our proposed approach. We acknowledge that additional uncertainty sources exist, but we have tried to incorporate as many as possible, as well as being explicit about them and their associated assumptions.
Fig. 2.
We used Bayesian phylogenetic generalized linear mixed models (PGLMM) (33, 34) to test the relationship between cranial capacity relative to body mass and time at both the intra- and interspecific levels. We take advantage of a “within-group centering” (35) approach (Materials and Methods) to study the relative effects of within-species and between-species variation. This procedure resulted in four different variables: the between-species body mass and time effects and the within-species body mass and time cofactors. We allowed slopes and intercepts to vary within-species for time (Model 1), as well as for time and body mass (Model 2) to account for potential species-specific differences. To distinguish between an anagenetic vs. a cladogenetic scenario of brain size evolution, we repeated Model 1 but also including an additional covariate (i.e., log10 node count) that can be regarded as a speciation rate metric obtained by counting the number of nodes between the root and each tip present in the phylogeny (Model 3). We also ran an additional set of models including an interaction term between the between- and within-time variables to assess whether brain size evolution accelerated through time (Model 4). Table 1 provides the definition of all our main models. We repeated every one of these modeling procedures 1,000 times using our 1,000 datasets and 1,000 phylogenies to ensure our results are robust to the various sources of uncertainty. We considered an effect significant if the obtained p Markov-chain Monte Carlo (pMCMC) values (36) were less than or equal to 0.05 in 95% of the 1,000 analyses carried out for each one of our modeling scenarios (Models 1 to 4). Rather than choosing one single model, these four alternatives are reported as all of them provide complementary aspects about relative brain size increase in hominin evolution such as the potential role of cladogenesis or whether there is an accelerating trend in relative brain size increase.
Table 1.
Model | Description | Definition | DIC | h2 | Marginal R2 | Conditional R2 |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
M1 | Model with varying slopes and intercepts for within-time effects | CC ∼ bmbetween + bmwithin + timebetween + timewithin+ (phylogeny) + (species:timewithin) | −938.718 | 0.952 | 0.612 | 0.935 |
M2 | Model with varying slopes and intercepts for within-body mass and within-time effects | CC ∼ bmbetween + bmwithin + timebetween + timewithin+ (phylogeny) + (species:bmwithin) + (species:timewithin) | −953.615 | 0.888 | 0.61 | 0.947 |
M3 | Model used to assess cladogenesis vs. anagenesis | CC ∼ bmbetween + bmwithin + timebetween + timewithin+ NC + (phylogeny) + (species:timewithin) | −938.943 | 0.951 | 0.641 | 0.939 |
M4 | Model used to test for accelerating evolution | CC ∼ bmbetween + bmwithin + timebetween + timewithin + timebetween:timewithin + (phylogeny) | −888.397 | 0.92 | 0.615 | 0.882 |
Results
Our results (Table 1 and SI Appendix, Tables S2–S5) show a strong and significant association between cranial capacity and between-species body mass. However, no association with body mass was found within species. We found no association between cranial capacity and between-species time differences, but there was a significant relationship at the intraspecific level for time (Fig. 3). Furthermore, even if there was a significant between-species time effect, its slope would be shallow as observed in all our models (SI Appendix, Table S4). As we found that there was no significant within-species body mass effect in our models, we report the results from Model 1 below (i.e., our model with a species-specific random effect for the within-species time variable), but all our models show qualitatively identical results (Table 1). Phylogenetic signal as measured by Lynch’s heritability (37) was close to one (h2 grand mean: 0.95) (Fig. 3D), highlighting the importance of considering shared ancestry when studying hominin brain data. R2 values [both marginal and conditional (38)] were consistently high (marginal R2 grand mean: 0.61; conditional R2 grand mean: 0.94), thus indicating the overall good fit of our models (SI Appendix, Table S3). Our results correspond to the expectations shown in Fig. 1 E and J in which there a is significant within-species slope variation for cranial capacity, with no significant effect at the between-species level with respect to time—along with no within-species slope variation and a significant between-species effect of body size. Later specimens exhibit larger cranial capacities as compared to earlier ones, but there is no evident trend at the interspecific level, together with a body mass between-species effect and no evident body mass–related trend within-species (Fig. 3 A and B and SI Appendix, Tables S4 and S5). It is important to keep in mind that our models are multiple regressions comprising both time and body mass between- and within-species effects, and as such the results need to be considered in tandem and not isolation. This means that cranial capacity increases within species and these increases persevere through time as there is a between-species body mass effect that allows cranial capacity to pick up where it left without a “reset” in terms of relative brain size. In other words, relative brain size increases along the branches of the phylogeny, and then the achieved relative brain size increments in every lineage persists through time owing to body mass increases at speciation points. It is important to bear in mind when interpreting our results that we are estimating the association between variables along the branches of a phylogenetic tree (i.e., evolutionary regression coefficients) (39). These coefficients indicate in our case how cranial capacity evolves along the branches of a phylogenetic tree as function of changes in other traits (i.e., within and between-species body mass and time), thus allowing us to recover the historical pattern of evolutionary change in cranial capacity.
Fig. 3.
Our findings illustrate the multilevel nature of hominin brain size evolution. Part of the contradictory observations made by previous studies are the result of not accounting for intra- vs. interspecies differences, as well as ignoring allometric and phylogenetic effects, as shown by the significant between-species body mass effect (Fig. 3B) and high phylogenetic signal (h2) values (Fig. 3D). Our modeling approach highlights the fundamental importance of disentangling intra- and interspecific levels of variation when analyzing brain size evolution, as not accounting for these differences results in observing an opposite encephalization pattern (SI Appendix, Table S4). A misleading result would also be obtained if a standard phylogenetic generalized least squares regression is applied, as such an approach would intrinsically conflate within- and between-species effects (SI Appendix, Table S4).
Discussion
Our models show a significant within-species time effect, where slopes differ at the intraspecific level. Hence, hominin encephalization cannot simply be characterized by a common and shared temporal trend across all hominin species, challenging the notion that brain evolution has been consistently driven by a single long-term, global, and constant low-level directional selective pressure (6). Instead, we observe a positive association between relative cranial capacity and time within species, indicating temporal trends within each one of the analyzed species (Fig. 3A). This suggests different intensities of selection toward larger brain sizes within each hominin species during different time periods (Fig. 3A). For example, our analyses (Model 4) reveal that the within-species trend escalated in more recent lineages, implying an overall pattern of gradual but accelerating relative brain size increase through time (Fig. 3C and SI Appendix, Table S4). Early hominins such as Au. africanus have a shallow slope between cranial capacity and time (Fig. 3C), and our models suggest that other early species like Au. afarensis, while too small to visualize independently, are likely to have shown similar patterns. On the other hand, later species (e.g., H. heidelbergensis or H. sapiens) increased their brain sizes at a faster pace. The correlation between this within-species temporal trend (i.e., slopes of the within-species time effect) and time is strong (Pearson’s r: 0.74; Pearson’s r without H. neanderthalensis: 0.9). H. neanderthalensis show the fastest increase in brain size through time. This may be owing to the greater degree of encephalization reported in late Neanderthals as compared to earlier members of this species. This result challenges the antiquated notion of Neanderthals as a uniform species unable to respond quickly to their changing environments (40).
Our results also show that there is a strong association between brain size increase and body mass (Fig. 3B), which is consistent with evidence that suggests brain size and body size have not independently evolved over evolutionary time (41). In multiple primate species (e.g., macaques, baboons, apes), as well as H. sapiens, there is a low correlation between brain size and body mass at the intraspecific level even in very large samples (41–45). Our results, showing no significant relationship between brain size and body mass within species, generalize these assessments to all hominin species. This finding is consistent with the seminal study by ref. 46 that found low intraspecific and high interspecific phenotypic correlations when studying brain size:body size relationships. In addition, the results from Model 3, which assessed the role of anagenesis vs. cladogenesis on encephalization, show that while speciation events are not significant predictors of relative cranial capacity, there is still a within-species age effect, which is consistent with an anagenetic pattern of brain size evolution. This is further confirmed by the results of an additional model excluding the within- and between-time effects in which node count was still nonsignificant (SI Appendix, Tables S2–S5).
Taken together, our results show that there was a within-species increase in brain size during human evolution and that this pattern explains the overall increase in relative brain size across human evolution. This means that relative brain size macroevolution in hominins seems to be entirely explained by microevolutionary, population-level processes. This process is consistent with an anagenetic pattern as further shown by our results considering speciation events (Model 3). Traditionally, a gradual trend has been understood as the consequence of consistent directional selection at the within-species (i.e., population) level for larger brains represented by a single common slope (Fig. 1B). However, here we show that a similar pattern can also arise when different species display their own intraspecific increasing trends (Figs. 1E and 3B). This contradicts conventional punctuated equilibrium views that regard encephalization as the result of brief episodes of rapid increase separated by extended periods of stasis (13–15). The absence of a between-species time effect does not mean that there are no differences between species, but rather that there is a positive association between relative cranial capacity and time within-species (Fig. 3A), in which brain size increases within each lineage through time. This means that intraspecific increments in cranial capacity persist over time as result of brain size tracking interspecific changes in body mass. Our results are also consistent with an accelerating within-species increase through time (Model 4), in line with hypotheses that evoke a coevolutionary positive feedback process such as between brain size and sociality, culture, technology, or language (47–51).
While some previous studies have reported an accelerating pattern in the rates of evolution of brain size across hominins at the species level (12, 16, 52), our study is the first to have identified an accelerated increase in the allometric relationship between brain and body size within species through time, providing an intraspecific mechanistic explanation for previously reported results across species. Overall, our results show the multilevel aspects of human brain expansion, as well as the need for future studies to incorporate this hierarchical complexity. Our methods offer an effective quantitative framework to study within- and between-species trait evolution, and as such open an avenue of research testing explicit hypotheses about which factors underly intra- and interspecific brain expansion during hominin evolution. A logical extension of the applied approach is to incorporate additional predictor variables, such as environmental and/or climatic factors, which could allow researchers to test longstanding hypotheses about the potential effect of climate on encephalization.
Materials and Methods
Phylogenetic Analyses.
To infer hominin phylogenetic relationships, we carried out a combined-evidence Bayesian phylogenetic analysis of extant and fossil hominin species, combining morphological and molecular data as well as stratigraphic range data (first and last occurrences from the fossil record e.g., refs. 53–55, Dataset S1), a “Fossilized Birth Death Range Process” (56) implemented within RevBayes v.1.1.0 (57). The morphological data came from ref. 58 and comprised a supermatrix of 391 craniodental characters. We removed H. antecessor from this matrix as it corresponds to a single juvenile individual with mostly missing data in the original dataset (58). We also added additional morphological characters that were originally coded as missing in two species (i.e., Au. anamensis and H. floresiensis) using information from refs. 59 and 60 (Dataset S2). The Mkv+Γ model (61) was used for the morphological data, partitioned by number of states and ordered or unordered characters. Possible ascertainment bias in the morphological matrix was considered by using RevBayes’ dynamic likelihood approach (62). The molecular data were complete mitogenomes without the D-loop region obtained from ref. 31. We used the GTR+Γ+I model of nucleotide sequence evolution to model each molecular partition, which accounted for rate variation among sites, as well as for invariable loci.
We ran the phylogenetic inference analysis for eight million Markov chain Monte Carlo (MCMC) generations. We visually inspected that the run achieved convergence and good mixing using trace plots and that all parameters had an effective sample size >1,000 using the package “coda” v.0.19-4 (63) in R v.4.0.2 (64). After discarding 25% of the chain as burn-in, we randomly sampled 1,000 phylogenies (Dataset S3) from the posterior sample that were used in the subsequent analyses after removing the outgroup taxa (Gorilla gorilla and Pan troglodytes) and Denisovan (which has no cranial capacity estimate available). For all prior specifications and more detailed implementation, see SI Appendix.
Cranial Capacity and Body Mass Estimates.
We assembled the largest collection, to our knowledge, of brain and body size estimates ever compiled for fossil hominins—ranging from ~7 Ma to end of the Pleistocene (Dataset S4). We collected specimen-level cranial capacity or endocranial volume (cm3) and body mass (kg) from a combination of recent compilations and meta-analyses (e.g., refs. 65 and 66 and primary sources. We preferred cranial estimates obtained directly from endocasts (either virtual or physical) over other methods (SI Appendix). For a few individuals, we used adult-projected values as available in the literature (n = 9) (SI Appendix).
All body mass estimates were obtained from the literature, e.g., refs. 65, 67, and 68 plus one additional estimate computed by us (K. platyops, KNM-WT 40000, Dataset S4). It is relevant to bear in mind that these body mass estimates are subject to error, as they are not directly measured values but mostly derived from regression models and as such subject to future change and revision. When available, body mass estimates calculated from lower limb anatomical elements (e.g., femur, tibia, etc.) or pelvic remains were generally preferred over those computed using upper limb, axial, and/or cranial remains), as it is largely agreed that weight-bearing skeletal elements correlate better with an individual’s body mass (67). Only body mass data from adult individuals were collected. Given the above, we then generated 1,000 datasets (Dataset S5) to account for different uncertainty sources in the following manner.
Age: Each specimen was assigned a randomly sampled age obtained from their specific temporal range based on the most updated dating information (SI Appendix) using a uniform distribution. Each specimen was assigned to a species based on consensus information (69), as well as on the hypodigm used by ref. 58 and in our phylogenetic inference analysis.
Taxonomy: For the specimens in which the taxonomic assignments were more controversial or unclear, we allowed them to be randomly classified as one of the species proposed for them in each of the 1,000 datasets. To give an example, if an individual has been classified as either H. neanderthalensis or H. heidelbergensis, we randomly allowed this specimen to be classified as H. neanderthalensis or H. heidelbergensis in different datasets.
The data compilation procedure described thus far (i.e., the collection of cranial capacity, body size, age, and taxonomy) resulted in a dataset comprising a total of 285 individuals. However, 184 specimens with cranial capacity did not have associated body mass estimates. For species where n < 20, we assigned each specimen a body size randomly sampled from within the observed range of estimates for that species. For species with larger sample sizes, we similarly performed random sampling, but explicitly considered geological age and corresponding rock unit (e.g., Chibanian, Calabrian, etc.) according to their sampled date, as well as the specific biogeographical realms (e.g., Afrotropical, Paleartic, Indomalayan, etc.) (70) based on their geographical location (Dataset S4). All these procedures were repeated a thousand times which resulted in a thousand hominin datasets (Dataset S5). Both body mass and cranial capacities were log10-transformed prior to the modeling step.
Within- vs. between-Species Effects Using Bayesian PGLMM.
We applied Bayesian PGLMM (33, 71) to assess the relationship between cranial capacity, body mass, and time while also considering phylogenetic relatedness (SI Appendix). In PGLMM, the phylogenetic information is incorporated by adding a random effect that assumes that phylogenetic effects are correlated according to a phylogenetic variance–covariance (or correlation) matrix. In our case, we computed 1,000 matrices using our sample of phylogenies. To assess the relationships between cranial capacity, body mass, and time at both intra- and interspecific levels, we used a technique known as within-group centering (35) which separates each predictor variable into two components: one containing the group-level mean of each predictor (i.e., the species-level means of body mass and time) and a second one containing the within-group variability, (the difference between each specimen and the specific mean).
We accounted for possible slope differences per species by including species-specific random effects for the within-group variability of time (Model 1). We then repeated the modeling procedure incorporating species-specific random effects for the within-group variability of both time and body mass (Model 2). To assess the role of cladogenesis vs. anagenesis on encephalization, we carried out an additional modeling scenario (Model 3) by including the number of nodes between the root and each tip present in the phylogeny (log10 node count) into Model 1. Finally, to test whether there was an accelerating relative brain size increase through time we ran an additional set of models (Model 4) including an interaction term between- and within-species time as covariates. Values reported in the main text correspond to results obtained from Model 1 as no significant within-body mass effect was found in our models, while Table 1 shows the results obtained for Models 1 to 4.
Phylogenetic signal was measured using a modified version of Lynch’s heritability h2 that remains valid for variance–covariance matrices computed from nonultrametric trees (37) (SI Appendix). As suggested by ref. 38, we computed both marginal (variance explained by fixed effects) and conditional (variance explained by both fixed and random effects) R2 values as measures of goodness-of-fit, as these two measures are especially designed to deal with the most common problems faced when generalizing R2 for mixed-effects models. All the above-mentioned steps were implemented using the “MCMCglmm” v.2.33 (72) R package. We used a diffuse normal distribution centered around zero (μ = 0) with very large variance (σ2 = 108) as prior for the fixed effects, while for the variances of the random effects, inverse-Gamma distributions with shape (α) and scale (β) parameters equal to 0.01 were applied. Burn-in time was 10,000 runs, and the total number of iterations was 1,000,000, with a thinning interval of 500.
Convergence and mixing were visually assessed by looking at the trace plots of each one of the fixed and random effects. All chains were run multiple times to ensure convergence, and we checked that effective sample sizes were >1,000. Every model tested was repeated 1,000 times using the previously mentioned datasets and phylogenies. By repeating our analyses in this way, using different phylogenies and datasets, we accounted for phylogenetic uncertainty as well as the uncertainty associated with chronometric ages and body mass data. We deemed an effect to be statistically significant when the pMCMC values obtained from the 1,000 analyses carried out for each one of our different modeling scenarios (SI Appendix, Table S2) were less than or equal to 0.05 in 95% of the cases. pMCMC is defined as twice the posterior probability that the estimate is either negative or positive, whichever probability is smaller (36). An R script is available to run Model 1 using the provided datasets and phylogenies (SI Appendix).
Assessing Alternative Modeling Scenarios.
Our approach incorporates numerous sources of uncertainty as well as explicitly testing several hypotheses regarding hominin encephalization (Fig. 1). However, there will always be some level of controversy associated with taxonomic affiliation as well as the methods and approaches used to measure values like dates and traits. We therefore assessed several additional modeling scenarios, incorporating alternative data-treatments or species-assignments to determine the impacts of such uncertainty on our analyses.
To do this, we repeated the analyses described above but using several different modeling scenarios, which we will briefly list here. However, all models and their justifications are described in detail in the SI Appendix—and a complete list with associated numerical results can be found in SI Appendix, Tables S2–S5. Briefly, our additional analyses include models: incorporating alternative species classifications accounting for taxonomic uncertainty, removing all species with only a single cranial capacity estimate, combining H. ergaster, Georgian H. erectus, and H. erectus into a single species, combining H. habilis and H. rudolfensis into a single species, accounting for different methodological approaches to estimating cranial capacity, body size, and dating, assessing an alternative approach to filling in missing body sizes using phylogenetic imputation rather than random sampling, removing or replacing all cranial-estimated body sizes, excluding all young specimens for which adult-projected estimates were used, testing for alternative cranial capacity estimates for the P. boisei specimen KNM-WT 17400 (24), assessing whether the small-brained H. naledi and H. floresiensis behave differently from the rest of the sample, and finally, excluding Sahelanthropus owing to ongoing controversy regarding its hominin status (72, 73).
None of the additional modeling scenarios result in any substantial change to the results presented in the main text of this work. This not only shows the robustness of our results to numerous sources of uncertainty but also the flexibility of our approach to incorporate different hypothetical scenarios.
Data, Materials, and Software Availability
All study data are included in the article and/or supporting information.
Acknowledgments
We are grateful to Carolynne Roberts, Jorge Avaria-Llautureo, Olivia Price, and Suzy White for helpful discussion. This work was funded by a Leverhulme Trust Research Leadership Award to CV (RL-2019-012).
Author contributions
T.A.P. and C.V. designed research; T.A.P., S.L.N., J.B., R.A.B., and C.V. performed research; T.A.P. and C.V. analyzed data; and T.A.P., S.L.N., J.B., R.A.B., and C.V. wrote the paper.
Competing interests
The authors declare no competing interest.
Supporting Information
Appendix 01 (PDF)
- Download
- 661.62 KB
Dataset S01 (XLSX)
- Download
- 23.11 KB
Dataset S02 (TXT)
- Download
- 11.41 KB
Dataset S03 (TXT)
- Download
- 643.10 KB
Dataset S04 (XLSX)
- Download
- 128.15 KB
Dataset S05 (CSV)
- Download
- 15.37 MB
References
1
M. González-Forero, A. Gardner, Inference of ecological and social drivers of human brain-size evolution. Nature 557, 554–557 (2018).
2
L. C. Aiello, P. Wheeler, The expensive-tissue hypothesis: The brain and the digestive system in human and primate evolution. Curr. Anthropol. 36, 199–221 (1995).
3
K. Isler, C. P. van Schaik, The Expensive Brain: A framework for explaining evolutionary changes in brain size. J. Hum. Evol. 57, 392–400 (2009).
4
A. Navarrete, C. P. van Schaik, K. Isler, Energetics and the evolution of human brain size. Nature 480, 91–93 (2011).
5
R. L. Holloway, “Toward a synthetic theory of human brain evolution” in Origins of the Human Brain, J.-P. Changeux, J. Chavaillon, Eds. (Clarendon Press Oxford, 1995), pp. 42–54.
6
S.-H. Lee, M. H. Wolpoff, The pattern of evolution in Pleistocene human brain size. Paleobiology 29, 186–196 (2003).
7
S. Shultz, E. Nelson, R. I. M. Dunbar, Hominin cognitive evolution: Identifying patterns and processes in the fossil and archaeological record. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B Biol. Sci. 367, 2130–2140 (2012).
8
A. Du et al., Pattern and process in hominin brain size evolution are scale-dependent. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B Biol. Sci. 285, 20172738 (2018).
9
C. De Miguel, M. Henneberg, Variation in hominid brain size: How much is due to method? Homo 52, 3–58 (2001).
10
H. M. McHenry, Tempo and mode in human evolution. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 91, 6780–6786 (1994).
11
J. E. Cronin, N. T. Boaz, C. B. Stringer, Y. Rak, Tempo and mode in hominid evolution. Nature 292, 113–122 (1981).
12
I. F. Miller, R. A. Barton, C. L. Nunn, Quantitative uniqueness of human brain evolution revealed through phylogenetic comparative analysis. Elife 8, e41250 (2019).
13
M. A. Hofman, Encephalization in hominids: Evidence for the model of punctuationalism. Brain Behav. Evol. 22, 102–117 (1983).
14
S. J. Gould, “Chapter 6. Bushes and ladders in human evolution” in Ever Since Darwin. Reflections in Natural History (W. W. Norton & Company, 1977), pp. 56–62.
15
S. J. Gould, N. Eldredge, Punctuated equilibria: The tempo and mode of evolution reconsidered. Paleobiology 3, 115–151 (1977).
16
J. A. F. Diniz-Filho, L. Jardim, A. Mondanaro, P. Raia, Multiple components of phylogenetic non-stationarity in the evolution of brain size in fossil hominins. Evol. Biol. 46, 47–59 (2019).
17
G. P. Rightmire, Brain size and encephalization in early to Mid-Pleistocene Homo. Am. J. Phys. Anthropol. 124, 109–123 (2004).
18
R. A. Foley, Mosaic evolution and the pattern of transitions in the hominin lineage. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B Biol. Sci. 371, 20150244 (2016).
19
L. Godfrey, K. H. Jacobs, Gradual, autocatalytic and punctuational models of hominid brain evolution: A cautionary tale. J. Hum. Evol. 10, 255–272 (1981).
20
M. W. Pennell, L. J. Harmon, J. C. Uyeda, Is there room for punctuated equilibrium in macroevolution? Trends Ecol. Evol. 29, 23–32 (2014).
21
C. Venditti, M. Pagel, Plenty of room for punctuational change. Trends Ecol. Evol. 29, 71–72 (2014).
22
J. A. Miller, Does brain size variability provide evidence of multiple species in Homo habilis? Am. J. Phys. Anthropol. 84, 385–398 (1991).
23
S.-H. Lee, Brief communication: Is variation in the cranial capacity of the Dmanisi sample too high to be from a single species? Am. J. Phys. Anthropol. 127, 263–266 (2005).
24
J. Hawks, No brain expansion in Australopithecus boisei. Am. J. Phys. Anthropol. 146, 155–160 (2011).
25
X.-J. Wu et al., Evolution of cranial capacity revisited: A view from the late Middle Pleistocene cranium from Xujiayao, China. J. Hum. Evol. 163, 103119 (2022).
26
S. R. Leigh, Cranial capacity evolution in Homo erectus and early Homo sapiens. Am. J. Phys. Anthropol. 87, 1–13 (1992).
27
E. M. Poza-Rey, A. Gómez-Robles, J. L. Arsuaga, Brain size and organization in the Middle Pleistocene hominins from Sima de los Huesos. Inferences from endocranial variation. J. Hum. Evol. 129, 67–90 (2019).
28
S. Elton, L. C. Bishop, B. Wood, Comparative context of Plio-Pleistocene hominin brain evolution. J. Hum. Evol. 41, 1–27 (2001).
29
W. H. Kimbel, B. Villmoare, From Australopithecus to Homo: The transition that wasn’t†. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B Biol. Sci. 371, 20150248 (2016).
30
J. M. DeSilva, J. F. A. Traniello, A. G. Claxton, L. D. Fannin, When and why did human brains decrease in size? A new change-point analysis and insights from brain evolution in ants. Front. Ecol. Evol. 9, 742639 (2021).
31
H. P. Püschel, O. C. Bertrand, J. E. O’Reilly, R. Bobe, T. A. Püschel, Divergence-time estimates for hominins provide insight into encephalization and body mass trends in human evolution. Nat. Ecol. Evol. 5, 808–819 (2021), https://doi.org/10.1038/s41559-021-01431-1.
32
H. P. Püschel, O. C. Bertrand, J. E. Reilly, R. Bobe, T. A. Püschel, Reply to: Modelling hominin evolution requires accurate hominin data. Nat. Ecol. Evol. 6, 1092–1094 (2022).
33
J. D. Hadfield, S. Nakagawa, General quantitative genetic methods for comparative biology: Phylogenies, taxonomies and multi-trait models for continuous and categorical characters. J. Evol. Biol. 23, 494–508 (2010).
34
P. de Villemereuil, S. Nakagawa, “General quantitative genetic methods for comparative biology” in Modern Phylogenetic Comparative Methods and Their Application in Evolutionary Biology: Concepts and Practice, L. Z. Garamszegi, Ed. (Springer, 2014), pp. 287–303.
35
M. van de Pol, J. Wright, A simple method for distinguishing within- versus between-subject effects using mixed models. Anim. Behav. 77, 753–758 (2009).
36
J. D. Hadfield, E. A. Heap, F. Bayer, E. A. Mittell, N. M. A. Crouch, Intraclutch differences in egg characteristics mitigate the consequences of age-related hierarchies in a wild passerine. Evolution 67, 2688–2700 (2013).
37
G. E. Leventhal, S. Bonhoeffer, Potential pitfalls in estimating viral load heritability. Trends Microbiol. 24, 687–698 (2016).
38
S. Nakagawa, H. Schielzeth, A general and simple method for obtaining R2 from generalized linear mixed-effects models. Methods Ecol. Evol. 4, 133–142 (2013).
39
M. Pagel, Seeking the evolutionary regression coefficient: An analysis of what comparative methods measure. J. Theor. Biol. 164, 191–205 (1993).
40
S. Peeters, H. Zwart, Neanderthals as familiar strangers and the human spark: How the “golden years” of Neanderthal research reopen the question of human uniqueness. Hist. Philos. Life Sci. 42, 33 (2020).
41
H. J. Jerison, Evolution of the Brain and Intelligence (Academic Press, 1973).
42
H. Pakkenberg, J. Voigt, Brain weight of the Danes. Cells Tissues Organs 56, 297–307 (1964).
43
R. L. Holloway, Within-species brain-body weight variability: A reexamination of the Danish data and other primate species. Am. J. Phys. Anthropol. 53, 109–121 (1980).
44
D. Sholl, The quantitative investigation of the vertebrate brain and the applicability of allometric formulae to its study. Proc. R. Soc. Lond. Ser. B Biol. Sci. 135, 243–258 (1948).
45
M. Gjukić, Ein Beitrag zum Problem der Korrelation zwischen Hirngewicht und Körpergewicht. Z. Morpholog. Anthropolog. 47, 43–57 (1955).
46
R. Lande, Quantitative genetic analysis of multivariate evolution, applied to brain: Body size allometry. Evolution 33, 402–416 (1979).
47
K. N. Laland, Darwin’s Unfinished Symphony: How Culture Made the Human Mind (Princeton University Press, ed. Illustrated, 2017).
48
C. Wills, The Runaway Brain: The Evolution of Human Uniqueness (Basic Books, 1993).
49
P. V. Tobias, B. Campbell, J. Z. Young, E. M. Jope, K. P. Oakley, The emergence of man in Africa and beyond. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B Biol. Sci. 292, 43–56 (1981).
50
A. V. Markov, M. A. Markov, Runaway brain-culture coevolution as a reason for larger brains: Exploring the “cultural drive” hypothesis by computer modeling. Ecol. Evol. 10, 6059–6077 (2020).
51
T. W. Deacon, The Symbolic Species: The Co-evolution of Language and the Brain (W. W. Norton & Company, ed. Illustrated, 1998).
52
M. Pagel, “Modelling the evolution of continuously varying characters on phylogenetic trees: The case of Hominid cranial capacity” in Morphology, Shape and Phylogeny, N. MacLeod, P. L. Forey, Eds. (CRC Press, 2002), pp. 269–286.
53
F. Ronquist et al., A total-evidence approach to dating with fossils, applied to the early radiation of the hymenoptera. Syst. Biol. 61, 973–999 (2012).
54
C. Zhang, T. Stadler, S. Klopfstein, T. A. Heath, F. Ronquist, Total-evidence dating under the fossilized birth-death process. Syst. Biol. 65, 228–249 (2016).
55
A. Gavryushkina et al., Bayesian total-evidence dating reveals the recent crown radiation of penguins. Syst. Biol. 66, 57–73 (2017).
56
T. Stadler, A. Gavryushkina, R. C. M. Warnock, A. J. Drummond, T. A. Heath, The fossilized birth-death model for the analysis of stratigraphic range data under different speciation modes. J. Theor. Biol. 447, 41–55 (2018).
57
S. Höhna et al., RevBayes: Bayesian phylogenetic inference using graphical models and an interactive model-specification language. Syst. Biol. 65, 726–736 (2016).
58
M. Dembo et al., The evolutionary relationships and age of Homo naledi: An assessment using dated Bayesian phylogenetic methods. J. Hum. Evol. 97, 17–26 (2016).
59
Y. Haile-Selassie, S. M. Melillo, A. Vazzana, S. Benazzi, T. M. Ryan, A 3.8-million-year-old hominin cranium from Woranso-Mille, Ethiopia. Nature 573, 214–219 (2019).
60
D. Argue, C. P. Groves, M. S. Y. Lee, W. L. Jungers, The affinities of Homo floresiensis based on phylogenetic analyses of cranial, dental, and postcranial characters. J. Hum. Evol. 107, 107–133 (2017).
61
P. Lewis, A likelihood approach to estimating phylogeny from discrete morphological character data. Soc. Syst. Biol. 50, 913–925 (2001).
62
E. S. Allman, J. A. Rhodes, Identifying evolutionary trees and substitution parameters for the general Markov model with invariable sites. Math. Biosci. 211, 18–33 (2008).
63
M. Plummer, N. Best, K. Cowles, K. Vines, CODA: Convergence diagnosis and output analysis for MCMC. R News 6, 7–11 (2006).
64
R Core Team, R: A language and environment for statistical computing (R Foundation for Statistical Computing, Vienna, Austria, 2022). http://www.R-project.org/. Accessed 24 November 2022.
65
M. Will, M. Krapp, J. T. Stock, A. Manica, Different environmental variables predict body and brain size evolution in Homo. Nat. Commun. 12, 4116 (2021).
66
M. Will, J. T. Stock, Spatial and temporal variation of body size among early Homo. J. Hum. Evol. 82, 15–33 (2015).
67
C. B. Ruff, M. L. Burgess, N. Squyres, J.-A. Junno, E. Trinkaus, Lower limb articular scaling and body mass estimation in Pliocene and Pleistocene hominins. J. Hum. Evol. 115, 85–111 (2018).
68
C. B. Ruff, N. Squyres, J.-A. Junno, Body mass estimation in hominins from humeral articular dimensions. Am. J. Phys. Anthropol. 173, 480–499 (2020).
69
B. Wood, Wiley-Blackwell Encyclopedia of Human Evolution (John Wiley & Sons, 2011).
70
D. M. Olson et al., Terrestrial ecoregions of the World: A new map of life on Earth: A new global map of terrestrial ecoregions provides an innovative tool for conserving biodiversity. BioScience 51, 933–938 (2001).
71
A. R. Ives, M. R. Helmus, Generalized linear mixed models for phylogenetic analyses of community structure. Ecol. Monographs 81, 511–525 (2011).
72
R. Macchiarelli, A. Bergeret-Medina, D. Marchi, B. Wood, Nature and relationships of Sahelanthropus tchadensis. J. Hum. Evol. 149, 102898 (2020).
73
G. Daver et al., Postcranial evidence of late Miocene hominin bipedalism in Chad. Nature 609, 94–100 (2022).
Information & Authors
Information
Published in
Classifications
Copyright
Copyright © 2024 the Author(s). Published by PNAS. This open access article is distributed under Creative Commons Attribution License 4.0 (CC BY).
Data, Materials, and Software Availability
All study data are included in the article and/or supporting information.
Submission history
Received: May 13, 2024
Accepted: October 11, 2024
Published online: November 26, 2024
Published in issue: December 3, 2024
Keywords
Acknowledgments
We are grateful to Carolynne Roberts, Jorge Avaria-Llautureo, Olivia Price, and Suzy White for helpful discussion. This work was funded by a Leverhulme Trust Research Leadership Award to CV (RL-2019-012).
Author contributions
T.A.P. and C.V. designed research; T.A.P., S.L.N., J.B., R.A.B., and C.V. performed research; T.A.P. and C.V. analyzed data; and T.A.P., S.L.N., J.B., R.A.B., and C.V. wrote the paper.
Competing interests
The authors declare no competing interest.
Notes
This article is a PNAS Direct Submission G.R. is a guest editor invited by the Editorial Board.
Authors
Metrics & Citations
Metrics
Citation statements
Altmetrics
Citations
Cite this article
121 (49) e2409542121,
Export the article citation data by selecting a format from the list below and clicking Export.
View Options
View options
PDF format
Download this article as a PDF file
DOWNLOAD PDFLogin options
Check if you have access through your login credentials or your institution to get full access on this article.
Personal login Institutional LoginRecommend to a librarian
Recommend PNAS to a LibrarianPurchase options
Purchase this article to access the full text.